8
Communicating risk information: The influence of graphical display format on quantitative information perception—Accuracy, comprehension and preferences Melanie Price a,b, * , Rachel Cameron b , Phyllis Butow a,b a Medical Psychology Research Unit, University of Sydney, Australia b School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Australia Received 18 December 2006; received in revised form 1 August 2007; accepted 12 August 2007 Abstract Objective: Statistical health risk information has proved notoriously confusing and difficult to understand. While past research indicates that presenting risk information in a frequency format is superior to relative risk and probability formats, the optimal characteristics of frequency formats are still unclear. The aim of this study is to determine the features of 1000 person frequency diagrams (pictographs) which result in the greatest speed and accuracy of graphical perception. Methods: Participants estimated the difference in chance of survival when taking or not taking Drug A, on a pictograph format, varying by mode (one-graph/two-graph), direction (vertical/horizontal), and shading (shaded/unshaded), and their preferences for the different formats. Their understanding of different components of the 1000 person diagram was assessed. Responses were timed and scored for accuracy. Results: Horizontal pictographs were perceived faster and more accurately than vertical formats. Two-graph pictographs were perceived faster than one-graph formats. Shading reduced response time in two-graph formats, but increased response times in one-graph formats. Shaded and one- graph pictographs were preferred. Conclusions: As shading and one-graph formats were preferred, further clarification as to why shading negatively impacts on response times in the one-graph format is warranted. Practice Implications: Horizontal pictographs are optimal. # 2007 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Risk communication; Risk perception; Medical decision-making; Graphical format; Quantitative information 1. Introduction One of the major challenges in promoting informed consent and shared decision-making relates to the effective commu- nication of risk and benefit information to patients about different treatment options [1–5]. Decision aids are increas- ingly being used to assist with this process. Decision aids are designed to facilitate shared decision making, and one aspect of this process is providing quantitative information about the risks and benefits associated with treatment choices [6]. Despite this, there is little empirical evidence about how best to represent quantitative data to improve risk communication [7,8]. While part of the challenge in communicating risk information may be due to the inherent difficulties in understanding probabilistic information [9,10], recent research suggests that it may be the format of the quantitative information which may be a source of confusion [11]. For example, research into cognitive biases generally uses single event or conditional probabilities (e.g. 0.03 or 3%), relative risk (e.g. an increase of 20%), or unrelated base rates (e.g. 9/100 versus 1/11), all of which have been found to result in confusion [12,13]. Of utmost concern are data that suggest that different treatment decisions are made when treatment outcomes are described in terms of absolute risk rather than relative risk [14,15]. Frequency statements (e.g. three women in 100 compared with seven women in the same group of 100), where the same class of observations are referred to, have been found to promote insight and facilitate statistical inferences [13]. www.elsevier.com/locate/pateducou Patient Education and Counseling 69 (2007) 121–128 * Corresponding author at: Medical Psychology Research Unit, School of Psychology, Brennan MacCallum Building (A18), the University of Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia. Tel.: +61 2 9351 3916; fax: +61 2 9036 5292. E-mail address: [email protected] (M. Price). 0738-3991/$ – see front matter # 2007 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.pec.2007.08.006

Communicating risk information: The influence of graphical display format on quantitative information perception—Accuracy, comprehension and preferences

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Communicating risk information: The influence of graphical display format on quantitative information perception—Accuracy, comprehension and preferences

Communicating risk information: The influence of graphical display

format on quantitative information perception—Accuracy,

comprehension and preferences

Melanie Price a,b,*, Rachel Cameron b, Phyllis Butow a,b

a Medical Psychology Research Unit, University of Sydney, Australiab School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Australia

Received 18 December 2006; received in revised form 1 August 2007; accepted 12 August 2007

www.elsevier.com/locate/pateducou

Patient Education and Counseling 69 (2007) 121–128

Abstract

Objective: Statistical health risk information has proved notoriously confusing and difficult to understand. While past research indicates that

presenting risk information in a frequency format is superior to relative risk and probability formats, the optimal characteristics of frequency

formats are still unclear. The aim of this study is to determine the features of 1000 person frequency diagrams (pictographs) which result in the

greatest speed and accuracy of graphical perception.

Methods: Participants estimated the difference in chance of survival when taking or not taking Drug A, on a pictograph format, varying by mode

(one-graph/two-graph), direction (vertical/horizontal), and shading (shaded/unshaded), and their preferences for the different formats. Their

understanding of different components of the 1000 person diagram was assessed. Responses were timed and scored for accuracy.

Results: Horizontal pictographs were perceived faster and more accurately than vertical formats. Two-graph pictographs were perceived faster

than one-graph formats. Shading reduced response time in two-graph formats, but increased response times in one-graph formats. Shaded and one-

graph pictographs were preferred.

Conclusions: As shading and one-graph formats were preferred, further clarification as to why shading negatively impacts on response times in the

one-graph format is warranted.

Practice Implications: Horizontal pictographs are optimal.

# 2007 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Risk communication; Risk perception; Medical decision-making; Graphical format; Quantitative information

1. Introduction

One of the major challenges in promoting informed consent

and shared decision-making relates to the effective commu-

nication of risk and benefit information to patients about

different treatment options [1–5]. Decision aids are increas-

ingly being used to assist with this process. Decision aids are

designed to facilitate shared decision making, and one aspect of

this process is providing quantitative information about the

risks and benefits associated with treatment choices [6]. Despite

this, there is little empirical evidence about how best to

* Corresponding author at: Medical Psychology Research Unit, School of

Psychology, Brennan MacCallum Building (A18), the University of Sydney,

NSW 2006, Australia. Tel.: +61 2 9351 3916; fax: +61 2 9036 5292.

E-mail address: [email protected] (M. Price).

0738-3991/$ – see front matter # 2007 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

doi:10.1016/j.pec.2007.08.006

represent quantitative data to improve risk communication

[7,8]. While part of the challenge in communicating risk

information may be due to the inherent difficulties in

understanding probabilistic information [9,10], recent research

suggests that it may be the format of the quantitative

information which may be a source of confusion [11]. For

example, research into cognitive biases generally uses single

event or conditional probabilities (e.g. 0.03 or 3%), relative risk

(e.g. an increase of 20%), or unrelated base rates (e.g. 9/100

versus 1/11), all of which have been found to result in confusion

[12,13]. Of utmost concern are data that suggest that different

treatment decisions are made when treatment outcomes are

described in terms of absolute risk rather than relative risk

[14,15]. Frequency statements (e.g. three women in 100

compared with seven women in the same group of 100), where

the same class of observations are referred to, have been found

to promote insight and facilitate statistical inferences [13].

Page 2: Communicating risk information: The influence of graphical display format on quantitative information perception—Accuracy, comprehension and preferences

M. Price et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 69 (2007) 121–128122

Graphical formats are increasingly being used to convey risk

information [5,9,16,17] and have been shown to assist in the

perception, understanding and interpretation of quantitative

information over textual or written formats [18,19]. Despite

this, few systematic studies have examined changes in the

perception or interpretation of risk according to variations in

graphical format [20,21].

Feldman-Stewart et al. [22] have reported a series of studies

comparing the accuracy and speed of perception of quantitative

information, in choice and estimate tasks, across different

presentation formats. For tasks involving a simple choice

(bigger versus smaller), systematic ovals (pictographs with

icons systematically highlighted), vertical bars, horizontal bars

and numbers were equally well perceived, while pie charts and

random ovals (pictographs with icons randomly highlighted)

resulted in slower and less accurate performances. For

estimating differences between risk magnitude (a more detailed

task), numbers led to the most accurate estimates, followed by

systematic ovals. The context of the estimate task (i.e. treatment

context or neutral context) did not affect accuracy.

Fagerlin et al. [9] and Burkell [23] have suggested that

pictographs (such as 100 or 1000 person dot/icon diagrams) are

the most advantageous format for presenting risk information,

as they are likely to be perceived and interpreted more

accurately than other visual formats, such as bar and pie graphs.

Pictographs show the reference class to which the person

belongs, thereby removing the main source of statistical

confusion. Visually presenting cure rates using pictographs, in

contrast to verbal presentation of cure rates, has been found to

significantly reduce the impact of anecdotal information on

hypothetical treatment decisions [9]. Risk information pre-

sented as icons have also been reported as more helpful in

decision-making than risk information presented as numbers or

vertical bars [24]. Improved understanding with presentation of

data in pictographs over numbers alone holds in both older and

younger patients [25,26]. Note that a major benefit of the

pictographs is that they do represent frequency (which is more

easily understood than probability).

A recent qualitative study investigating preferences for the

presentation of risk information between different 1000 dot

pictograph formats, reported a preference for dot diagrams

highlighted as separately coloured blocks rather than indivi-

dually highlighted dots [27]. Horizontal presentations of risk

data were preferred over vertical pictograph presentation [27].

To date, there has been no research systematically examining

the effect of varying pictograph format on the accuracy,

comprehension, and format preferences of quantitative risk

information perception.

Given that the visual communication of health risk

information is still in its infancy [19], the most appropriate

place to begin examining the effects of visual formats and

testing their relative impact on performance, is at one of the

most elementary stages of processing, namely perception

[18,28,29]. Cleveland and McGill’s [28] hierarchy of elemen-

tary perceptual tasks provides some guidance as to the expected

features within a graph that influence speed and accuracy of

performance [18]. From the more accurately processed to less

accurately processed, these perceptual features are: position

along a common scale; positions along non-aligned scales;

length; slope; angle; area; volume; and colour hue, saturation

and density [29]. Graphs with features at the more accurately

processed end of the hierarchy will facilitate a better

organisation of quantitative information and increase the

chances of correct perception of relevant patterns [18,28].

Pictographs fall within the first, most accurately processed end

of the hierarchy.

In the health context, most often the goal of risk

communication involves risk magnitude (how large or small)

and relative risk (the difference between two options).

Carswell’s [30] taxonomy of basic graphical tasks, includes

two tasks often required in risk communication, namely ‘local

comparison’ (comparison between two risks) and ‘point

reading’ (precise risk information). Both fall within Cleveland

and McGill’s most accurately processed tasks [30]. However,

greater computational effort may be required even in the low

level, focused task of ‘local comparison’ (making a comparison

between two graphs) compared with ‘point reading’ (reading a

value from a single graph) [31].

Ideally, a graphical display of this information should

include the relevant information in a form that minimises the

computation effort required to understand it [31]. The addition

of a perceptual feature to a pictograph, such as length (or block

shading as preferred by participants in McCaffery et al. [27])

may add extraneous detail and visual complexity to the graph

without adding any extra information [31]. Alternatively, this

may aid perception by highlighting the area of relevance

[18,28]. According to Tufte [32], this would reduce the data-ink

ratio (the ratio of ink devoted to data compared to the total

amount of ink used for all the other features of a graph), and

thereby potentially reduce accuracy and efficiency of percep-

tion [33]. While the reported preference for shading in

pictographs by McCaffery et al. [27] is relevant, they may

not facilitate performance [16].

With respect to the vertical–horizontal differential of

graphical display, perception research also provides some

guidance. Western text is traditionally presented left to right

along horizontal lines and read more quickly than vertically

aligned text [34]. Seo and Lee [35] found that the Korean

language, written vertically, is also read more quickly hori-

zontally than vertically. This appears to be due to larger eye gaze

amplitudes for horizontal reading, the smaller number of rapid

eye movements, and the longer period of time the eye remains

stable and gathers the information when reading horizontal text

[35]. Whether the same pattern of significantly faster perception

of risk information presented horizontally rather than vertically

also occurs within pictographs is worth examining.

It is possible that framing may influence understanding and

perception of risk. Studies of the effect of verbal framing on

interpretation and decision-making have generally focused on

positive (e.g. 75% chance of survival) versus negative framing

(25% chance of dying). While there is evidence to support the

greater effectiveness of positive framing in persuading people

to take risky or more invasive treatment options, some studies

suggest a negative frame may be more effective in promoting

Page 3: Communicating risk information: The influence of graphical display format on quantitative information perception—Accuracy, comprehension and preferences

M. Price et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 69 (2007) 121–128 123

screening uptake [5,14]. The use of neutral framing overcomes

the possibility of framing bias [5].

Drawing on theories of health risk communication and

graphical and text perception, this study was designed to

investigate the impact of systematically varying a number of

features of dot pictographs on perception. There is recent

evidence suggesting that partially shaded figures should be

avoided in frequency pictographs, due to a ‘rounding up’ effect

[23]. Given that frequency risk information is being developed

and used in a wide variety of contexts in addition to treatment

outcomes, such as treatment side effects health screening and

prevention, 1000 person dot diagrams formed the basis of the

experiment. This is particularly relevant in the context of health

screening and prevention, where for example, 15 new breast

cancers may be diagnosed for every 1000 women screened.

Using a 100 person pictograph in this context would require

partial shading.

Three aspects with the potential to affect speed and accuracy

of perception of pictographs were examined:

(a) t

wo simple graphs versus one compound graph: complex

graphs (combining several data points) may be more

efficient and require less visual scanning [35], or may add

processing and computational effort [31], increasing the

likelihood of judgement errors and the time needed to

perform the task [36];

(b) s

hading versus no shading: the addition of length or block

shading may highlight the area of relevance and assist in the

extraction of the quantitative information, or may make it

harder to count the data [31]; and

(c) h

orizontal versus vertical presentation of data: horizontal

data may be perceived more quickly than vertical [35].

With patient preferences increasingly considered in the

healthcare setting, preferences with respect to the different

pictograph formats are also of interest.

Therefore, the aims of the study were to:

(1) s

ystematically evaluate the differential effects of a variety

of pictograph formats displaying hypothetical treatment

Fig. 1. Example of one complex graph, horizontal data display

risks and benefits on the accuracy and speed of perception

of such quantitative information in an estimate task;

(2) a

ssess the level of understanding of the visually presented

risk information; and

(3) e

xamine participant preferences for pictographic formats.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

A convenience sample of 76 first year psychology students

were recruited via an online research sign up program and

participated in the study for course credit. Eligibility criteria

included normal or corrected-to-normal vision, normal colour

vision and fluency in English. Approval was obtained from the

University of Sydney Human Research Ethics Committee.

2.2. Design and procedure

The study was conducted on computer displaying a series of

pictographs representing a hypothetical treatment risk context.

The quantitative information presented was the chance of

survival and the chance of dying, with and without treatment.

Treatment was a hypothetical drug named ‘Drug A.’ Survival

and mortality data only were presented (rather than also listing

side effects) for simplicity and neutrality of framing.

Participants were instructed to work quickly and accurately.

2.2.1. Estimate Task

The experiment was a three way within-subject design

investigating: mode of graph presentation (two simple graphs

versus one compound graph); direction (vertical versus

horizontal); and shading (shaded versus unshaded). Examples

of pictographs are displayed in Figs. 1 and 2.

Twelve trials of each of the eight possible combinations (96

trials) of graphical displays were presented in two blocks. The

predominant outcome presented (survival or death) were

counterbalanced for each graphical combination. The trials

were divided into two blocks of 48 by mode, with Block A

consisting of the 48 one compound graph formats and Block B

, shaded pictograph (predominant outcome death).

Page 4: Communicating risk information: The influence of graphical display format on quantitative information perception—Accuracy, comprehension and preferences

Fig. 2. Example of two simple graphs, vertical data display, unshaded (predominant outcome survival).

M. Price et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 69 (2007) 121–128124

consisting of the 48 two simple graph formats. This was done to

minimise the degree of image change presented from trial to

trial. This enabled the text describing each graph and the legend

related to each graph to be consistently placed for all the one-

graph formats and for all two graph formats. The blocks were

counterbalanced to prevent order effects, with 47.4% of

participants beginning with Block A and 52.6% beginning with

Block B. The order of trials within each block was randomised

for each participant.

2.2.1.1. Stimulus material. The text accompanying Block A

(one compound graph) pictographs read: ‘Of 1000 people in

their 50 s, over 10 years.’ In Block B (two simple graph)

pictographs the text over the first pictograph read ‘Of 1000

people in their 50 s, over 10 years, who DON’T take Drug A’

and over the second graph read’ Of 1000 people in their 50 s,

over 10 years, who DO take Drug A.’ The legend explaining the

meaning of each colour was located on the upper right side of

each card. Yellow represented ‘Chance of survival whether or

not Drug A is taken,’ blue represented ‘Chance of dying

whether or not Drug A is taken,’ and orange (only in the

compound graphs) represented ‘Chance of survival due to Drug

A.’ The question asked in relation to each pictograph was:

‘What is the difference in the chance of survival between those

who don’t take Drug A versus those who do?’ Each block

included an example and three practice trials with feedback.

Response time for each format was calculated on median

response time across the six variations of each pictograph

format. Accuracy was scored as the absolute size of the error in

the difference estimate.

2.2.2. Comprehension Task

Participants were presented with five multiple choice

questions designed to test their understanding of the meaning

of the blue, yellow and orange areas depicted in the pictographs

(see Appendix A). A comprehension score was calculated by

summing the number of correct responses to the multiple

choice questions.

2.2.3. Preferences task

Participants were presented with two of each of the eight

pictograph combinations and were simply asked to rate each

format on a five point Likert scale from 1 ‘do not like at all’ to 5

‘like very much.’

3. Results

The sample comprised 55 women and 21 men, ranging in

age from 18 to 54 years (M = 19.5 years, SD = 4.9 years). Data

were analysed using SPSS 12.0.1 for Windows [SPSS, Inc.].

There were no differences on any of the dependent variables

due to gender or according to the order of completing Blocks A

and B. Thus, all data were collapsed across gender and order of

completion.

3.1. Response time on estimate task

Table 1 summarises the data for response time as a function

of pictograph format. Analysis of variance revealed a

significant difference in response times for direction, with

quicker reaction times for horizontal pictographs than vertical

pictographs (F(1,75) = 32.27, p < 0.001). While there were no

significant differences within mode ( p > 0.05) or shading

( p > 0.05), there was a significant interaction between shading

and mode. In the one-graph condition, shading was associated

with slower response times (mean (standard deviation) shaded

reaction time (RT) = 10.72 (2.63) in seconds versus unshaded

RT = 10.29 (2.54)), whereas in the two-graph condition,

Page 5: Communicating risk information: The influence of graphical display format on quantitative information perception—Accuracy, comprehension and preferences

Table 1

Mean (standard deviation) response time in seconds, accuracy (absolute size of

error) and preferences rating as a function of pictograph mode, direction, and

shading (N = 76)

Format Response timea Accuracyb Preferencesc

Horizontal 10.06 (0.25)* 0.16 (0.02)* 3.08 (0.07)

Vertical 10.83 (0.29)* 0.21 (0.03)* 2.98 (0.07)

Two-graph 10.37 (0.29) 0.18 (0.03) 2.55 (0.06)*

One-graph 10.52 (0.29) 0.19 (0.02) 3.51 (0.07)*

Shaded 10.42 (0.26) 0.18 (0.02) 3.67 (0.08)*

Unshaded 10.46 (0.27) 0.19 (0.02) 2.40 (0.09)*

a Mean (standard deviation) response time in seconds.b Mean (standard deviation) of absolute size of error—lower scores equate to

higher accuracy.c Mean (standard deviation) score between 1 and 5—higher scores equate to

greater preference.* p < 0.05.

Table 2

Number of absolute error score as a function of pictograph mode, direction and

shading

Format Absolute error frequencies

0 1 2 3 4 5+

One-graph 3138 380 87 24 6 6

Two-graph 3169 339 73 30 9 7

Vertical 3097* 405 81 29 12 8

Horizontal 3210* 314 79 25 3 5

Shaded 3151 351 92 23 9 10

Unshaded 3156 368 68 31 6 3

* p < 0.05.

M. Price et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 69 (2007) 121–128 125

shading was associated with faster responses times (shaded

RT = 10.10 (2.50) versus unshaded RT = 10.63 (2.62)),

(F(1,75) = 42.50, p < 0.001).

3.2. Accuracy on estimate task

Over 86% of responses were correct and 99.2% of errors

were between 0 and 3 (see Table 2). Analysis of variance

revealed a significant difference in accuracy for direction, with

greater accuracy for the horizontal formats than vertical formats

(F(1,75) = 5.0, p < 0.05). There were no significant differences

within mode or shading, nor their interactions.

3.3. Accuracy on comprehension questions

The mean comprehension score was 4.1 (SD = 1.0) on a

scale of one to five (range 2–5). However, despite the

Table 3

Mean (standard deviation) response time in seconds and accuracy (absolute size

of error) on estimate task as a function of comprehension task score (N = 76)

Comprehension task score Response timea Accuracyb

Score of 5 10.90 (0.28)a 0.10 (0.03)*

Score of <5 10.11 (0.25) 0.25 (0.03)*

a Mean (standard deviation) response time in seconds.b Mean (standard deviation) of absolute size of error—lower scores equate to

higher accuracy.* p < 0.05.

conceptual content being the same, questions involving two

graphs were significantly less likely to be answered correctly

than questions involving one graph (63.8% correct versus

93.0% respectively, t(75) = 5.4, p < 0.001). There were no

significant differences detected in comprehension within

direction or shading (Table 3).

3.4. Preferences

Shaded pictographs were preferred to unshaded pictographs

(F(1,75) = 144.8, p < 0.001) and one-graph presentations

were preferred to two-graph presentations (F(1,75) = 106.9,

p < 0.001) (see Table 1). There were no differences in

preference ratings for direction (F(1,75) = 1.5, p > 0.05).

There were no significant interactions.

4. Discussion and conclusions

4.1. Discussion

This study was conducted to systematically investigate

the effects of different 1000 person pictograph formats on

the speed and accuracy of perception, participant pre-

ferences and conceptual understanding of treatment risk

scenarios. As predicted, based on previous research findings

of horizontal aligned text being read more quickly than

vertically aligned text [34,35], data presented along the

horizontal axis of the pictograph were perceived faster than

vertical presentation. The current results are the first to

demonstrate that risk information presented in horizontal

format is also perceived more accurately than that presented

in vertical format. This link between accuracy and response

time has been previously reported in the graphical perception

literature, indicating that there was no speed-accuracy trade-

off [18,29].

Some findings were contrary to expectations. Firstly, the

lack of differences in speed and accuracy of risk perception

according to mode (one compound graph versus two simple

graphs) was somewhat surprising. However, both point

reading (compound graph condition) and local comparisons

(two simple graph conditions) are considered to be low level

and focused perceptual tasks [30], and it is possible that any

impact of differences in complexity on response time and

accuracy is minimal. Secondly, also contrary to expectation,

was the finding that overall, the addition of shading had no

impact on the speed and accuracy of risk perception [32].

However, there was a significant interaction between shading

and mode, with shading having a detrimental affect on

response time in the one compound graph condition and a

significantly beneficial impact on response times in the two

simple graph condition. Of interest, there were no differences

in accuracy detected, indicating the trade-off was restricted

to perception time only. This result suggests that an increase

in the data-ink ratio with the addition of shading (an

additional perceptual feature of length), reduces the

computational effort required in the local comparison task

(two simple graphs), while interfering or increasing the effort

Page 6: Communicating risk information: The influence of graphical display format on quantitative information perception—Accuracy, comprehension and preferences

M. Price et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 69 (2007) 121–128126

required in the point comparison task (one compound graph)

[31].

With respect to preferences, shaded graphs were preferred to

unshaded graphs, providing empirical support for the findings

of McCaffery et al. [27]. A reason for this may be that the

shading seems to unify the mass of dots into a coherent whole

and provides a definable boundary to the 1000 person dot

matrix, potentially a more aesthetically pleasing result. One

compound graph formats were preferred to two simple graphs,

most likely due to the relative coherence and simplicity of

presenting data together in one graph. The compound graph

format also increased saliency of the difference in risk, by

highlighting it in a different colour.

In contrast to McCaffery et al. [27], there was no evidence to

suggest a preference for horizontal pictographs over vertical

pictographs. However, McCaffery et al. [27] ascertained these

preferences using a compound graph format only, and it is

possible that the large preferences participants in the current

study reported for compound graphs over simple graphs and

shaded compared to unshaded graphical presentations over-

shadowed the direction of directional preferences.

This study also explored the level of conceptual under-

standing of treatment risk scenarios, as presented by the

diagrams. Overall, the participants’ comprehension of the

information presented was good. The high level of under-

standing is consonant with previous research results, where

frequency formats (of which a pictograph is a visual

analogue) were usually correctly comprehended by between

50% and 80% of participants, compared with 20–40%

understanding single event and conditional probabilities

[11,13]. Comprehension was greater in the one compound

graph formats than the two-simple graph formats. The

compound graph condition allows the risk information to be

presented in a simple, condensed format. The legend of the

compound graph format specifies which section of the

pictograph relates to the ‘chance of survival whether or not

Drug A is taken,’ the ‘chance of dying whether or not Drug A

is taken,’ and most importantly, the ‘chance of survival due to

Drug A,’ the focus of the estimate task. The legend of the two

simple graph formats, due to its layout, designates a ‘chance

of survival’ section and ‘chance of dying’ section. Therefore,

it is understandable that the answers to the questions were not

directly provided in the key (namely ‘What represents the best

description of the blue area of diagram B?’ and ‘What

represents the best description of the yellow area of diagram

A?’), but required computation in the two-graph format, and

therefore would be more difficult.

Some limitations to this study should be noted, namely the

small sample size and student sample, and it would be useful

to replicate the study in a larger clinical sample. However,

Feldman-Stewart et al. [22] found similarities in risk

perception between students and clinical samples. In this

study only 1000 dot pictographs were presented and

therefore it cannot be assumed that the results are

generalisable to results from using 100 dot pictographs.

Further studies are required to compare and contrast the

accuracy and speed of perception of quantitative risk

information between 100 and 1000 dot pictograph formats.

Perhaps more problematic is the skewness of the accuracy

task data. The majority of participants responded to all items

without error. While this may be a feature of the relatively

straight forward perceptual task, repeating the study in a

clinical sample is required, as they are likely to be slower and

less accurate overall [22].

4.2. Conclusions

This study is the first to systematically investigate the effect

of pictograph format on perception and to provide some support

for the application of general perceptual theories to the

perception of pictograph formats. This is clearly demonstrated

in the main effect result for direction, with the horizontal

presentation of risk data being perceived more quickly and

accurately than vertical format. The use of general perception

theories may guide future investigation into the impact of task

complexity in understanding the significant interaction found

between shading and mode (one-graph versus two-graphs) on

perceptual performance. Future research should move towards

assessing the impact of incorporating more complex data, likely

to be used in medical decision-making, such as the inclusion of

side effects data. Additionally, the effects of other pictograph

formats on perception and understanding should be explored in

clinical populations, in tandem with the influence of numeracy

or quantitative ability.

4.3. Practice implications

These results demonstrate differences in the understanding

of risk perception according to the way quantitative informa-

tion is displayed in 1000 person pictographs. These results

provide useful information for those developing health

promotion materials, patient information materials and

decision aids. Taken together, the results of this study indicate

that horizontal pictographs are perceived both more accurately

and efficiently, and should be the format of choice. The one

compound graph format is preferred to the two simple graph

format, is easier to comprehend, and there is no trade-off in

terms of accuracy of perception between the risk data

presented in one-graph compared to two-graph formats. There

is a clear preference for including shading as an additional

feature. While, shading had no detrimental impact on accuracy

of risk perception, it did result in slower response times in the

compound graph formats (but not the shaded two-graph

formats). This suggests that the addition of shading to the one

graph formats has a trade-off in risk perception in terms of

response time, but not accuracy.

Acknowledgements

Melanie Price is supported by the School of Psychology

Postdoctoral Research Fellowship, University of Sydney.

Phyllis Butow is supported by an NHMRC of Australia

Principal Research Fellowship.

Page 7: Communicating risk information: The influence of graphical display format on quantitative information perception—Accuracy, comprehension and preferences

Appendix A. Comprehension task questions

M. Price et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 69 (2007) 121–128 127

References

[1] Ruland CM. Improving patient safety through informatics tools for shared

decision making and risk communication. Int J Med Inform 2004;73:551–

7.

[2] Cantillon P, Irish B, Sales D. Using computers for assessment in medicine.

Br Med J 2004;329:606–9.

[3] O’Connor AM, Legare F, Stacey D. Risk communication in practice: the

contribution of decision aids. Br Med J 2003;327:736–40.

[4] Edwards A. Flexible rather than standardised approaches to communicat-

ing risks in health care. Qual Saf Health Care 2004;13:169–70.

[5] Edwards A, Elwyn G, Covey J, Matthews E, Pill R. Presenting risk

information—a review of the effects of ‘‘framing’’ and other manipula-

tions on patient outcomes. J Health Commun 2001;6:61–82.

[6] O’Connor AM, Rostom A, Fiset V, Tetroe J, Entwistle V, Llewellyn-

Thomas H, Holmes-Rovner M, Barry M, Jones J. Decision aids for

patients facing health treatment or screening decisions: systematic review.

Br Med J 1999;319:731–4.

[7] Edwards A, Thomas R, Williams R, Ellner AL, Brown P, Elwyn G.

Presenting risk information to people with diabetes: evaluating effects and

preferences for different formats by a web-based randomised controlled

trial. Patient Educ Couns 2006;63:336–49.

[8] Waters EA, Weinstein ND, Colditz GA, Emmons K. Formats for improv-

ing risk communication in medical tradeoff decisions. J Health Commun

2006;11:167–82.

[9] Fagerlin A, Wang C, Ubel PA. Reducing the influence of anecdotal

reasoning on people’s health care decisions: is a picture worth a thousand

statistics? Med Decis Making 2005;25:398–405.

Page 8: Communicating risk information: The influence of graphical display format on quantitative information perception—Accuracy, comprehension and preferences

M. Price et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 69 (2007) 121–128128

[10] Fagerlin A, Rovner D, Stableford S, Jentoft C, Wei JT, Holmes-Rovner M.

Patient education materials about the treatment of early-stage prostate

cancer: a critical review. Ann Intern Med 2004;140:721–8.

[11] Gigerenzer G, Edwards A. Simple tools for understanding risks: from

innumeracy to insight. Br Med J 2003;327:741–4.

[12] Yamagishi K. When a 12.86% mortality is more dangerous than 24. 14%:

implications for risk communication. Appl Cognitive Psych 1997;11:495–

506.

[13] Hoffrage U, Lindsey S, Hertwig R, Gigerenzer G. Communicating

statistical information. Science 2000;290:2261–2.

[14] Moxey A, O’Connell D, McGettigan P, Henry D. Describing treatment

effects to patients. J Gen Intern Med 2003;18:948–59.

[15] Wills CE, Holmes-Rovner M. Patient comprehension of information for

shared treatment decision making: state of the art and future directions.

Patient Educ Couns 2003;50:285–90.

[16] Feldman-Stewart D, Brundage MD. Challenges for designing

and implementing decision aids. Patient Educ Couns 2004;54:

265–73.

[17] Epstein RM, Alper BS, Quill TE. Communicating evidence for

participatory decision making. J Amer Med Assoc 2004;291:

2359–66.

[18] Cleveland WS, McGill R. Graphical perception: theory, experimentation

and application to the development of graphical methods. J Am Stat Assoc

1984;79:531–54.

[19] Lipkus IM, Hollands JG. The visual communication of risk. J Natl Cancer

Inst Monogr 1999;25:149–63.

[20] Schapira MM, Nattinger AB, McHorney CA. Frequency or probability? A

qualitative study of risk communication formats used in health care. Med

Decis Making 2001;21:459–67.

[21] Schapira MM, Nattinger AB, McAuliffe TL. The influence of graphic

format on breast cancer risk communication. J Health Commun

2006;11:569–82.

[22] Feldman-Stewart D, Kocovski N, McConnell BA, Brundage MD, Mack-

illop WJ. Perception of quantitative information for treatment decisions.

Med Decis Making 2000;20:228–38.

[23] Burkell J. What are the chances? Evaluating risk and benefit information

in consumer health materials. J Med Libr Assoc 2004;92:200–8.

[24] Timmermans D, Molewijk B, Stiggelbout A, Kievit J. Different formats

for communicating surgical risks to patients and the effect on choice of

treatment. Patient Educ Couns 2004;54:255–63.

[25] Fuller R, Dudley N, Blacktop J. Risk communication and older people-

understanding of probability and risk information by medical inpatients

aged 75 years and older. Age Ageing 2001;30:473–6.

[26] Fuller R, Dudley N, Blacktop J. How informed is consent? Understanding

of pictorial and verbal probability information by medical inpatients.

Postgrad Med J 2002;78:543–4.

[27] McCaffery KJ, Trevena L, Barratt A, Clarke J, Armstrong B, O’Connor A.

Examining the acceptability of a decision aid developed for HRT among

consumers from multiethnic backgrounds and health practitioners. In:

International Shared Decision Making Conference; 2005.

[28] Cleveland WS, McGill R, Graphical Perception. Graphical methods for

analyzing scientific data. Science 1985;229:828–33.

[29] Hollands JG, Spence I. The discrimination of graphical elements. Appl

Cognitive Psych 2001;15:413–31.

[30] Carswell CM. Choosing specifiers: an evaluation of the basic tasks model

of graphical perception. Hum Factors 1992;34:535–54.

[31] Sparrow JA. Graphical displays in information systems: some data proper-

ties influencing the effectiveness of alternative forms. Behav Inform

Technol 1989;8:43–56.

[32] Tufte ER. The visual display of quantitative information, 2nd ed.,

Cheshire: Graphics Press; 2001.

[33] Gillan DJ, Richman EH. Minimalism and the syntax of graphs. Hum

Factors 1994;36:619–44.

[34] Laarni J, Simola J, Kojo I, Nasanen R. Reading vertical text from a

computer screen. Behav Inform Technol 2004;23:75–82.

[35] Seo H, Lee C. Head-free reading of horizontally and vertically arranged

texts. Vision Res 2002;42:1325–37.

[36] Lipkus IM, Crawford Y, Fenn K, Biradavolu M, Binder RA, Marcus A,

Mason M. Testing different formats for communicating colorectal cancer

risk. J Health Commun 1999;4:311–24.