10
Psychological Science 24(8) 1398–1407 © The Author(s) 2013 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0956797612473122 pss.sagepub.com Research Article In alphabetic orthographies, letters represent phonemes (speech sounds). Such orthographies differ in their pho- nological consistency (how predictable the relationship is between letters in printed words and phonemes in spoken words). English, of all languages studied, has the least consistent alphabetic orthography in this respect, and as a result, children appear to learn to read more slowly in English than in languages with more consistent orthographies (Share, 2008; Ziegler & Goswami, 2005). Most evidence about differences in the rate of reading development among different orthographies comes from cross-sectional studies. In a study of 14 European coun- tries, Seymour, Aro, and Erskine (2003) showed that chil- dren learning to read in languages with relatively consistent orthographies had fluent and accurate (> 80%) word reading by the end of Grade 1, whereas in English, children had much lower levels of accuracy (34%; see also Share, 2008; Ziegler & Goswami, 2005). However, no study has yet been conducted to directly compare the growth of early reading skills among different language groups in a longitudinal study using directly equivalent measures. Therefore, it is not known how patterns of growth in reading differ, nor whether the predictors of growth in reading skill differ, across languages. We addressed these questions here, in the first cross-linguistic study of the patterns of growth in reading. Rates of Growth in Different Orthographies Longitudinal studies focusing on the earliest phases of reading development in individual languages report 473122PSS XX X 10.1177/0956797612473122Caravolas et al.Literacy Development research-article 2013 Corresponding Author: Charles Hulme, Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, Chandler House, 2 Wakefield St., London, England WC1N 1PF E-mail: [email protected] Different Patterns, but Equivalent Predictors, of Growth in Reading in Consistent and Inconsistent Orthographies Markéta Caravolas 1 , Arne Lervåg 2 , Sylvia Defior 3 , Gabriela Seidlová Málková 4 , and Charles Hulme 5 1 School of Psychology, Bangor University; 2 Department of Educational Research, University of Oslo; 3 Department of Educational Psychology, University of Granada; 4 Department of Education, Charles University; and 5 Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London Abstract All alphabetic orthographies use letters in printed words to represent the phonemes in spoken words, but they differ in the consistency of the relationship between letters and phonemes. English appears to be the least consistent alphabetic orthography phonologically, and, consequently, children learn to read more slowly in English than in languages with more consistent orthographies. In this article, we report the first longitudinal evidence that the growth of reading skills is slower and follows a different trajectory in English than in two much more consistent orthographies (Spanish and Czech). Nevertheless, phoneme awareness, letter-sound knowledge, and rapid automatized naming measured at the onset of literacy instruction did not differ in importance as predictors of variations in reading development among the three languages. These findings suggest that although children may learn to read more rapidly in more consistent than in less consistent orthographies, there may nevertheless be universal cognitive prerequisites for learning to read in all alphabetic orthographies. Keywords cognitive development, reading, educational psychology Received 8/14/12; Revision accepted 11/27/12

C1.M52_Cuadro_Caravolas+et+al%2C+2013%2C+Diff+patterns.pdf

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: C1.M52_Cuadro_Caravolas+et+al%2C+2013%2C+Diff+patterns.pdf

Psychological Science24(8) 1398 –1407© The Author(s) 2013Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navDOI: 10.1177/0956797612473122pss.sagepub.com

Research Article

In alphabetic orthographies, letters represent phonemes (speech sounds). Such orthographies differ in their pho-nological consistency (how predictable the relationship is between letters in printed words and phonemes in spoken words). English, of all languages studied, has the least consistent alphabetic orthography in this respect, and as a result, children appear to learn to read more slowly in English than in languages with more consistent orthographies (Share, 2008; Ziegler & Goswami, 2005). Most evidence about differences in the rate of reading development among different orthographies comes from cross-sectional studies. In a study of 14 European coun-tries, Seymour, Aro, and Erskine (2003) showed that chil-dren learning to read in languages with relatively consistent orthographies had fluent and accurate (> 80%) word reading by the end of Grade 1, whereas in English, children had much lower levels of accuracy (34%; see also Share, 2008; Ziegler & Goswami, 2005). However, no study has yet been conducted to directly compare the

growth of early reading skills among different language groups in a longitudinal study using directly equivalent measures. Therefore, it is not known how patterns of growth in reading differ, nor whether the predictors of growth in reading skill differ, across languages. We addressed these questions here, in the first cross-linguistic study of the patterns of growth in reading.

Rates of Growth in Different Orthographies

Longitudinal studies focusing on the earliest phases of reading development in individual languages report

473122 PSSXXX10.1177/0956797612473122Caravolas et al.Literacy Developmentresearch-article2013

Corresponding Author:Charles Hulme, Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, Chandler House, 2 Wakefield St., London, England WC1N 1PF E-mail: [email protected]

Different Patterns, but Equivalent Predictors, of Growth in Reading in Consistent and Inconsistent Orthographies

Markéta Caravolas1, Arne Lervåg2, Sylvia Defior3, Gabriela Seidlová Málková4, and Charles Hulme5

1School of Psychology, Bangor University; 2Department of Educational Research, University of Oslo; 3Department of Educational Psychology, University of Granada; 4Department of Education, Charles University; and 5Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London

AbstractAll alphabetic orthographies use letters in printed words to represent the phonemes in spoken words, but they differ in the consistency of the relationship between letters and phonemes. English appears to be the least consistent alphabetic orthography phonologically, and, consequently, children learn to read more slowly in English than in languages with more consistent orthographies. In this article, we report the first longitudinal evidence that the growth of reading skills is slower and follows a different trajectory in English than in two much more consistent orthographies (Spanish and Czech). Nevertheless, phoneme awareness, letter-sound knowledge, and rapid automatized naming measured at the onset of literacy instruction did not differ in importance as predictors of variations in reading development among the three languages. These findings suggest that although children may learn to read more rapidly in more consistent than in less consistent orthographies, there may nevertheless be universal cognitive prerequisites for learning to read in all alphabetic orthographies.

Keywordscognitive development, reading, educational psychology

Received 8/14/12; Revision accepted 11/27/12

Page 2: C1.M52_Cuadro_Caravolas+et+al%2C+2013%2C+Diff+patterns.pdf

Literacy Development 1399

patterns of growth that are nonlinear in English (Compton, 2003; Skibbe, Grimm, Bowles, & Morrison, 2012; Torgesen et al., 1999), Finnish (e.g., Leppänen, Niemi, Aunola, & Nurmi, 2004; Parrila, Aunola, Leskinen, Nurmi, & Kirby, 2005), and Dutch (Verhoeven & van Leeuwe, 2011). The study by Skibbe et al. (2012) of English-speaking U.S. children from preschool to Grade 2 found that growth in single-word reading skills accelerated at the start of schooling and decelerated in second grade. However, the duration and perhaps the acceleration of the growth spurt that accompanies the start of reading instruction may vary across different orthographies. Children learn-ing to read English seem to undergo a longer lasting growth spurt in kindergarten and Grade 1 (Hill, Bloom, Rebeck Black, & Lipsey, 2008; Skibbe et al., 2012), whereas the growth spurt for children learning the highly consistent Finnish orthography appears limited to the first grade only (Leppänen et al., 2004; Parrila et al., 2005). However, these studies differed in many critical ways, including the reading measures used, which makes comparisons among languages difficult.

Predictors of Growth in Different Orthographies

In addition to understanding the patterns of growth in reading across languages, it is important to understand the cognitive skills influencing individual differences in rates of growth. Claims about possible differences in the predictors of reading achievement in different languages have generated controversy. There is now good evidence that word-reading ability in English is predicted by three core skills: letter knowledge, awareness of phonemes in spoken words, and rapid automatized naming (RAN) of visual stimuli. However, there has been considerable debate about the relative importance of these three abili-ties as predictors of reading development in other alpha-betic orthographies (see Caravolas et al., 2012, and Share, 2008). Some authors have argued that in consistent orthographies, phoneme awareness is a less important, and RAN a more important, predictor of variations in reading ability than in English (e.g., de Jong & van der Leij, 1999; Wimmer, Mayringer, & Landerl, 2000). In con-trast, recent large-scale cross-linguistic studies have sug-gested that the cognitive correlates of word-reading accuracy and speed are relatively similar across orthogra-phies differing in consistency (Vaessen et al., 2010; Ziegler et al., 2010). These studies reported only concur-rent associations, however, which makes any claims about causal processes ambiguous.

Recently, we conducted a short-term longitudinal study comparing beginning readers of English with begin-ning readers of three languages with highly consistent orthographies (Spanish, Slovak, and Czech; Caravolas

et al., 2012). Phonemic awareness, letter knowledge, and RAN (assessed prior to or at the start of formal schooling) were longitudinal predictors of reading skill in the middle of Grade 1 and had similar relative importance in all lan-guages. These results suggest that the cognitive skills driv-ing reading development might be universal across alphabetic orthographies. However, we estimated predic-tors of reading achievement only in the earliest phase of literacy development, when children are learning the alphabetic principle—that graphemes (letters) correspond more or less systematically to phonemes (sounds). In the current study, we explored, for the first time, the patterns of growth and the predictors of all components of growth in three languages over a 28-month period from kindergarten (reception year in England) until the end of Grade 2.

Several longitudinal growth-curve studies have assessed the cognitive skills that predict initial reading levels (intercepts) and rates of growth in reading (slopes) in different individual orthographies (Compton, 2000, 2003; Leppänen et al., 2004; Lervåg & Hulme, 2009; Nakamoto, Lindsey, & Manis, 2007; Torgesen et al., 1999). However, these studies provide few insights into possible differences in the predictors of growth in different orthog-raphies because they focused on different phases of chil-dren’s reading development using different measures of reading ability. We argue that, theoretically, if the core predictors of early reading achievement are the same across alphabetic orthographies (Caravolas et al., 2012; Melby-Lervåg, Lyster, & Hulme, 2012), then phoneme awareness, letter knowledge, and RAN should all predict early starting levels of reading (intercepts) in consistent orthographies as well as in English. Moreover, if the learning mechanisms that enable children to develop word-recognition skills are also universal, then the pre-dictors of rates of growth and acceleration of growth should also be similar across orthographies.

The Current Study

No study has reported a direct cross-linguistic compari-son of the patterns of growth during the early phases of reading development in consistent versus inconsistent orthographies. Here, we followed three language groups (English, Czech, and Spanish) from our earlier study (Caravolas et al., 2012) and assessed them six times between kindergarten and the end of second grade. All groups had comparable levels of initial reading ability (see Table 1). To ensure parity in the measures used across languages, we created a battery of parallel tests and administered an identical measure of reading at all time points. This design allowed us to ask two questions of considerable theoretical importance. First, do the rates and patterns of growth in early word reading differ as a function of orthographic consistency? Second, do the

Page 3: C1.M52_Cuadro_Caravolas+et+al%2C+2013%2C+Diff+patterns.pdf

1400 Caravolas et al.

predictors of individual variations in growth rate vary as a function of orthographic consistency?

Method

Participants

Groups of pupils were assessed six times (at intervals of roughly 6 months, at the middle and end of each school year) over 3 school years from reception year (England) and final kindergarten year (Czech Republic, Spain) until the end of second grade. The sample comprised 523 par-ticipants. The 185 English participants (97 boys, 88 girls) attended nine primary schools in cities throughout the North of England. The 150 Czech participants (74 boys, 76 girls), recruited from Prague and smaller cities in Bohemia, came from 20 kindergartens and then moved into 44 different primary schools in Grade 1. The 188 Spanish participants (103 boys, 85 girls) attended 5 kin-dergartens in the first year and 7 primary schools around Granada in the subsequent years of the study. For the whole sample, the percentages of missing children between Time 1 and later time points were 5.35 (Time 2), 5.16 (Time 3), 7.08 (Time 4), 10.13 (Time 5), and 13.96 (Time 6). All children were monolingual. At initial testing, English children had a mean age of 60.27 months (SD = 3.67, range = 53–67), Spanish children had a mean age of 66.72 months (SD = 3.66, range = 61–73), and Czech chil-dren had a mean age of 71.86 months (SD = 4.04, range = 64–85). The group differences in age reflect differences in the age of school entry across countries. Further details

regarding these groups’ general ability scores and the schooling practices in the three countries are reported in Caravolas et al. (2012).

Measures

Parallel batteries of tests were created in each language. At each time point, children were assessed on a battery of language and literacy tests administered in the same fixed order. Here, we describe only the measures with direct relevance to the present study, namely letter knowledge, phoneme awareness, and RAN, which were measured at Time 1 as predictors of growth, and the measure of read-ing ability, which was administered at each of the six time points. The descriptive statistics and reliability estimates for each measure are presented in Table 1 (full details of these tasks are given in Caravolas et al., 2012).

Letter knowledge (Time 1).  To assess letter knowl-edge, we asked children to pronounce the sounds and names of each letter of their alphabet. Letters were pre-sented separately in upper and lower case, and separate estimates of letter-sound and letter-name knowledge were derived by summing across both cases.

Phoneme awareness (Time 1).  We administered two different tasks to measure phoneme awareness: pho-neme isolation and phoneme blending. These tasks were parallel in design and content across languages, and we administered them individually on separate days. Pho-neme isolation required the child to pronounce in

Table 1.  Mean Scores and Reliabilities for All Variables at All Time Points for the Three Language Groups

English Spanish Czech

Measure M Reliability M Reliability M Reliability

Letter-sound knowledge: Time 1a 38.53 (17.00) α = .96 18.53 (16.65) α = .98 22.19 (18.55) α = .98Letter names knowledge: Time 1a 15.81 (14.90) α = .97 24.59 (17.33) α = .98 7.59 (9.40) α = .95Phoneme isolation: Time 1a 40.10 (17.18) α = .94 21.81 (21.41) α = .97 33.58 (20.14) α = .96Phoneme blending: Time 1a 5.55 (2.36) α = .75 2.27 (2.82) α = .89 2.19 (3.55) α = .94RAN for pictures: Time 1b 57.98 (16.41) r = .77 54.55 (12.12) r = .71 51.88 (11.40) r = .72RAN for colors: Time 1b 66.78 (21.40) r = .85 64.39 (20.75) r = .80 57.79 (17.18) r = .77Reading ability: Time 1c 7.08 (3.45) r = .60 7.54 (5.69) r = .80 7.75 (4.61) r = .75Reading ability: Time 2c 9.48 (4.36) r = .67 9.46 (6.68) r = .78 9.49 (4.87) r = .68Reading ability: Time 3c 13.05 (6.22) r = .78 14.27 (7.18) r = .71 14.60 (6.02) r = .65Reading ability: Time 4c 17.87 (7.94) r = .86 22.99 (6.35) r = .79 24.48 (6.76) r = .73Reading ability: Time 5c 22.08 (9.10) r = .83 28.26 (6.96) r = .72 31.72 (6.99) r = .67Reading ability: Time 6d 28.01 (9.85) r = .83 34.84 (7.93) r = .72 36.74 (8.47) r = .67

Note: Standard deviations are given in parentheses. The interval between each time of assessment was approximately 6 months.aReliability values for these measures are Cronbach’s αs. bReliability values for rapid automatized naming (RAN) are test-retest correlations between scores at this time point and at 4 months later. cReliability values for reading ability at Times 1 through 5 are correlations between scores at this and at the following time point. dReliability values for reading ability at Time 6 are correlations between reading at this and at the previous time point.

Page 4: C1.M52_Cuadro_Caravolas+et+al%2C+2013%2C+Diff+patterns.pdf

Literacy Development 1401

isolation the initial or final phoneme in 32 nonword items; phoneme blending required the child to blend aurally presented phonemic segments into high fre-quency, imageable words.

RAN (Time 1).  Parallel versions of an object-naming and a color-naming task were created such that, across languages, the depicted stimuli were identical and cor-responded to names of one or two syllables that were comparable in familiarity and frequency. In two trials, children were asked to name sequentially and as fast as they could five items repeated eight times over five lines of an A4 (8.3 in × 11 in.) card.

Picture-Word Matching Test (Times 1 through 6).  To assess reading ability, we administered a picture-word matching test. In this test, children read silently and selected from sets of four possible printed words the one that corresponded to an accompanying picture. A graded list of 52 (63 at Time 6) target words was created, which were cognates across all three languages and were equated as closely as possible across languages for grade level, frequency band, and syllable structure. Three dis-tractor words accompanied each target item: These con-sisted of one with a similar spelling, one with a similar meaning, and one unrelated word. The order of items was the same in all languages, and the order of the target and distractors was counterbalanced. The test was admin-istered in paper-and-pencil format to groups of three to five children for 3 min. Further details are provided in the Supplemental Material available online.

Results

The mean scores, standard deviations, and reliabilities for all variables at all time points are shown for all groups in Table 1. Analyses were done with Mplus 6.1 (Muthén & Muthén, 2011) using maximum-likelihood estimation. Extreme outliers (p < .001) in the reading scores at Time 1 were deleted from the data set. Extreme outliers (p < .001) in other variables were replaced by the next highest value (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). Missing values were handled with full-information maximum-likelihood estimation. Correlations among all variables at all time points for the English, Spanish, and Czech samples are shown in Tables S1, S2, and S3, respectively, in the Supplemental Material.

Differences in patterns of growth in reading across orthographies

To compare the growth of reading skills in the three orthographies, we constructed a three-group, uncondi-tional latent-growth model. For the consistent orthogra-phies, we expected slow growth before the initiation of formal reading instruction (Time 1–Time 2), a marked

acceleration immediately after formal reading instruction started (Time 2–Time 4), and a period of slower growth once basic alphabetic skills were acquired at the later time points (Time 4–Time 6). For the English group, which had just begun formal instruction at the start of our study, we expected relatively faster growth at the outset, followed by a steadier rate of growth. To model this pattern, we estimated a piecewise growth model in which the first part (Times 1–4) consisted of a quadratic model, and the second part consisted of a linear model. This model had four growth constructs; a simplified graphical description of it is shown in Figure 1. The first construct (intercept) represents the initial level of reading skills at Time 1. The second construct represents early linear growth (Slope 1: the instantaneous rate of linear growth at Time 1). The third construct (quadratic) represents the acceleration of growth between Times 1 and 4, and the fourth construct (Slope 2) represents the rate of linear growth between Times 4 through 6.

This model, in which the residuals of the observed variables were fixed to be equal but the four growth con-structs were freely estimated over the three groups, fitted the data very well, χ2(33, N = 523) = 45.52, p < .072; com-parative fit index (CFI) = .994; Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) = .992; root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = .047, 90% confidence interval (CI) = [.000, .077]; standard-ized root mean residual (SRMR) = .053. This model was then used to test for mean differences in each of the four growth constructs over the three orthographies.

The means and standard deviations for the final model, in which means that were not significantly different were fixed to be equal, are shown in Table 2. The correlations between the different growth constructs for each lan-guage group in this unconditional-growth model are shown in Table 3. Although children in all three orthog-raphies started out with equal reading skills, the growth patterns differed between the English group and the Spanish and Czech groups. The latter two groups showed faster growth in reading than English children did once formal reading instruction began. This final model fitted the data very well, χ2(37, N = 523) = 51.81, p < .054; CFI = .993; TLI = .991; RMSEA = .048, 90% CI = [.000, .077]; SRMR = .063; the estimated mean growth curves are plotted in Figure 2. Despite faster growth at the out-set, the English group showed steadier early growth dur-ing reception year and Grade 1 than the Spanish and Czech groups did; this was indicated by a significantly slower acceleration term. For the Spanish and Czech groups, the acceleration of early growth was pronounced and strongly negatively correlated with the early linear growth. Children who started with a steep linear growth did not accelerate as quickly later on compared with those who showed slower early growth (presumably because there is a limit to the maximal rate at which reading can grow in this period). After a growth spurt in

Page 5: C1.M52_Cuadro_Caravolas+et+al%2C+2013%2C+Diff+patterns.pdf

1402 Caravolas et al.

Grade 1, the children learning more consistent orthogra-phies resumed a linear pattern of growth in reading, albeit at a faster rate than their English peers.

Differences in the prediction of growth in reading across orthographies

We next examined whether letter knowledge, phoneme awareness, and RAN at Time 1 differed in influence as predictors of individual differences in the growth of reading skills across the three orthographies. To simplify the models, we constructed composite scores of the z- transformed variables (M = 0, SD = 1) of the three predic-tor constructs. We then constructed a three-group model in which the four growth constructs were predicted from

the three predictors. In addition, we let the lower-order growth constructs predict the higher-order growth con-structs. This was done so that the impact of the predictor variables on the later growth constructs would not be confounded by covariation between the predictor vari-ables and the earlier growth constructs (see Lervåg & Hulme, 2010, for a discussion). In the current model, ini-tial linear growth was negatively correlated with the acceleration of growth. Therefore, when we assessed whether a predictor was related to the acceleration of growth, we partialed out the effects of initial linear slope (a failure to do so would have resulted in a negative cor-relation between the predictor and acceleration, which would be an artifact of the negative correlation between the initial slope and the acceleration term). A path

Accelerationof Early Growth

Beginning Status Time 1

Early Linear Growth

Later Linear Growth

Picture-Word MatchingTime 1

Picture-Word MatchingTime 2

Picture-Word MatchingTime 3

Picture-Word MatchingTime 4

Picture-Word MatchingTime 5

Picture-Word MatchingTime 6

Fig. 1.  Unconditional three-group piecewise growth model of beginning reading skills. The rectangles refer to the observed reading variables at the six time points, and the ellipses refer to the latent growth constructs that underlie reading ability from Times 1 through 6. The gray arrows represent the factor loadings that are fixed in order to produce the four latent growth constructs: These constructs are initial level of reading skills (beginning status), the instantaneous rate of growth at Time 1 (early linear growth), the acceleration of growth between Times 1 and 4 (acceleration of early growth), and the linear growth between Times 4 and 6 (later lin-ear growth). The black arrows represent the covariances (correlations) between the latent growth constructs.

Table 2.  Means for the Unconditional-Growth Model

Variable English Spanish Czech Comparison of means

Initial status 7.28* (2.92**) 7.28** (5.41**) 7.28** (4.24**) English = Spanish, Czech; Spanish = CzechEarly growth 6.11** (5.88*) 2.64** (8.92**) 2.64** (6.66*) English > Spanish, Czech; Spanish = CzechAcceleration of early growth

1.22* (2.79) 6.94** (6.48**) 6.94** (5.29**) English < Spanish, Czech; Spanish = Czech

Later growth 9.58** (3.00) 11.65** (2.80) 13.13** (2.80) English < Spanish < Czech

Note: Standard deviations are given in parentheses. Mean values in boldface did not differ across groups and were hence fixed to be equal across groups; the mean values of the slopes (early and later growth) and the quadratic term (acceleration of early growth) indicate the growth and acceleration in year units.*p < .05. **p < .01.

Page 6: C1.M52_Cuadro_Caravolas+et+al%2C+2013%2C+Diff+patterns.pdf

Literacy Development 1403

diagram summarizing the model is shown in Figure 3. This model fitted the data well, χ2(51, N = 523) = 62.55, p < .129; CFI = .995; TLI = .991; RMSEA = .036, 90% CI = [.000, .064]; SRMR = .039, and was used as a baseline model in order to test the degree to which the impact of letter knowledge, phoneme awareness, and RAN on the four growth constructs differed between the three groups.

First, we found that there was a difference in the pre-dictive pattern among the three groups: A model in which the impact of letter knowledge, phoneme aware-ness, and RAN on the four growth constructs was con-strained to be equal over groups fitted the data significantly less well than a comparable model in which the regressions were freely estimated over groups, Δχ2(24) = 55.56, p = .001. Second, we found that there were no differences in the predictive pattern between the Spanish and the Czech groups: A model in which the impact of letter knowledge, phoneme awareness, and RAN on the four growth constructs was constrained to be equal between the Spanish and the Czech group had a

similar fit to the model in which the regressions were freely estimated over groups, Δχ2(15) = 15.71, p = .205.

Further testing of models in a hierarchical fashion showed that only one predictor differed in its influence on growth across groups: Letter knowledge was a stron-ger predictor of initial reading skills in English than in Spanish and Czech. A further model was tested in which all the regressions from the three predictors to the four growth constructs were constrained to be equal over the three groups, except the regression from letter knowl-edge to initial status in reading (which was freely esti-mated for the English group only). This model did not differ from the model in which all regressions were freely estimated over all groups, Δχ2(23) = 24.76, p = .363.

Thus, the role of letter knowledge, phoneme aware-ness, and RAN as predictors of beginning reading skills was similar in all three orthographies; the exception was letter knowledge, which was a less important concurrent predictor at the first time point in English than in Spanish and Czech. This final model fitted the data very well, χ2(74, N = 523) = 87.31, p < .138; CFI = .995; TLI = .993; RMSEA = .032, 90% CI = [.000, .057]; SRMR = .049; the unstandardized and standardized regression coefficients for this model can be found in Table 4. As can be seen, all three predictors explained variations in initial reading skills. Furthermore, letter knowledge and phoneme awareness both explained variations in the growth of reading skills, but only RAN explained variations in the acceleration of growth in reading skills. None of the pre-dictors explained variations in the later growth in read-ing, which, together with the nonsignificant variance in the later growth factor, suggest that once a child’s early reading system is set up, there is a high degree of longi-tudinal stability in the rate of later growth.1

Discussion

This is the first cross-linguistic study to assess reading growth trajectories in children learning to read English and more consistent orthographies (Spanish and Czech). The English children began formal reading instruction

Table 3.  Correlations Between Growth Constructs in the Unconditional-Growth Model for the Three Language Groups

English Spanish Czech

Variable 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4

1. Initial status — .06 .07 .17 — .17 −.34* −.06 — −.21 .23 −.292. Early growth — −.56* .41 — −.91** −.33 — −.90** .303. Acceleration of early growth

— −.22 — .37 — .16

4. Later growth — — —

*p < .05. **p < .01.

0 2 4 6 80

5

10

15

20

25

30

35

English Group

Czech GroupSpanish Group

10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24 26 28 30Month

Pict

ure-

Wor

d-M

atch

ing

Scor

e

Fig. 2.  Results from the unconditional three-group piecewise growth model: estimated reading ability (measured by scores on the picture-word matching test) as a function of time.

Page 7: C1.M52_Cuadro_Caravolas+et+al%2C+2013%2C+Diff+patterns.pdf

1404 Caravolas et al.

around Time 1 (Month 0 in Fig. 2), whereas the Spanish and Czech children only began formal reading instruction between Time 2 and Time 3 (Month 8). Strikingly, in this first 8-month period, when only the English children were being taught to read, the rates of growth in reading were essentially identical in the three orthographies. However, once the Spanish and Czech children started formal read-ing instruction (around Month 8), they showed a steep increase in growth followed by a subsequent decelera-tion. In short, we saw clear evidence that learning to read is more difficult in English than in the two more consis-tent orthographies. For both consistent orthographies, as soon as reading instruction begins, there is a rapid growth spurt followed by deceleration once children have “cracked the code.” In English, children show slower rates of growth in reading during their first 8 months or so of reading instruction, and their reading skills continue to develop at a relatively slow and steady rate over an extended period of time. This pattern confirms the view (Seymour et al., 2003; Share, 2008) that learning to read in English is more difficult than in more consistent orthographies.

Despite enduring group differences in the rate of growth, a common finding in all three languages was the absence of significant variation in growth within groups

between Times 2 and 3. This is consistent with the sugges-tion that once a child’s early reading system is set up, there is a high degree of longitudinal stability (e.g., Lervåg, Bråten, & Hulme, 2009; Parrila et al., 2005): If a child was a good reader or a poor reader by the end of Grade 1 (16 months into our study), he or she was likely to remain so.

The other critical issue addressed here was whether the same cognitive factors predict the growth of reading skills in English as in other more consistent orthographies. Until recently, a dominant claim has been that reading development in consistent orthographies depends on a rather different mix of underlying skills than it does in inconsistent orthographies, such as English. According to this argument, phoneme awareness plays only a weak role (e.g., Georgiou, Parrila, & Papadopoulos, 2008) or a transient role (e.g., de Jong & van der Leij, 1999) as a pre-dictor of reading development in consistent orthogra-phies, whereas it is a reliable long-term predictor in English. In contrast, RAN has been argued to be a strong predictor of reading development over a protracted devel-opmental period in consistent orthographies (e.g., Wimmer et al., 2000).

The design of the current study allowed for a fine-grained analysis of how critical cognitive skills (pho-neme awareness, letter knowledge, and RAN) relate to

Acceleration of Early Growth

Beginning Status Time 1

Early Linear Growth

Later Linear Growth

Picture-Word Matching

Time 1

Picture-Word Matching

Time 2

Picture-Word Matching

Time 3

Picture-Word Matching

Time 4

Picture-Word Matching

Time 5

Picture-Word Matching

Time 6

Phoneme Awareness

Time 1

RAN Time 1

Letter Knowledge

Time 1

Fig. 3.  Conditional three-group piecewise growth model: the growth of beginning reading skills as predicted from letter knowledge, phoneme awareness, and rapid automatized naming (RAN). The rectangles represent observed variables, and the ellipses represent the latent growth constructs underlying development in reading between Times 1 through 6. The gray arrows from the latent growth constructs to the observed reading scores represent the factor loadings that were fixed in order to produce the four growth constructs. The black arrows from Time 1 letter knowledge, phoneme awareness, and RAN to the latent growth factors represent the fact that the growth constructs are predicted from those three variables plus earlier growth constructs.

Page 8: C1.M52_Cuadro_Caravolas+et+al%2C+2013%2C+Diff+patterns.pdf

Literacy Development 1405

the different aspects of the growth of reading skills. We found that, with one exception, the patterns of predic-tion did not differ in strength across the three languages. Phoneme awareness, letter knowledge, and RAN all pre-dicted individual differences in initial levels of reading ability. The impact of phoneme awareness and RAN did not differ across the three languages, but letter knowl-edge was a weaker predictor of initial reading levels in English than in the two consistent orthographies. This last effect may relate to the impact of orthographic con-sistency; whereas good letter knowledge allows for accurate decoding in phonologically consistent orthog-raphies, in which letters correspond to sounds in highly predictable ways, this is less true in an inconsistent orthography like English.

Looking at predictors of the rate of reading growth between Time 1 and Time 4 (the first 16 months of our study), variations in early growth were predicted by pho-neme awareness and letter knowledge to similar degrees across all three languages, whereas RAN predicted the rate of acceleration of growth in this period—also to a similar degree. Thus, whereas phoneme awareness and letter knowledge were associated with the very early growth of reading skills, RAN was associated with how quickly this growth rate accelerated during the first 16 months. This result aligns well with earlier studies that have found RAN to predict reading for a more pro-tracted period than phoneme awareness and letter knowledge (e.g., Landerl & Wimmer, 2008; Lervåg et al., 2009).

Table 4.  Results From the Model Predicting the Growth of Reading Skills From Letter Knowledge, Phoneme Awareness, and Rapid Automatized Naming at Time 1

Dependent growth construct

Predictor Initial status Early growthAcceleration of early growth Later growth

Letter knowledge    English 0.82* (0.21**) 3.56** (0.45**) −0.25 (−0.08) 0.98 (0.29)  Spanish 2.81** (0.58**) 3.56** (0.43**) −0.25 (−0.04) 0.98 (0.43)  Czech 2.81** (0.58**) 3.56** (0.54**) −0.25 (−0.05) 0.98 (0.41)Phoneme awareness    English 0.68** (0.17**) 2.76** (0.33**) −0.26 (−0.08) −0.02 (−0.01)  Spanish 0.68** (0.13**) 2.76** (0.30**) −0.26 (−0.04) −0.02 (−0.01)  Czech 0.68** (0.16**) 2.76** (0.49**) −0.26 (−0.06) −0.02 (−0.01)Rapid automatized naming    English −0.78** (−0.29**) −0.61 (−0.11) −0.72** (−0.32*) −0.18 (−0.08)  Spanish −0.78** (−0.14**) −0.61 (−0.07) −0.72** (−0.11**) −0.18 (−0.07)  Czech −0.78** (−0.15**) −0.61 (−0.09) −0.72** (−0.14**) −0.18 (−0.07)Initial status    English — −0.84 (−0.41) 0.08 (0.09) −0.01 (−0.01)  Spanish — −0.56 (−0.33) −0.21** (−0.18**) −0.05 (−0.11)  Czech — −1.37** (−1.01**) 0.04 (0.04) −0.36 (−0.72)Early growth    English — — −0.25* (−0.62**) 0.16 (0.38)  Spanish — — −0.62** (−0.87**) 0.12 (0.44)  Czech — — −0.62** (−0.84**) 0.35 (0.95)Acceleration of early growth    English — — — 0.28 (0.27)  Spanish — — — 0.40 (1.03)  Czech — — — 0.63 (1.25)R2    English .29** .44** .50* .28  Spanish .53** .27** .88** .32  Czech .63** .45* .77** .54

Note: Values outside parentheses (with the exception of the R2s) are unstandardized regression coefficients; values inside parentheses are standardized. Unstandardized regression coefficients in bold did not differ across groups and were hence fixed to be equal across groups.*p < .05. **p < .01.

Page 9: C1.M52_Cuadro_Caravolas+et+al%2C+2013%2C+Diff+patterns.pdf

1406 Caravolas et al.

Finally, none of our predictors (phoneme awareness, letter knowledge, and RAN) predicted variations in the rate of reading growth between Time 4 and Time 6 (the final 12 months of our study). This, along with the absence of significant variation in growth in later devel-opment, points to a high degree of longitudinal stability in typical reading development across languages. Further studies are needed to investigate the possibility that there may be subgroups of children with distinctive reading-growth profiles (cf. Parrila et al., 2005), which may differ between orthographies. Overall, however, our findings suggest an essentially universal pattern of prediction of growth in reading in all three languages studied (at least in the measure of silent word reading used here, which it should be noted correlates highly with reading aloud).

In summary, children learn to read in English (a pho-nologically inconsistent orthography) more slowly than in more consistent orthographies. However, learning to read in all alphabetic orthographies studied so far appears to depend heavily on individual differences in three core cognitive skills: phoneme awareness, letter knowledge, and RAN. The similar patterns of prediction from these three measures across languages suggest that the same mechanisms are involved in learning to read in any alphabetic orthography.

Author Contributions

M. Caravolas and C. Hulme developed the study concept. M. Caravolas, S. Defior, and G. S. Málková oversaw linguistic adap-tations of test materials, collection, and processing of English, Spanish, and Czech data. A. Lervåg performed the data analysis and modeling. M. Caravolas, A. Lervåg, and C. Hulme drafted the manuscript, and C. Hulme provided critical revisions to the overall manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to research fellows of the Enhancing Literary Development in European Languages network, Petroula Mousikou, Naymé Salas, Eduardo Onochie-Quintanilla, and Miroslav Litavský for their contributions to this study.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.

Funding

The research leading to this study was supported by funding from the European Community’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013) under Grant Agreement No. ELDEL PITN-GA- 2008-215961-2 to Markéta Caravolas.

Supplemental Material

Additional supporting information may be found at http://pss .sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data

Note

1. An equivalent model controlling for the possible confound-ing effects of age was also run. The patterns of prediction from letter knowledge, phoneme awareness, and RAN remained unchanged, and age was not a significant predictor of reading growth for any language group.

References

Caravolas, M., Lervåg, A., Mousikou, P., Efrim, C., Litavský, M., Onochie-Quintanilla, E., . . . Hulme, C. (2012). Common patterns of prediction of literacy development in different alphabetic orthographies. Psychological Science, 23, 678–686. doi:10.1177/0956797611434536

Compton, D. L. (2000). Modeling the growth of decoding skills in first-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4, 219–285.

Compton, D. L. (2003). Modeling the relationship between growth in rapid naming speed and growth in decoding skill in first-grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 225–239. doi:10.1207/S1532799XSSR0403_3

de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (1999). Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 450–476.

Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2008). Predictors of word decoding and reading fluency across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 566–580. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.100.3.566

Hill, C., Bloom, H., Rebeck Black, A., & Lipsey, M. (2008). Empirical benchmarks for interpreting effect sizes in research. Child Development Perspectives, 2, 172–177.

Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word read-ing fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: An 8-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 150–161. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.1001.150

Leppänen, U., Niemi, P., Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2004). Development of reading skills among preschool and pri-mary school pupils. Reading Research Quarterly, 39, 72–93.

Lervåg, A., Bråten, I., & Hulme, C. (2009). The cognitive and linguistic foundations of early reading development: A Norwegian latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 45, 764–781. doi:10.1037/a0014132

Lervåg, A., & Hulme, C. (2009). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) taps a mechanism that places constraints on the develop-ment of early reading fluency. Psychological Science, 20, 1040–1048. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02405.x

Lervåg, A., & Hulme, C. (2010). Predicting the growth of early spelling skills: Are there heterogeneous developmental trajectories? Scientific Studies of Reading, 14, 485–513. doi:10.1080/10888431003623488

Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S.-A. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Pho-nological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 322–352. doi: 10.1037/a0026744

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2011). Mplus (Version 6.1). Los Angeles, CA: Author.

Nakamoto, J., Lindsey, K., & Manis, F. (2007). A longitudinal analysis of English language learners’ word decoding and

Page 10: C1.M52_Cuadro_Caravolas+et+al%2C+2013%2C+Diff+patterns.pdf

Literacy Development 1407

reading comprehension. Reading and Writing, 20, 691–719. doi:10.1007/s11145-006-9045-7

Parrila, R., Aunola, K., Leskinen, E., Nurmi, J., & Kirby, J. (2005). Development of individual differences in reading: Results from longitudinal studies in English and Finnish. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 299–319. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.97.3.299

Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174.

Share, D. L. (2008). On the anglocentricities of current read-ing research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584–615. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584

Skibbe, L. E., Grimm, K. E., Bowles, R. P., & Morrison, F. J. (2012). Literacy growth in the academic year versus sum-mer from preschool through second grade: Differential effects of schooling across four skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 141–165. doi:10.1080/10888438.2010.543446

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate analysis. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Torgesen, J., Wagner, R., Rashotte, C., Rose, E., Lindamood, P., Conway, T., & Garvan, C. (1999). Preventing reading failure in young children with phonological processing disabilities:

Group and individual responses to instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 579–593.

Vaessen, A., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Faisca, L., Reis, A., & Blomert, L. (2010). Cognitive development of fluent word reading does not qualitatively differ between trans-parent and opaque orthographies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 827–842. doi:10.1037/a0019465

Verhoeven, L., & van Leeuwe, J. (2011). Role of gender and lin-guistic diversity in word decoding development. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 359–367. doi:10.1016/j.lin-dif.2011.02.004

Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (2000). The double-deficit hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a reg-ular orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 668–680. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.668

Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Reis, A., Faísca, L., . . . Blomert, L. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross- language investigation. Psychological Science, 21, 551–559. doi:10.1177/0956797610363406

Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, devel-opmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3