13
BUCLD 35 Proceedings To be published in 2011 by Cascadilla Press Rights forms signed by all authors Intonation of Early Answers to Confirmation-Seeking Questions in Spontaneous Speech Ana Isabel Mata and Ana Lúcia Santos 1. Introduction 1 In this paper, we present exploratory research on intonation in early spontaneous dialogues in European Portuguese (EP), focusing on intonation patterns in children’s affirmative answers. We explore possible correlations between intonation in children’s answers and the intonation pattern and the pragmatic value of adult’s questions, which will allow us to understand whether the choice of an intonation pattern in the answer is determined by an early sensitivity to the pragmatic value of the question, which is partly encoded by the intonation of the question. This will add to our knowledge of early intonation development. Previous research on intonation in spontaneous dialogues has already looked at the relation between discourse function and intonation in adult’s short answers. In particular, it has been shown that intonation can distinguish answers according to function (acknowledgments/ agreements, backchannels) - see Curl & Bell (2001) for yeah “agreements” and yeah “backchannels”; Benus et al. (2007) for “backchannels” and agreement short answers. On the other hand, previous research has also shown the existence of “pitch concord” mechanisms in dialogue. These mechanisms result in “relative pitch height” matching in dialogue and are known to characterize a cooperative behavior in adult-adult dialogues (see Brazil, 1985; Couper-Kuhlen & Selting, 1996; Wichmann, 2000; Wennerstrom, 2001; Heldner, Edlund & Hirschberg, 2010 show that pitch in backchannels adapts to the pitch in the interlocutor’s utterance). This means that the adult intonation of short answers may be expected to be affected by at least two factors: (i) the pragmatic function of the answer, which is related to the pragmatic function of the question and (ii) the pitch height in the preceding utterance. In our research, we investigate to what extent children’s short answers are also affected by these two factors. Previous literature on early intonational development has shown that early stages of language development are characterized by a high proportion of falling contours (see Snow, 2006 and references therein and Chen & Fikkert, 2007, * Ana Isabel Mata ([email protected] ), Ana Lúcia Santos ([email protected] ), FLUL/CLUL, University of Lisbon, Portugal. This work was partially funded by FCT project PTDC/LIN/66202/2006.

BUCLD 35 Proceedings To be published in 2011 by Cascadilla

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    3

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: BUCLD 35 Proceedings To be published in 2011 by Cascadilla

BUCLD 35 Proceedings

To be published in 2011 by Cascadilla Press

Rights forms signed by all authors

Intonation of Early Answers to Confirmation-Seeking

Questions in Spontaneous Speech

Ana Isabel Mata and Ana Lúcia Santos

1. Introduction1

In this paper, we present exploratory research on intonation in early

spontaneous dialogues in European Portuguese (EP), focusing on intonation

patterns in children’s affirmative answers. We explore possible correlations

between intonation in children’s answers and the intonation pattern and the

pragmatic value of adult’s questions, which will allow us to understand whether

the choice of an intonation pattern in the answer is determined by an early

sensitivity to the pragmatic value of the question, which is partly encoded by the

intonation of the question. This will add to our knowledge of early intonation

development.

Previous research on intonation in spontaneous dialogues has already

looked at the relation between discourse function and intonation in adult’s short

answers. In particular, it has been shown that intonation can distinguish answers

according to function (acknowledgments/ agreements, backchannels) - see Curl

& Bell (2001) for yeah “agreements” and yeah “backchannels”; Benus et al.

(2007) for “backchannels” and agreement short answers.

On the other hand, previous research has also shown the existence of “pitch

concord” mechanisms in dialogue. These mechanisms result in “relative pitch

height” matching in dialogue and are known to characterize a cooperative

behavior in adult-adult dialogues (see Brazil, 1985; Couper-Kuhlen & Selting,

1996; Wichmann, 2000; Wennerstrom, 2001; Heldner, Edlund & Hirschberg,

2010 show that pitch in backchannels adapts to the pitch in the interlocutor’s

utterance).

This means that the adult intonation of short answers may be expected to be

affected by at least two factors: (i) the pragmatic function of the answer, which

is related to the pragmatic function of the question and (ii) the pitch height in the

preceding utterance. In our research, we investigate to what extent children’s

short answers are also affected by these two factors.

Previous literature on early intonational development has shown that early

stages of language development are characterized by a high proportion of falling

contours (see Snow, 2006 and references therein and Chen & Fikkert, 2007,

* Ana Isabel Mata ([email protected]), Ana Lúcia Santos ([email protected]), FLUL/CLUL,

University of Lisbon, Portugal. This work was partially funded by FCT project

PTDC/LIN/66202/2006.

Page 2: BUCLD 35 Proceedings To be published in 2011 by Cascadilla

a.o.). On the other hand, recent work has suggested that the adult inventory of

nuclear accents and boundary tones and their F0 alignment patterns are mastered

very early, in certain cases before or around 2;0 – this has been suggested by

Frota & Vigário (2008) for Portuguese, Vanrell et al. (2010) for Spanish and

Catalan, a.o., although Chen & Fikkert (2007) do not confirm for Dutch a very

early acquisition of the adult tonal inventory.

As far as F0 scaling is concerned, Frota & Vigário (2008) suggest that it is

still under development at 2;0; Snow (2006) shows that accent F0 range

develops at about 18 months. Given these two types of results, it is unclear

whether we should expect pitch modification effects or effects of pragmatic

function in the intonation of children’s short answers. However, if these effects

are found, we expect age differences, namely a difference between <2;0 and

>2;0.

In the next section, we report previous results on the intonation of

confirmation seeking yes-no questions in child-directed speech. In section 3 we

describe the data used for the present study and in section 4 we report results on

the intonation of children’s answers to those confirmation-seeking questions,

pointing out that after 2;0 answers start to show convergence with the intonation

pattern of declaratives in EP and correlation with subtypes of questions’

function. Finally, in section 5 conclusions are presented.

2. Previous work on the intonation of confirmation-seeking questions

In a previous study (Mata & Santos, 2010), we analyzed 301 confirmation-

seeking yes-no questions uttered by three adults and answered by two children

during 28h of spontaneous speech (a subset of the corpus of Santos, 2006/2009).

These 301 questions correspond to all the confirmation-seeking yes-no

questions1 uttered by the adults, that were given an affirmative answer by

children and that were suitable for an acoustic analysis.

The 301 questions were coded according to three levels of action that

correspond to the problem justifying the confirmation request (see Clark 1996,

and also Rodríguez & Schlangen 2004, Venditti, Hirschberg & Liscombe 2006):

contact, perception and understanding. If the confirmation request is related to

‘understanding’, the question is related to the meaning of what was previously

uttered; if the confirmation request is related to ‘perception’, it is an attempt to

confirm what was pronounced. On the contrary, contact questions do not result

from a real communication problem: the speaker asks for a confirmation only

seeking to maintain the ongoing interaction.

The analysis of the different sets of confirmation-seeking questions shows

that pragmatic/ discourse subtypes of questions affect nuclear pitch accent

distribution and F0 height:

1 The definition of confirmation-seeking questions was assumed to be according to

Prévot (2004).

Page 3: BUCLD 35 Proceedings To be published in 2011 by Cascadilla

(i) H* and L+H* mainly occur in perception related confirmation-seeking

questions;

(ii) L*+H mainly occurs in understanding related confirmation-seeking

questions;

(iii) In general, perception-seeking yes-no questions are uttered in a higher

register than understanding-seeking questions (higher F0 for both nuclear pitch

accent and final boundary tone).

Confirmation-seeking questions related to ‘understanding’ were also coded

according to level of acceptance (neutral / non-neutral), another level of action

(Clark, 1996). Non-neutral acceptance includes the suggestion of incorrectness,

disbelief or surprise. Results have shown that questions expressing different

levels of acceptance correlate with different pitch accents: H+L* (the most

commonly used nuclear accent in information-seeking questions in EP) mainly

occurs in neutral questions; in non-neutral confirmation-seeking questions we

mainly find L*+H and ^H*. It is important to notice that these pitch accents

occurring in understanding questions coded as non-neutral may both be

associated with the notion of Contrast (see Mata & Santos, 2010 for discussion):

L*+H has been associated with narrow / contrastive focus in questions by Frota

(1998/2000, 2002); ^H* was associated with specification or correction of given

information by Viana et al. (2007).

In the present paper, we examine the intonation patterns of children’s

affirmative answers to these different subtypes of confirmation requests uttered

by adults (understanding, including neutral vs. non-neutral acceptance,

perception, contact).

3. Data

For the present study, we use the same corpus of child-directed speech that

was examined for the study of confirmation-seeking questions: 232 question-

answer pairs were analyzed (<2;0 – 89; ≥ 2;0 – 143), corresponding to 77,1% of

the total amount of 301 questions analyzed in Mata & Santos (2010). 69

answers were excluded (31 to understanding-seeking yes-no questions, 26 to

perception-seeking and 12 to contact-seeking ones) from the 301 question-

answer pairs that we had selected initially, due to background noise or speech

overlapping.

Table 1. Number of answers per child < 2;0 and ≥≥≥≥ 2;0

Child Age MLUw Answers < 2;0 Answers ≥≥≥≥ 2;0

INM 1;7.6 - 2;6.19 1.285 - 2.265 68 80

TOM 1;7.14 - 2;8.9 1.272 - 2.979 21 63

All answers are grammatical affirmative answers and correspond to one of

the following patterns:

- sim ‘yes’;

Page 4: BUCLD 35 Proceedings To be published in 2011 by Cascadilla

- é ‘is’ / foi ‘was’ – a frozen form of the verb ser ‘to be’;

- main verb or auxiliary in a VP ellipsis structure (Santos, 2006/2009).

94% of these (one-word) short answers correspond to one syllable; the

remaining 6% present two syllables.

Intonation coding and F0 measurements. The 232 question-answer pairs

were analyzed using the WaveSurfer 1.8.5 speech analysis package

(http://www.speech.kth.se/wavesurfer/). The intonation coding of the corpus was

done manually, using the system Towards a P_ToBI by Viana et al. 2007 (see

http://www.ling.ohio-state.edu/~tobi/). 7 out of the 8 pitch accents (H+L*,

H*+L, L*+H, L+H*, H*, L*, ^H*) and all the final boundary tones (L%, H%,

!H%, LH%, HL%) that are covered in that proposal were used in this task (see

the schematic F0 contours in figure 1).

Pitch accent type

H+L* H*+L L*+H L+H* H* L* ^H* (…)

Boundary Tones

L% H% !H% LH% HL%Towards a P_ToBI

Pitch accent type

H+L* H*+L L*+H L+H* H* L* ^H* (…)

Boundary Tones

L% H% !H% LH% HL%Towards a P_ToBI

Figure 1: Schematic F0 contours for the pitch accents and boundary

tones used in the transcription of question-answer pairs, following Towards a P_ToBI. (Lines ▬ indicate the stressed syllable.)

For each question and answer, F0 values corresponding to high and low

targets within nuclear accents and final boundary tones were measured and

converted to semitones (ST). In the case of pitch accents, the maximum value of

F0 (H) and the minimum value of F0 (L) were measured inside (or in the

immediate vicinity) of the stressed syllable of the accented word. In the case of

boundary tones, identical F0 data (maxima and minima) was extracted at the

rightmost edge of questions and answers. F0 range (the difference between F0

maximum and F0 minimum values) was also extracted for both pitch accents

and final boundary tones.

4. Results

4.1. Intonation of children’s answers to confirmation-seeking questions

The analysis of 232 children’s answers to confirmation-seeking yes-no

questions shows that from 2;0 onwards there is a general increase of low /

Page 5: BUCLD 35 Proceedings To be published in 2011 by Cascadilla

falling accents (χ2 (1)= 11.946, p=.001). Whereas before 2;0 there is

approximately 50% cases of high / rising - (L+)H*, L*+H - and low / falling

pitch accents - (H+)L* -, after 2;0 about 70% of the answers present low /

falling pitch accents (see table 2).

Table 2. Distribution of pitch accent types across affirmative answers

per subtype of confirmation request and age group Children’s

affirmative answers

Pitch Accents

Subtypes of

confirmation

requests uttered

by adults

Age

Group

High / Rising

(L+)H*, L*+H

Low / Falling

(H+)L*

N

(%)

Understanding

< 2 years

≥ 2 years

22

21

24

38

105

(45.3)

Perception

< 2 years

≥ 2 years

22

18

13

44

97

(41.8)

Contact

< 2 years

≥ 2 years

3

4

5

18

30

(12.9)

N

(%)

< 2 years

≥ 2 years

47 (52.8)

43 (30.1)

42 (47.2)

100 (69.9)

232

(100)

We also find in answers uttered after 2;0 a general increase of falling final

boundary tones (L%) – this increase is particularly visible in answers to

perception and contact questions (see figure 2). The general increase of low /

falling pitch accents, together with the global increase of falling boundary tones

results in a clear association of these answers with a falling nuclear contour

from 2;0 onwards.

Final Boundary Tone

30,425,4

48,6

24,2

50

22,7

69,674,6

51,4

75,8

50

77,3

0

20

40

60

80

100

< 2 => 2 < 2 => 2 < 2 => 2

Understanding Perception Contact

% non-falling

falling

Figure 2: Percentage of falling/non-falling boundaries across

affirmative answers per subtype of confirmation request and age group

Page 6: BUCLD 35 Proceedings To be published in 2011 by Cascadilla

Many studies (see Snow, 2006 and references therein; Chen & Fikkert,

2007, a.o.) have suggested that there is a general tendency, in first stages of

acquisition, to produce falling contours. However, the age effect that we have

found in our data excludes this type of explanation. We rather interpret the

increase of falling contours in affirmative answers as the result of a convergence

with the intonation expected in declaratives: H+L* L% is the canonical contour

of declaratives in EP.

Nevertheless, the results in table 2 also call for an analysis of answers by

subtypes of questions’ pragmatic values (function). Looking at particular

subcategories, we can see that answers to perception related confirmation

requests are more likely to present low / falling pitch accents after 2;0 years-old

(χ2 (1)= 10.563, p=.001). Answers to contact questions always define

themselves predominantly as low / falling in both age groups, although the

proportion of low/falling raises after 2;0. Finally, in the case of answers to

understanding related confirmation requests, although there is an increase of low

/ falling pitch accents after age 2;0, the difference between the two age groups is

not significant. This requires further inquiry into understanding related

questions.

4.2. Intonation cues in the answers to (non-)neutral questions

Pitch accents. To understand the different behavior in answers to

understanding related confirmation questions, we must recall that these

questions are not all equivalent, instead we find in this group two pragmatic

subtypes: neutral and non-neutral acceptance. According to our previous study

(see section 2), non-neutral understanding confirmation requests associate with

particular pitch accents: L*+H, ^H*; neutral ones frequently take H+L*, the

most commonly used pitch accent in EP yes-no questions. When we look at

answers to understanding confirmation questions taking into account the

acceptance level encoded in the question, results start speaking by themselves.

In fact, whereas before 2;0 we do not find a clear tendency favoring high / rising

or low / falling accents in answers to either neutral or non-neutral questions,

after 2;0 only answers to neutral questions converge with the general tendency

to present low / falling accents (see table 3). This means that, after 2;0, it is

more likely for an answer to a non-neutral question than for an answer to a

neutral one to associate with a high / rising accent. The distribution differences

of high / rising and low / falling accents across answers to neutral and non-

neutral questions are statistically significant after 2;0 (χ2 (1)= 4.664, p<.05); for

ages < 2;0 no correlation was found.

F0 height. An analysis of F0 height differences between answers to neutral

and non-neutral understanding questions shows that F0 maxima of accents is

affected: Max(imum)_P(itch) A(ccent) is higher for answers to non-neutral than

for answers to neutral questions from 2;0 onwards (Mann-Whitney Test, U=

Page 7: BUCLD 35 Proceedings To be published in 2011 by Cascadilla

272.50, p<.05), although not before 2;02. Moreover, as figure 3 shows, when

answers are broken down by contour type, falling contours associated with non-

neutral acceptance are higher than falling contours associated with neutral

acceptance.

Table 3. Distribution of pitch accent types across affirmative answers to

understanding related confirmation requests by acceptance level and age group

Children’s

affirmative answers

Pitch Accents

Subtypes of

confirmation requests

uttered by adults

Age

Group

High / Rising

(L+)H*, L*+H

Low / Falling

(H+)L*

N

(%)

Understanding

neutral acceptance

< 2 years

≥ 2 years

13

6

17

22

58

(55.2)

Understanding

non-neutral acceptance

< 2 years

≥ 2 years

9

15

7

16

47

(44.8)

N

(%)

< 2 years

≥ 2 years

22 (47.8)

21 (35.6)

24 (52.2)

38 (64.4)

105

(100)

Pitch accents - F0 means at Max_PA for answers=>2;0

0

1

2

3

4

5

Neutral

acceptance

Non-neutral

acceptance

ST

Falling contours

Non-falling contours

Figure 3: F0 means on pitch accents across answers to (non-)neutral

questions by contour type ≥≥≥≥2;0

These results suggest that after age 2;0 children are using two answer

strategies: answers to a non-neutral understanding-seeking question may be

signaled either by a non-falling nuclear contour or by a falling nuclear contour

which starts higher than one associated with an answer to a neutral question.

2 An analysis of Maximum_Contour produced similar results ≥2;0 (Mann-Whitney Test,

U= 271.50, p<.05). As for F0 range of PA (F0max-F0min PA) in answers ≥2;0, answers

to neutral present lower range than answers to non-neutral, although the difference is not

statistically significant.

Page 8: BUCLD 35 Proceedings To be published in 2011 by Cascadilla

Examples below illustrate these answer strategies3. Intonation cues

signaling non-neutral questions were associated with Contrast by Mata & Santos

(2010). Apparently, around 2;0 the child starts to show some awareness of

prosodic encoding of Contrast.

Answers to non-neutral questions Answers to neutral questions

Falling contours

a. Child: INM b. Child: INM

100100

200

300

400

500

600

Fun

dam

enta

l fr

equen

cy (

Hz)

0

tem (‘has’)

H+L* L%

100100

200

300

400

500

600

Fu

nd

amen

tal

freq

uen

cy (

Hz)

0 0.5

sim (‘yes’)

H+L* L%

c. Child: TOM d. Child: TOM

100100

200

300

400

500

600

Fu

nd

amen

tal

freq

uen

cy (

Hz)

0 0.5

é (‘is’)

H* L%

100100

200

300

400

500

600

Fu

nd

amen

tal

freq

uen

cy (

Hz)

0 0.5

andam (‘walk’)

H* L%

Figure 4: Falling contours in answers to (non-)neutral understanding-

seeking questions by two children ≥≥≥≥2;0.

3 The figures were done using Praat and Pauline Welby’s scripts. (These scripts may

draw the F0 contour in unvoiced portions of the signal.)

Page 9: BUCLD 35 Proceedings To be published in 2011 by Cascadilla

Answers to non-neutral questions Answers to neutral questions

Non-falling contours

e. Child: TOM f. Child: TOM

100100

180

260

340

420

500

Fu

nd

amen

tal

freq

uen

cy (

Hz)

0 0.5

tenho (‘have’)

L*+H !H%

100100

180

260

340

420

500

Fun

dam

enta

l fr

equen

cy (

Hz)

0 0.5

tens (‘have’)

L*+H !H%

g. Child: INM h. Child: INM

100100

180

260

340

420

500

Fun

dam

enta

l fr

equen

cy (

Hz)

0

é (‘is’)

H* !H%

100100

180

260

340

420

500

Fun

dam

enta

l fr

equen

cy (

Hz)

0

é (‘is’)

H* !H%

Figure 5: Non-falling contours in answers to (non-)neutral

understanding-seeking questions by two children ≥≥≥≥2;0.

On the other hand, we wanted to know whether children’s answers show

pitch concord effects with respect to the question. In fact, there is a correlation

between F0 maxima of nuclear contours in questions and answers after age 2;0,

in the case of answers to understanding related questions. For answers with a

falling contour, the correlation is significant between Max(imum) P(itch)

A(ccent) (as well as Maximum_Contour) of the understanding confirmation

question and Max(imum) P(itch) A(ccent) of the answer (rs = .480, p (one-

tailed) < .0005). As for answers with a non-falling contour, the correlation is

significant between Max(imum) P(itch) A(ccent) of the understanding

confirmation question and Max(imum) B(oundary) T(one) of the answer (rs =

.467, p (one-tailed) < .05). Before age 2;0 no correlations were found.

These facts show that in both answer strategies mentioned above, the

beginning of a falling contour and the end of a non-falling one correlate with

Page 10: BUCLD 35 Proceedings To be published in 2011 by Cascadilla

nuclear F0 height of questions, suggesting that the child may use mechanisms of

pitch modification across turns.

Finally, we would like to show that the asymmetries in the distribution of

falling / non-falling contours according to age are not due to variability in the

input. We compared the number of occurrences of different pitch accent types in

each subtype of confirmation request addressed by adults to children

(understanding, perception, contact, neutral vs. non-neutral acceptance) before

and after 2;0 years-old: the analysis reveals no significant differences. This

means that, with regard to nuclear accents, a question addressed to a child

before age 2;0 is similar to a question directed to a child from age 2;0 onwards.

Moreover, pitch accents in children’s answers are not a simple copy of nuclear

accents in the question - no correlations were found between accent types in the

question and accent types in the answer. Considering understanding questions

expressing neutral and non-neutral acceptance, different nuclear pitch accents

associated with the different pragmatic values are produced either before 2;0 or

after 2;0 (Contrast between accent types High/Rising and Low/Falling: <2;0 χ2

(1)= 5.858, p<.05 ; ≥ 2;0 χ2 (1)= 14.930, p<.0001. Contrast between nuclear

accents L*+H, ^H*, H*, L+H* H*+L, L*, H+L*: <2;0 Fisher Exact Test p<.05 ;

≥ 2;0 Fisher Exact Test p<.0001.)

With regard to F0 height, an important effect of acceptance level is also

observed for understanding related confirmation questions (with non-falling

nuclear contours4): H targets of pitch accents have higher F0 in non-neutral

questions than in neutral ones; H targets of final boundaries have lower F0 in

non-neutral questions than in neutral ones. A comparison based on the age

group of the children to whom these questions were addressed shows that

Max(imum)_B(oundary) T(one) differences between neutral and non-neutral

questions are significant for both age groups (see figure 6b). However, as for

Max(imum)_P(itch) A(ccent), the difference between neutral and non-neutral

questions is significant after 2;0 (Mann-Whitney Test, U= 342.50, p<.05),

although not before 2;0 (see figure 6a).

a. b.

Pitch accents in non-falling contours - F0 means by

acceptance level

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

Neutral

acceptance

Non-neutral

acceptance

ST

Max_PA < 2

Max_PA => 2

Non-falling boundaries - F0 means by acceptance level

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

Neutral

acceptance

Non-neutral

acceptance

ST

Max_BT <2

Max_BT =>2

Figure 6: Nuclear F0 height (F0 maxima) across understanding-seeking

questions by acceptance level and age group

4 A non-falling boundary tone (LH%, H%, !H%) occurs in more than 75% of the

understanding questions that were analyzed and is predominant across all subtypes of

confirmation requests in our data.

Page 11: BUCLD 35 Proceedings To be published in 2011 by Cascadilla

It is known that child-directed speech may differ from speech directed to

adults in two main ways: by an exaggerated marking of contrasts (e.g. prosodic

contrasts) that exist in adults speech (see Fernald & Simon, 1984, for a recent

review, Payne et al., 2010, and also Kempe, Schaeffler & Thoresen, 2010) or by

avoiding the use of certain structures (e.g. syntactic structures) in the early

stages of the linguistic development of children (see Evers-Vermeul, 2005 for

Dutch and Costa et al., 2008 for Portuguese, based on this same corpus).

The above results for adult-child question-answer pairs pointed out that the

phonetic contrasts between neutral and non-neutral understanding questions

may be more strongly marked for questions addressed to older children,

suggesting that the use of mechanisms disambiguating the interpretation of these

subtypes of confirmation questions may increase with age.

5. Conclusions

Our aim in this paper was to examine whether the intonation patterns of

children’s verbal responses varies according to the discourse context and signals

early sensitivity to the pragmatic interpretation of the input.

Results described in the present work, together with those discussed in a

previous one (Mata & Santos, 2010), have shown that intonation cues (nuclear

accent types and F0 height of both nuclear accents and boundary tones) can

disambiguate the interpretation of confirmation-seeking yes-no questions in

child-directed speech in European Portuguese.

In this paper, we analyzed question-answer correlations, concentrating on

the intonation patterns of children’s one-word affirmative answers to

confirmation-seeking yes-no questions. The analysis of affirmative answers

indicates a convergence with the intonation pattern of declaratives in European

Portuguese from 2;0 onwards, although not before. The answers under analysis

before 2;0 suggest the existence of a fuzzy period (approximately 50% have

high / rising pitch accents)5 followed by convergence with the adult grammar.

Moreover, we have shown that after 2;0 intonation cues in children’s

answers vary according to the discourse / pragmatic context and correlate with

subtypes of questions’ function: low/falling pitch accents - (H+)L* - and

high/rising pitch accents - (L+)H*, L*+H -, as well as F0 height (F0 maxima

within pitch accents) of answers correlate with neutral/non-neutral acceptance in

“understanding” confirmation-questions.

Finally, after 2;0 there is evidence for a correlation between nuclear F0

height in questions and answers, suggesting that the child may use mechanisms

of pitch modification across turns.

5 A high variability was already reported for Portuguese-speaking children’s productions

before 2 years‐old with regard to other prosodic parameters, namely word stress (see

Correia, 2009).

Page 12: BUCLD 35 Proceedings To be published in 2011 by Cascadilla

This work may be seen as a first attempt to discover more about meaning

and form relations in the context of adult-child spontaneous dialogues, aiming at

a better understanding of intonation-discourse interactions in EP.

References

Balog, Heather L. 2010. A comparison of maternal and child intonation: Does adult input

support child production? Infant Behavior & Development 33: 337-345.

Benus, Stefan, Agustín Gravano and Julia Hirschberg. 2007. The prosody of

backchannels in American English, In Proceedings ICPhS XVI, Saarbrücken: 1065-

1068.

Brazil, David. 1985. Phonology: Intonation in Discourse. In Teun A. van Dijk, T. (ed.)

Handbook of Discourse Analysis II: Dimensions of Discourse. pp. 57-75. London:

Academic Press.

Chen, Aoju and Paula Fikkert. 2007. Intonation of early two-word utterances in Dutch. In

Proceedings ICPhS XVI, Saarbrücken: 315-320.

Clark, Herbert H. 1996. Using Language. Cambridge: CUP.

Correia, Susana. 2009. The Acquisition of Primary Word Stress in European Portuguese.

PhD Dissertation, University of Lisbon.

Costa, Ana Luísa, Nélia Alexandre, Ana Lúcia Santos and Nuno Soares. 2008. Efeitos de

modelização no input: o caso da aquisição de conectores. In Sónia Frota and Ana

Lúcia Santos (eds.) Textos seleccionados do XXIII Encontro da Associação

Portuguesa de Linguística, Lisboa: 131-142.

Couper-Kuhlen, Elizabeth and Margret Selting (eds.). 1996. Prosody in Conversation:

Interactional Studies. Cambridge: CUP.

Curl Traci S. and Alan Bell. 2001. Yeah, yeah, yeah: Prosodic differences of pragmatic

functions. University of Colorado.

Evers-Vermeul, Jacqueline. 2005. The Development of Dutch Connectives: Change and

Acquisition as Windows on Form-Function Relations. Utrecht: LOT.

Fernald, Anne and Thomas Simon. 1984. Expanded intonation contours in mothers'

speech to newborns. Developmental Psychology, 20(1), 104-113.

Frota, Sónia. 1998). Prosody and Focus in European Portuguese. PhD Dissertation,

University of Lisbon. [published 2000, Garland].

Frota, Sónia. 2002. Nuclear falls and rises in European Portuguese: A phonological

analysis of declarative and question intonation. Probus 14: 113-146.

Frota, Sónia and Marina Vigário. 2008. The intonation of one-word and first two-word

utterances in European Portuguese. Paper presented at the XI International

Conference for the Study of Child Language Conference (IASCL), Edinburgh.

Heldner, Mattias, Jens Edlund and Julia Hirschberg. 2010. Pitch similarity in the vicinity

of backchannels. In Proceedings InterSpeech 2010, Makuhari: 3054-3057.

Kempe, Vera, Sonja Schaeffler and John. C. Thoresen. 2010. Prosodic disambiguation in

child-directed speech. Journal of Memory and Language 62: 204-225.

Mata, Ana Isabel and Ana Lúcia Santos. 2010. On the Intonation of Confirmation

Seeking Requests in Child-Directed Speech. In Proceedings of Speech Prosody

2010, Chicago.

Payne, Elinor, Brechtje Post, Lluïsa Astruc, Pilar Prieto and Maria del Mar Vanrell.

2010. A cross-linguistic study of prosodic lengthening in child directed speech. In

Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2010. Chicago.

Page 13: BUCLD 35 Proceedings To be published in 2011 by Cascadilla

Prévot, Laurent. 2004. Structures sémantiques et pragmatiques pour la modélisation de

la cohérence dans des dialogues finalisés. Doctoral Dissertation. Université Paul

Sabatier, Toulouse III.

Rodríguez, Kepa Joseba and David Schlangen. 2004. Form, intonation and function of

clarification requests in German task-oriented spoken dialogues, SemDial 2004,

Barcelona.

Santos, Ana Lúcia. 2006. Minimal Answers. Ellipsis, syntax and discourse in the

acquisition of European Portuguese. PhD Dissertation, University of Lisbon.

[published 2009, John Benjamins]

Snow, David. 2006. Regression and reorganization of intonation between 6 and 23

months. Child Development, vol. 77 (2): 281-296.

Vanrell, Maria del Mar, Pilar Prieto, Lluïsa Astruc, Elinor Payne and Brechtje Post.

2010. Early acquisition of F0 alignment and scaling patterns in Catalan and Spanish.

In Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2010, Chicago.

Venditti, Jennifer J., Julia Hirschberg and Jackson Liscombe. 2006. Intonational cues to

student questions in tutoring dialogs. In Proceedings of Interspeech 2006 - ICSLP,

Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania: 549-552.

Viana, Céu, Sónia Frota, Isabel Falé, Flaviane Fernandes, Isabel Mascarenhas, Ana

Isabel Mata, Helena Moniz and Marina Vigário. 2007. Towards a P_ToBI.

PAPI2007. Workshop on the Transcription of Intonation in Ibero-Romance.

Universidade do Minho.

Wennerstrom, Ann. 2001. The Music of Everyday Speech: Prosody and Discourse

Analysis. Oxford: OUP.

Wichmann, Anne. 2000. Intonation in Text and Discourse: Beginnings, Middles and

Ends. Harlow: Longman.