15
This article was downloaded by: [University of Kiel] On: 09 November 2014, At: 06:30 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Early Years: An International Research Journal Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ceye20 Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing Lily Chan a , Cheng Zi Juan a & Chan Lai Foon b a Faculty of Education , The Chinese University of Hong Kong b Po Leung Kuk Tin Ka Ping Siu Hong Kindergarten Published online: 14 Sep 2010. To cite this article: Lily Chan , Cheng Zi Juan & Chan Lai Foon (2008) Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing, Early Years: An International Research Journal, 28:2, 135-148, DOI: 10.1080/09575140801945304 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09575140801945304 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms- and-conditions

Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing

  • Upload
    chan

  • View
    219

  • Download
    1

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing

This article was downloaded by: [University of Kiel]On: 09 November 2014, At: 06:30Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registeredoffice: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Early Years: An International ResearchJournalPublication details, including instructions for authors andsubscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ceye20

Chinese preschool children's literacydevelopment: from emergent toconventional writingLily Chan a , Cheng Zi Juan a & Chan Lai Foon ba Faculty of Education , The Chinese University of Hong Kongb Po Leung Kuk Tin Ka Ping Siu Hong KindergartenPublished online: 14 Sep 2010.

To cite this article: Lily Chan , Cheng Zi Juan & Chan Lai Foon (2008) Chinese preschool children'sliteracy development: from emergent to conventional writing, Early Years: An InternationalResearch Journal, 28:2, 135-148, DOI: 10.1080/09575140801945304

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09575140801945304

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as tothe accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinionsand views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Contentshould not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sourcesof information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever orhowsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arisingout of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Anysubstantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Page 2: Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing

Chinese preschool children’s literacy development: from emergent toconventional writing

Lily Chana*, Cheng Zi Juana and Chan Lai Foonb

aFaculty of Education, The Chinese University of Hong Kong; bPo Leung Kuk Tin Ka Ping SiuHong Kindergarten

A Chinese script is represented by Chinese characters and each character is asquare-shaped configuration with condensed strokes. Children in Hong Kong areexplicitly taught to write at a very young age. They are guided to draw verticaland horizontal lines at age three, and are required to write simple characters withfew strokes at age four. When children are ready for formal schooling at age six,many of them can write around 50–60 characters. Typical writing lessons consistof penmanship drills and assigned character copying tasks, but in somepreschools teachers have begun to change the writing curriculum and make adistinction between emergent literacy and conventional literacy. Early writingsare marked by scribbles, drawings and invented spellings, and these are regardedas signs of active exploration for children to understand the writing system. Thepresent study introduces the learning experience of a four-year-old girl who wasencouraged by her teacher and parents to be an emergent writer. Her writingswent through several stages of development within a year, and her spontaneousexperiments with writing in the initial stage contributed significantly to herbecoming a fairly independent and confident writer.

Keywords: written Chinese; early childhood education; literacy development

Introduction

Evidence has been gathered in the past 30 years demonstrating that preschool

children brought up in a literate society actively make sense of the print they

encounter. If they are provided with opportunities to engage in reading and writing

activities, they can develop many insights into the functions of written language for

themselves. It is believed that significant written language development can occur

during this preschool stage (Goelman et al. 1984; Farr 1985; Teale and Sulzby 1986).

The term ‘emergent’ was introduced to describe features of early reading and writing

development as distinct from those expected in conventional literacy. In this

emergent stage, children begin to match spoken words with print, to recognize some

words in their environment, and to engage in pretend reading and writing activities.

Young children learn to capture their own ideas through symbols, beginning with

pictures and scribbling, and eventually learn the letters/characters that are used in

their society. They also learn to use writing for their own purposes – to record

information and to express their own ideas.

Early literacy develops through stages during which the conflicting information

provided by the complex written language system has to be assimilated and

accommodated (Ferreiro 1986), and which are marked by children making apparent

progress and then seeming to regress. In order to be able to write, children have to

*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]

Early Years

Vol. 28, No. 2, July 2008, 135–148

ISSN 0957-5146 print/ISSN 1472-4421 online

# 2008 TACTYC

DOI: 10.1080/09575140801945304

http://www.informaworld.com

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f K

iel]

at 0

6:30

09

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 3: Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing

understand the relationship between speech and print, the differentiation between

drawing and writing, and the orthographic structure of a particular script. A number

of studies have documented the kind of learning that is essential for children to learn

an English script. For example, Read (1971) depicts the amazing journey children

have to go through to make sense of the sound–letter correspondence, and Clay(1975) highlights a series of graphemic principles children have to develop in order to

learn to write and spell using alphabets. There are only a handful of studies that

examine literacy learning in Chinese, especially its emergent stage. Written Chinese is

the oldest script still in use today, and the earliest Chinese writing in existence dates

back 3500 years. The following section provides a brief description of written

Chinese and its orthographic structure.

What is written Chinese?

Chinese has long been classified as a logographic language, which refers to the use of

symbols to represent words or morphemes. Temple (Temple et al. 1988) uses theterm ‘ideographic writing’ and describes Chinese writing as a system based on the

principle of using a single symbol to represent an idea. This statement, however, is

only partly correct, as only around 10% of the characters still in use today are

ideographic in nature, and originated from pictorial or symbolic depiction. Over 90%

of the characters consist of at least some linguistic information that provides

phonological and/or semantic cues (Zhu 1987). A character is a squared-shaped

configuration made up of strokes and the combination of strokes forming stroke-

patterns. While strokes provide only graphic information, stroke-pattern in acharacter normally provides linguistic information to get to the meaning and

pronunciation. As for the size of the Chinese lexicon, about 3000 characters account

for 99% of the total frequency of Chinese words in modern books and newspapers in

mainland Chinese, according to the Foreign Languages Press Beijing (1989).

According to another survey in Taiwan, there are about 4500 characters and about

40,000 words in daily use (Liu et al. 1975).

Orthographic units of written Chinese

The Chinese script looks very different from an alphabetic script because the basicorthographic units of the two scripts are very different. Written English has three

principal structural levels (letter, word and sentence), whereas Chinese has five levels

(stroke, stroke-pattern, character, word and sentence) (Figure 1). A stroke is the

smallest writing unit in Chinese. There are eight types of strokes commonly in use,

such as dots, lines and hooks. The number of strokes in a character can vary from

one to over 20. Similar to alphabetic writing, there is a sequence for putting the

strokes together to form Chinese characters. Putting letters in order is simple in

English since all words are written in a linear, left to right direction. But Chinesecharacters are two-dimensional; the sequence might start anywhere, with no set end-

point. It is believed that a fixed sequence of strokes for all characters can prevent any

strokes from being left out, and characters can be remembered better if proper

sequence is observed.

Stroke-patterns are formed by putting individual strokes together. Many stroke-

patterns can be used as characters in isolation, or they can be used as components in

complicated characters. The total number of stroke-patterns commonly in use is

136 Lily Chan et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f K

iel]

at 0

6:30

09

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 4: Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing

estimated to be in the hundreds. Unlike strokes, most stroke-patterns are

pronounceable, and have a meaning of their own.

A character can be classified as simple or complicated. A simple character

consists of only one stroke-pattern and a complicated character is formed from two

or more stroke-patterns. Every Chinese character is confined to a fixed region,

irrespective of the number of strokes in a character. Simple and complicated

characters all occupy the same prescribed area in regular printed materials. The same

does not hold for alphabetic scripts, where the greater the number of letters, the

longer the string. Characters can be written either in vertical columns from the top

down, or in horizontal columns from left to right.

In English, a word is identified by the space at either end of a letter string in a

text. In Chinese, a word can be written using one character, two characters or three

characters with no graphic linkage between the characters to indicate the boundary

of a word.

Figure 1. Orthographic units in written Chinese.

Early Years 137

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f K

iel]

at 0

6:30

09

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 5: Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing

A sentence consists of words. Characters in text are evenly spaced with no extra

space between words. Readers have to segment character strings into words for

comprehension. Segmenting larger segments in Chinese sentences is the same as in

English text; clause and phrase boundaries are indicated by commas and sentence

boundaries are marked by full stops.

Children learning written Chinese

Many studies have demonstrated that visual skills are important in learning to read

Chinese. Visual skills have been found to relate to Chinese reading at first grade (Lee

et al. 1986) as well as at third grade (Huang and Hanley 1995). Ho and Bryant (1997)

found that differences in visual-perceptual skills in preschool children significantly

predict reading ability one year later. Hanley et al. (1999) highlighted the importance

for children to be able to distinguish thousands of different visual symbols in order

to become proficient readers of Chinese. Given that Chinese characters are more

visually distinct than words in an alphabetic script (Chen 1996), children are likely to

rely heavily on visual strategies to learn to read new characters. In an earlier study

(Chan and Nunes 1998), we found that preschool children were able to recognize

the positions of stroke-patterns, and were able to recognize graphically acceptable

stroke-patterns at a character level.

In phonological processing, five-year-old preschool children can use one-to-one

correspondence to match a syllable to a character when they read (Lee 1989; Chan

1990). This finding is consistent with studies carried out with young children reading

alphabetic script (Ferreiro and Teberosky 1982), and confirms that even preschool

children have little difficulty matching a syllable to a written word.

How can all these studies contribute to our understanding of preschool children’s

writing development? It appears that Chinese children have already acquired some

rudimentary knowledge of the written language system, and they can apply this

knowledge if they are given an opportunity to write. Writing one’s own name has

always been regarded as an important landmark in a child’s early literacy

development. Children can usually write their own names at a more advanced

developmental level compared with writing other unfamiliar words (Tolchinsky-

Landsman and Levin 1987; Scarlett 1989). In order to examine whether Chinese

children can write their own names at preschool level, Chan and Louie (1992) invited

60 children, aged 2–6 years from Hong Kong to draw a picture of themselves and

write their own name. It was found that 75% of the five-year-olds could write their

names correctly, and more than 50% of the four-year-olds could use strokes and

stroke-patterns to make appropriate writing. As for the three-year-olds, 75% of them

could differentiate drawing from writing, but only 20% of them were able to produce

appropriate writing. More studies are needed to trace this crucial developmental

stage and examine how preschool literacy practices can support children to gradually

develop their understanding of Chinese writing.

In Hong Kong, over 98% of children from two to six years of age are sent to

either nurseries or kindergartens (Chan and Chan 2003). Literacy instruction begins

at age three, when children are systematically introduced to learning to read and

write Chinese. The literacy activities include character recognition, storybook

reading and guided writing. Li (2005) observed that insufficient attention is given to

reading activities in most preschools in Hong Kong, reading sessions are short and

138 Lily Chan et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f K

iel]

at 0

6:30

09

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 6: Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing

infrequent, and there are insufficient books available in the classroom for children to

read. Teachers spend a lot of time helping children to recognize individual

characters. They introduce one to two new characters every day, pronounce them,

and explain the graphic structure of the characters. A typical character recognition

activity lasts 20–30 minutes, during which children are required to sit together andpay attention to the teacher. The teacher introduces the pronunciation of the

character and then shows the children how to write it, paying a lot of attention to the

placement and order of strokes. Children are required to practice writing the newly

taught character in the following group session, and the teacher walks around to

check whether the character is correctly written in exercise books marked with big

squares. If the character is not correctly written, individual guidance is provided. In

the reception class (for children aged five) in some preschools, guided writing is

introduced, children are encouraged to make sentences with some of the taughtvocabularies, and they are encouraged to write short sentences on their own.

Learning to write is regarded as the outcome of writing instruction, and precision

and accuracy in character formation are the major criteria for good writing at

preschool level.

Recently, the emergent view in literacy acquisition has been attracting the

attention of some preschool teachers. Reading and writing are treated as a means to

engage children in literacy, rather than an end to ensure that children learn a

particular set of characters or sentence structures. They are more aware of the impactof environmental print, they introduce more stories to class, and they observe how

each child is actively making sense out of print in his or her unique way. The

developmental approach to writing is adopted, and writing can be drawing,

scribbling or invented characters. Children are told that writing is fun, and they are

encouraged to write for meaningful, authentic literacy events. The following section

describes the kind of changes being introduced in a preschool classroom, and the

impact on writing development for a young child.

Methodology

This study uses a longitudinal case study of a four-year-old girl to investigate howshe interpreted both the forms and functions of the Chinese writing system. A total

of 30 pieces of writing were collected from the child within a period of 12 months,

from age 4 years 5 months to 5 years 4 months. A variety of data collection methods

were used to capture the child’s progress and kind of support that facilitated her

becoming an independent writer.

The participant

Ting-ting was brought up in a typical Hong Kong family. Her father was a driver

and her mother was a full-time housewife. She had an elder brother three years olderthan her. At home, she could observe various literacy activities such as her parents

reading newspapers, and her brother doing his homework.

Ting-ting enrolled in a preschool in Hong Kong when she was three years of age.

She enjoyed going to school, and actively participated in activities. In school, she

loved to draw and play with other children. The literacy curriculum of her preschool

included storybook reading, singing songs, discussing and acting out stories, and

writing a variety of texts. Children gathered together to listen to stories in the

Early Years 139

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f K

iel]

at 0

6:30

09

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 7: Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing

morning. They were invited to spend time in the comfortable reading area, which

was equipped with big books, poems, puppets and tape-recorders. In the writing

area, teachers placed different kinds of writing paper, pencils, crayons and name

stamps on the shelves, and a whiteboard to display children’s work. A writing

routine was set up, and children were encouraged to draw pictures and write aboutwhat interested them on a weekly basis. From time to time, the teacher demonstrated

proper ways to write Chinese characters to help children to understand the graphic

conventions of written Chinese. At the same time, children were told that they were

writers, and were encouraged to express their ideas in drawing, scribbles, and in

characters or words that they knew.

Tin-ting was very interested in the print in the environment and her subsequent

spontaneous experiments with writing. She spent a lot of time in the reading and

writing areas, reading stories, poems, and enjoying herself with various writingactivities.

Results

Ting-ting’s writing could be divided into three stages. Each piece of writing was

analyzed according to its superordinate graphic features and its ordinate graphic

features (Tolchinsky-Landsman and Levin 1985). Superordinate graphic features

refer to constricted size, presence of units, regularity of blanks and directionality. In

written Chinese, constricted size can be assessed by judging whether all the strokes

are packed into a square-shaped configuration. Presence of units refers to the use of

more than one kind of stroke in forming a character. Regularity of blanks ismeasured by equal space being allocated between characters, and directionality in

written Chinese is correct if the writing is from left to right, or from top to bottom.

One score is given to each category if a written configuration is assessed to have

attained the prescribed requirements.

Ordinate graphic features indicate how well a written configuration can be

treated as a Chinese character, and it can fall into one of the following categories: use

of vertical and horizontal strokes only; approximate writing related to utterance; and

correct writing. A written configuration can be made by using vertical and horizontal

strokes; it may look very much like a Chinese character, but no one can make a guess

regarding what the configuration is about. Approximate writing related to utterance

refers to an attempt to write a correct character, but there are some mistakes made

either in the combination of strokes, or in stroke-patterns. Correct writing indicates

that the choice of character is correct and it is composed accurately. One score is

allocated to any one of the three categories to decide which description is the best fit.

Stage 1: emergent writing

A total of seven pieces of writing were collected and clearly illustrated thecharacteristics of emergent writing in written Chinese. The first piece of writing was

made at the request of the teacher to see if Ting-ting could draw a picture of herself

and write her own name. Ting-ting made a very vivid picture of herself, and began

her first writing task (Figure 2). In her name writing, she demonstrated the presence

of units by using various strokes and stroke-patterns in her writing, but she could not

constrict the size of characters, and made differentiation of the blanks to be allocated

between characters and between stroke-patterns. In addition, Ting-ting had not yet

140 Lily Chan et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f K

iel]

at 0

6:30

09

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 8: Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing

acquired the knowledge of directionality of the Chinese writing system. She wrote

her own name upside down, with her surname at the bottom, and the last name on

top. (The surname of Ting-ting is and her given name is . In Chinese, the

whole name is written as , with the surname in front.)

Ting-ting was actively exploring the functions of writing. She wrote a story using

invented spelling and drew a picture with flowers, trees and insects. She wrote, ‘Trees

are pretty, love the nature, rainbow, bees, flowers, butterfly, so happy’ (Figure 3).

She was able to use one-to-one correspondence, matching a character with a syllable

when she read the story. All the written configurations in the story were made-up

characters, but Ting-ting paid attention that none of them was the same. She

manipulated the strokes and stroke-patterns, and created 21 totally different written

configurations. She demonstrated her understanding of two very important rules in

the writing system: matching the sound to the words, and using different symbols to

represent different sounds.

Apart from making stories, Ting-ting wrote diaries in her daily writing routine,

mimicking the habit of her elder brother. One day, Ting-ting told her teacher that she

wanted to write a diary, and began her first diary writing. She used symbols such as

smiling face to represent ‘I’ and ‘me’. She was able to write two simple characters

correctly. When she read, she matched one character to one symbol. She wrote, ‘I

love rainbow, there are butterflies and birds in the sky, and there are pigs, horses and

sheep in the farm. I am very happy. Four years old. My brother loves me. 22-11

(date), (and her name)’ (Figure 4). Table 1 presents the performance of Ting-ting in

Figure 2. Writing of Ting-ting in emergent writing stage (writing 1).

Early Years 141

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f K

iel]

at 0

6:30

09

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 9: Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing

her writing in three stages. In stage 1, she made a total of 101 written configurations,

with an average of 14 written configurations in each piece of writing. She demonstrated

that she had grasped the superordinate graphic features of the Chinese writing system,

even though she was not able to make many correct pieces of writing.

Stage 2: approximate writing

Ting-ting paid a lot of attention to environmental print, and she was keen to copy

the characters she came across in books or in other reading materials. She copied the

name of her preschool, and made a poster for the preschool supermarket in her

pretend play. She learned the names of her teachers and her best friend in class. She

used both drawing and writing to facilitate her learning; for example, she wrote

down the stages of the growth of a butterfly, she noted down information she found

about the flower, lilac, and she also used drawing and writing to explain how leaves

produce chlorophyll (Figure 5).

A total of 13 pieces of writing were collected in a five-month period. Ting-ting

wrote less in Stage 2 as compared with Stage 1: she made only 118 written

configurations, with an average of nine in each piece of writing. She adopted the

superordinate graphic features in her writing, and improved significantly in the

ordinate graphic features. Through observation and copying, Ting-ting was able to

make 88% correct writing, and 12% approximate writing related to utterance (see

Table 1).

Figure 3. Writing of Ting-ting in emergent writing stage (story writing).

142 Lily Chan et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f K

iel]

at 0

6:30

09

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 10: Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing

Stage 3: conventional writing

Ting-ting enjoyed drawing and writing, and initiated a lot of writing activities in

class. She copied the rhymes from the rhyme books (Figure 6), and made her own

poetic lines for pleasure. Writing was also utilized for recording to facilitate learning.

She had also acquired the written ability required for her to make short sentences in

her assignments.

Figure 4. Writing of Ting-ting in emergent writing stage (diary writing).

Early Years 143

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f K

iel]

at 0

6:30

09

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 11: Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing

Instead of using short phrases or just a few characters, Ting-ting wrote in lines

and paragraphs. She paid attention to the sentence structure, and use punctuation

marks such as commas, full stops and question marks. Her literacy acquisition

enabled her to be more expressive with her feelings, as shown in Figure 4. Ting-ting

missed her grandma and would like to have sent her a present. She took out a book

and copied a rhyme entitled ‘Visiting my grandmother’. There were altogether 24

Table 1. Percentage of written configurations demonstrating superordinate graphic features

and ordinate graphic features, and total number of written configurations in the children’s

writing (Stages 1–3)

Stage 1 Stage 2 Stage 3

Superordinate graphic features:

Constricted size 90% 98% 100%

Presence of units 100% 100% 100%

Regularity of blanks 98% 99% 100%

Directionality 97% 98% 100%

Ordinate graphic features:

Use of vertical and horizontal strokes 86% 0% 0%

Approximate writing related to utterance 0% 12% 1%

Correct writing 14% 88% 99%

Number of written configurations:

Total number of written configurations 101 118 216

Average number of written configurations in each writing 14.4 9.1 21.6

Figure 5. Writing of Ting-ting in approximate writing stage.

144 Lily Chan et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f K

iel]

at 0

6:30

09

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 12: Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing

characters in the rhyme. Many of them were complicated characters which consisted

of 13 to 20 strokes. It would be impossible for her to be able to remember and copy

these characters unless she had already acquired visual analysis skills, and was able

to segment the characters into stroke-patterns.

It appeared that Ting-ting had also acquired the necessary skills to be able to

write independently. In Stage 3, she made 10 pieces of writing, with a total of 216

characters. The number of characters in each piece of writing increased by 100% as

compared with Stage 2. Not only could she follow the superordinate graphic features

in her writing but she managed to get 99% of her writing correct. Her performance in

Stage 3 was excellent and way above the overall performance of her classmates who

would never venture to using writing for pleasure, or be able to copy a rhyme

without any assistance from the teacher.

Discussion

The case study that we presented describes three distinctive stages that a four-year-

old child has gone through in the process of learning to write. The following five

areas of literacy practices in the preschool have probably facilitated Ting-ting’s

becoming a young writer.

First, teachers have to value children’s imagination and their ability to draw in

order to express their ideas. When Ting-ting was first introduced to writing, her

writing was used as an important source of information for the teacher to

communicate with her. In the initial stage, Ting-ting used a variety of expressive

Figure 6. Writing of Ting-ting in conventional writing stage.

Early Years 145

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f K

iel]

at 0

6:30

09

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 13: Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing

modes in her writing, experimenting with her writing styles, and explored the graphic

structure of written Chinese. The teacher supported her exploration and observed

her progress. Encouraging Ting-ting to become a writer has allowed her to

externalize her understanding about the environment, and to communicate it to

other people.

Second, visual skills and spatial concepts are important in learning to write

Chinese. From time to time, the teacher conducted demonstrations and lessons to

explain how characters can be segmented into stroke-patterns, how the strokes can

be combined to form a character, and how characters can be written in a proper way.

Ting-ting was given a lot of time to practice what she had learned, which enabled her

to strengthen her fine motor skills and learn how to combine strokes with precision

and accuracy. Ting-ting could hold her pen for a long time, and coped with the

physical demands of the writing tasks. Her writing is legible in the very initial stage

of learning to write, and she demonstrated good handwriting skills. Her fine motor

skills were so well developed that she could draw and write for half an hour at age

five. When automaticity with handwriting is achieved, she can free up mental

capacity to deal with other demands of the writing process, such as compositional

demands.

The third element is encouragement from her teacher, and the constant

reinforcement that Ting-ting is a good writer. The head teacher met up with Ting-

ting every week to discuss her work for one whole year. As a general practice, Ting-

ting was invited to introduce her drawing and writing, and positive feedback was

given to praise the child’s effort. Ting-ting loved her head teacher who was pretty

and happy, and she would like to be like her and be a head teacher when she grows

up. Dunsmuir and Blatchford (2004) point out that teacher assessments of children’s

attitudes to writing are significantly associated with writing competence. This may

not just be a reflection of the child’s attitude as assessed by the teacher, but an

indicator of a positive interaction between them around writing activities.

The fourth element is related to the variety of writing activities Ting-ting was

invited to engage in. Ting-ting’s learning process was supported by a print-rich

environment; the teachers brought to class stories, rhymes and all kinds of books

that were of interest to the children. More flexible arrangements were made for

children to be able to read and write in class. She was provided with regular extended

writing experiences across a range of forms, such as stories, poems, greetings and

note-taking. These activities have been found to be strongly associated with progress

in writing, as reported in the study by Snow et al. (1991)’s longitudinal on five- to

seven-year-old children. Children who have the opportunity to write for different

purposes are found to be more advanced in their writing ability than children whose

teachers provide fewer and narrower writing opportunities.

The last element and probably the most crucial factor is the child’s motivation to

learn to write. Motivated writers enjoy the activity and gain intrinsic satisfaction on

completion of a task. The reason why Ting-ting is so motivated to write is probably

because no one has ever told her that she cannot write, or that what she has written is

incorrect. Children who are anxious that their writings are being judged or assessed

are more likely to state that they do not enjoy it and procrastinate. These children

will have difficulty generating content and are more likely to avoid writing,

displaying higher levels of off-task behavior than motivated writers. The implication

of this case study is that capturing a child’s interest and enjoyment may be the key to

146 Lily Chan et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f K

iel]

at 0

6:30

09

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 14: Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing

promoting writing development, and educators need to be aware of the desirability

of providing tasks that children perceive as purposeful and valuable.

The learning process of written Chinese is similar to those learning an alphabetic

script. Chinese children begin their search for the nature of the representational

system of written Chinese. Initially they differentiate drawing and writing, then they

create their own writing with strokes and stroke-patterns, and apply unique graphic

features exclusive for written Chinese in their creative writing. Children gradually

become aware that words convey meaning, and that writing is a vehicle for

transmitting information. They are conscious that ideas must be written in a

conventional way so that other people can understand them too. Children also

acquire the basic understanding of the relationship between script and speech, that

is, the knowledge of one-to-one correspondence between the syllable and the

character in their writing.

The acquisition of ordinate graphic features highlights the specific knowledge to

decode written Chinese. It appears that very young children can perceive visual

features of the graphic displays – linearity, constricted size, non-pictoriality – in their

early writing. Children initially establish their basic writing model using vertical or

horizontal lines, and separate symbol units. The perception of the composition of

strokes and stroke-patterns is increasingly differentiated with age, and is quite well

differentiated by age five. However, it is insufficient merely to rely on this kind of

global graphic and phonological awareness to achieve a higher level of proficiency.

To be able to read and write Chinese, children have to acquire the skill to conduct

intra-character analysis and be able to understand the forms and functions of the

stroke-patterns.

To conclude, there is evidence that Chinese preschool children are actively

exploring the written language system. Emergent literacy can be facilitated in the

preschool setting in Hong Kong, by providing a print-rich environment and quality

adult support. Mastery of the graphic conventions of written Chinese is demanding,

but measures can be introduced to make this learning process more fun and

meaningful.

References

Chan, L. 1990. Preschool children’s understanding of Chinese writing. Unpublished Master’s

thesis, University of London.

Chan, L., and L. Louie. 1992. Developmental trend of Chinese preschool children in drawing

and writing. Journal of Research in Childhood Education 6, no. 2: 93–99.

Chan, L., and T. Nunes. 1998. Children’s understanding of the formal and functional

characteristics of written Chinese. Applied Psycholinguistics 19: 115–131.

Chan, L.K.S., and L. Chan. 2003. Early childhood education in Hong Kong and its

challenges. Early Child Development and Care 173: 7–17.

Chen, H.-C. 1996. Chinese reading and comprehension: a cognitive psychology perspective. In

The handbook of Chinese psychology, ed. M.H. Bond, 43–62. Hong Kong: Oxford

University Press.

Clay, M.M. 1975. What did I write? Auckland: Heinemann Educational.

Dunsmuir, S., and P. Blatchford. 2004. Predictors of writing competence in 4- to 7-year-old

children. British Journal of Educational Psychology 74: 461–483.

Farr, M. 1985. Advances in writing research, Vol. 1: Children’s early writing development.

Norwood, NJ: Albex.

Early Years 147

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f K

iel]

at 0

6:30

09

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 15: Chinese preschool children's literacy development: from emergent to conventional writing

Ferreiro, E. 1986. The Interplay between information and assimilation in beginning literacy.

In Emergent literacy, writing and reading, ed. W.H. Teale and E. Sulzby, 15–49.

Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Ferreiro, E., and A. Teberosky. 1982. Literacy before schooling. Exeter: Heinemann

Educational.

Foreign Languages Press Beijing. 1989. Chinese characters. Beijing: Foreign Language Press.

Goelman, H., A. Oberg, and F. Smith. 1984. Awakening to literacy. London: Heinemann

Educational.

Hanley, J.R., O. Tzeng, and H.-S. Huang. 1999. Learning to read Chinese. In Learning to read

and write: A cross linguistic perspective, ed. M. Harris and G. Hatano, 173–195.

Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ho, C.S.-H., and P. Bryant. 1997. Phonological skills are important in learning to read

Chinese. Developmental Psychology 33: 946–951.

Huang, H.S., and J.R. Hanley. 1995. Phonological awareness and visual skills in learning to

read Chinese and English. Cognition 54: 73–98.

Lee, L.-J. 1989. Emergent literacy in Chinese: print awareness of young children in Taiwan.

Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Arizona.

Lee, S.-Y., J.W. Stigler, and H.W. Stevenson. 1986. Beginning reading in Chinese and English.

In Acquisition of reading skills: Cultural constraints cognitive universals, ed. B.R.

Foorman and A.W. Siegel, 123–148. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Li, H., and N. Rao. 2005. Curricular and instructional influences on early literacy attainment:

evidence from Beijing, Hong Kong and Singapore. International Journal of Early Years

Education 13, no. 3: 235–253.

Liu, I.M., C.J. Chuang, and S.C. Wang. 1975. Frequency count of 40,000 Chinese words.

Taipei: Lucky Books.

National Committee on Chinese Langauge. 1989. Chinese characters. Beijing: Beijing

Language Press.

Read, C. 1971. Pre-school children’s knowledge of English phonology. Educational Review 41:

1–34.

Scarlett, C.M. 1989. Children’s understanding of written language before formal teaching

instruction. Unpublished MA dissertation, University of London.

Snow, C.E., W.S. Barnes, J. Candler, I.F. Goodman, and L. Hemphill. 1991. Unfulfilled

expectations: home and school influences on literacy. London: Harvard University Press.

Teale, W.H., and E. Sulzby. 1986. Emergent literacy: writing and reading. Norwood, NJ:

Ablex.

Temple, C., R. Nathan, N. Burris, and F. Temple. 1988. The beginning of writing. Boston,

MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Tolchinsky-Landsman, L., and I. Levin. 1985. Writing in preschoolers: an age-related analysis.

Applied Psycholinguistics 6: 310–339.

———. 1987. Writing in four-to six-year-olds: representation of semantic and phonetic

similarities and differences. Journal of Child Language 14: 127–144.

Zhu, Y.P. 1987. Analysis of cueing functions of the phonetic in modern China. Unpublished

paper, East China Normal University (in Chinese).

148 Lily Chan et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f K

iel]

at 0

6:30

09

Nov

embe

r 20

14