7
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Research report Using the behavioral exibility operant task to detect long-term decits in rats following middle cerebral artery occlusion Melissa M.M. Andrews a,b , Sarah Peruzzaro a,b , Shelby Raupp b,c , Jordin Wilks b,c , Julien Rossignol a,b,d , Gary L. Dunbar a,b,c,e, a Field Neurosciences Institute Laboratory for Restorative Neurology, Central Michigan University, Mount Pleasant, MI 48859, United States b Program in Neuroscience, Central Michigan University, Mount Pleasant, MI 48859, United States c Department of Psychology, Central Michigan University, Mount Pleasant, MI 48859, United States d College of Medicine, Central Michigan University, Mount Pleasant, MI 48859, United States e Field Neurosciences Inst., 4677 Towne Centre Rd. Suite 101 Saginaw, MI 48604, United States ARTICLE INFO Keywords: MCAo Long-term Operant Behavioral exibility ABSTRACT Stroke is a leading cause of death and disability and currently only has one FDA approved pharmacological treatment (tissue plasminogen activator), which is only administered to a fraction of stroke patients due to contraindications. New treatments are desperately needed but most treatments fail in clinical trials, even after showing benet in animal models of stroke. To increase the translatability of animal stroke research to humans, sensitive functional measures for both the acute and chronic stages in animal models of stroke are needed. The objective of this study was to determine the sensitivity of certain behavioral tasks, up to seven weeks following occlusion of the middle cerebral artery (MCAo) in rats. A battery of behavioral tasks, including rotorod, cylinder, and limb-placement, was conducted weekly for seven weeks. Also, a behavioral exibility operant task was introduced at the end of the study to measure cognitive decits. All functional outcome measures showed sig- nicant dierences between stroke and control groups, indicating that these tasks are sensitive enough to detect decits in a long-term MCAo study in rats. This provides useful information for those trying to increase trans- latability in their own stroke research by providing long-term sensitive testing paradigms in a relevant stroke model. 1. Introduction Stroke is a cerebrovascular disease in which a blood vessel within the brain or leading to the brain becomes blocked or ruptures. Although the frequency of stroke has been declining, it is still a leading cause of death and long-termdisability [1]. There are limited treatment options following stroke which include surgery and tissue plasminogen acti- vator. Because of the contraindications of these treatments, less than 10% of stroke patients benet from these interventions [2,3]. Although other treatments have been shown to be benecial in animal stroke models, none have proved to be consistently eective in phase III clinical trials [4]. This lack of translatability may, in part, be due to the lack of long- term functional testing in rodent stroke models. It has been suggested that functional outcome measures be assessed at both the acute and chronic (30+days) stages of stroke [5,6]. However, functional testing at these later time points is dicult and can be complicated by spon- taneous recovery [7]. In addition, there are only a few tests that are suciently sensitive to detect chronic stroke decits. One underutilized option for detecting long-term functional decits is operant conditioning. Since humans can have a vast array of decits, ranging from motor and sensory, to cognitive decits such as learning and memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive exibility [810], a sensitive operant task, capable of detecting long-term cognitive decits, could prove useful in translational research. Operant conditioning in- volves modifying behavior by manipulating environmental con- sequences, which can be conducted eciently by using operant con- ditioning chambers, whereby rats learn to press levers in a specied https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2018.08.008 Received 22 May 2018; Received in revised form 7 August 2018; Accepted 10 August 2018 Abbreviations: ANOVA, analysis of variance; FR, xed ratio; HSD, Honest signicant dierence; MCAo, middle cerebral artery occlusion; PBS, phosphate-buered saline; PFA, paraformaldehyde Corresponding author. E-mail addresses: [email protected] (M.M.M. Andrews), [email protected] (S. Peruzzaro), [email protected] (S. Raupp), [email protected] (J. Wilks), [email protected] (J. Rossignol), [email protected] (G.L. Dunbar). Behavioural Brain Research 356 (2019) 1–7 Available online 11 August 2018 0166-4328/ © 2018 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. T

Behavioural Brain Research Lab Doc… · In addition, work in our lab and those of others have indicated that operant techniques can be sufficiently sensitive to detect subtle, long-term

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Behavioural Brain Research Lab Doc… · In addition, work in our lab and those of others have indicated that operant techniques can be sufficiently sensitive to detect subtle, long-term

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Behavioural Brain Research

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr

Research report

Using the behavioral flexibility operant task to detect long-term deficits inrats following middle cerebral artery occlusion

Melissa M.M. Andrewsa,b, Sarah Peruzzaroa,b, Shelby Rauppb,c, Jordin Wilksb,c,Julien Rossignola,b,d, Gary L. Dunbara,b,c,e,⁎

a Field Neurosciences Institute Laboratory for Restorative Neurology, Central Michigan University, Mount Pleasant, MI 48859, United Statesb Program in Neuroscience, Central Michigan University, Mount Pleasant, MI 48859, United Statesc Department of Psychology, Central Michigan University, Mount Pleasant, MI 48859, United Statesd College of Medicine, Central Michigan University, Mount Pleasant, MI 48859, United Statese Field Neurosciences Inst., 4677 Towne Centre Rd. Suite 101 Saginaw, MI 48604, United States

A R T I C L E I N F O

Keywords:MCAoLong-termOperantBehavioral flexibility

A B S T R A C T

Stroke is a leading cause of death and disability and currently only has one FDA approved pharmacologicaltreatment (tissue plasminogen activator), which is only administered to a fraction of stroke patients due tocontraindications. New treatments are desperately needed but most treatments fail in clinical trials, even aftershowing benefit in animal models of stroke. To increase the translatability of animal stroke research to humans,sensitive functional measures for both the acute and chronic stages in animal models of stroke are needed. Theobjective of this study was to determine the sensitivity of certain behavioral tasks, up to seven weeks followingocclusion of the middle cerebral artery (MCAo) in rats. A battery of behavioral tasks, including rotorod, cylinder,and limb-placement, was conducted weekly for seven weeks. Also, a behavioral flexibility operant task wasintroduced at the end of the study to measure cognitive deficits. All functional outcome measures showed sig-nificant differences between stroke and control groups, indicating that these tasks are sensitive enough to detectdeficits in a long-term MCAo study in rats. This provides useful information for those trying to increase trans-latability in their own stroke research by providing long-term sensitive testing paradigms in a relevant strokemodel.

1. Introduction

Stroke is a cerebrovascular disease in which a blood vessel withinthe brain or leading to the brain becomes blocked or ruptures. Althoughthe frequency of stroke has been declining, it is still a leading cause ofdeath and long-termdisability [1]. There are limited treatment optionsfollowing stroke which include surgery and tissue plasminogen acti-vator. Because of the contraindications of these treatments, less than10% of stroke patients benefit from these interventions [2,3]. Althoughother treatments have been shown to be beneficial in animal strokemodels, none have proved to be consistently effective in phase IIIclinical trials [4].

This lack of translatability may, in part, be due to the lack of long-term functional testing in rodent stroke models. It has been suggested

that functional outcome measures be assessed at both the acute andchronic (30+days) stages of stroke [5,6]. However, functional testingat these later time points is difficult and can be complicated by spon-taneous recovery [7]. In addition, there are only a few tests that aresufficiently sensitive to detect chronic stroke deficits.

One underutilized option for detecting long-term functional deficitsis operant conditioning. Since humans can have a vast array of deficits,ranging from motor and sensory, to cognitive deficits such as learningand memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility [8–10], asensitive operant task, capable of detecting long-term cognitive deficits,could prove useful in translational research. Operant conditioning in-volves modifying behavior by manipulating environmental con-sequences, which can be conducted efficiently by using operant con-ditioning chambers, whereby rats learn to press levers in a specified

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2018.08.008Received 22 May 2018; Received in revised form 7 August 2018; Accepted 10 August 2018

Abbreviations: ANOVA, analysis of variance; FR, fixed ratio; HSD, Honest significant difference; MCAo, middle cerebral artery occlusion; PBS, phosphate-bufferedsaline; PFA, paraformaldehyde⁎ Corresponding author.E-mail addresses: [email protected] (M.M.M. Andrews), [email protected] (S. Peruzzaro), [email protected] (S. Raupp),

[email protected] (J. Wilks), [email protected] (J. Rossignol), [email protected] (G.L. Dunbar).

Behavioural Brain Research 356 (2019) 1–7

Available online 11 August 20180166-4328/ © 2018 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

T

Page 2: Behavioural Brain Research Lab Doc… · In addition, work in our lab and those of others have indicated that operant techniques can be sufficiently sensitive to detect subtle, long-term

manner to earn reinforcers (such as sucrose pellets or sweetened li-quids). Because these procedures can be automated, substantialamounts of data can be gathered in an efficient and objective manner.In addition, work in our lab and those of others have indicated thatoperant techniques can be sufficiently sensitive to detect subtle, long-term deficits up to 21 weeks following stroke [11–14].

A relatively sensitive operant task for detecting deficits in stroke isthe behavioral flexibility task, developed by Linden et al. [13]. This taskis inexpensive and easy to administer and translates nicely to cognitiveflexibility deficits observed in human stroke patients. In this procedure,mice or rats are offered two levers within an operant chamber for which5 lever presses on the active lever delivers a pellet. Each time a pellet isearned, the active lever alternates between the two available levers.Linden et al. [13] demonstrated a deficit in mice given the middlecerebral artery occlusion (MCAo) at three weeks prior to testing. TheMCAo mice earned fewer reinforcers and made more incorrect leverpresses than control mice.

The present study utilized the behavioral flexibility task, along withmore commonly used assessments (i.e., the rotorod, limb-placement,and cylinder tasks) for a 7-week period following unilateral MCAo inrats. We assessed whether a 60-minute occlusion would be sufficient toaffect the behavioral measures on these tasks, given that this durationshould produce measurable deficits, yet spare sufficient amounts ofsalvageable tissue for assessing potential treatment effects [15,16].

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Subjects

Twenty male Sprague Dawley rats bred onsite from breeders ob-tained or purchased directly from Charles River (Wilmington, MA) wereused for this study. Rats were group housed with food and wateravailable ad libitum within the home cage until day 24, when rats weresingle housed and given daily supplemental feeding throughout theremainder of the study, with water available ad libitum (Fig. 1). All ratsweighed between 250–300 g at the time of surgery and no between-group differences in body weights were observed, thereafter. All ratswere housed on a reversed day/night cycle with lights on at 2000 h andoff at 0800 h, with all testing occurring during the dark part of thecycle. Rats were randomly assigned to receive either a sham surgery (noMCAo, n= 10) or a 60-minute MCAo (n=10). Randomization waschosen to alleviate any systematic differences between groups. Allprocedures in this study were approved by the Institutional Animal Careand Use Committee at Central Michigan University.

2.2. MCAo surgery

At 250–300 g of weight, rats underwent the MCAo surgery followingthe procedures of Longa et al. [17]. Briefly, rats were anesthetized usinga mixture of O2 and isoflurane. A small incision in the neck exposed thecarotid arteries. The external carotid artery was cauterized. At thatpoint, if rats belonged to the sham group they were sutured closed andplaced into recovery. For the 60-minute MCAo group, microvascularclips were placed on the common and internal carotid arteries and asilicon coated nylon filament (0.37 mm in diameter; Doccol, MA) wasinserted into the external carotid artery. The clips were removed, andthe filament maneuvered through the internal carotid artery to blockblood flow at the MCA. The filament was left in place for 60-minutes.Since no differences or significant paw preferences were observed in thebaseline testing of the cylinder task, occlusions were made in the same(right) hemisphere for each rat to maintain consistency. Once the fila-ment was removed, the wound was sutured, and the rat was closelymonitored for post-operative recovery.

2.3. Behavioral testing

2.3.1. Baseline performances and trainingThe timeline for behavioral testing is depicted in Fig. 1. Prior to any

surgical manipulation, baseline performance was assessed on the cy-linder-, rotorod-, and limb-placement-tests. Preceding the rotorodbaseline, rats were trained to maintain balance on an accelerating ro-torod (beginning at 4 rpm, with the rod accelerating at 0.2 rpm/second)for at least 2 min. At post-operative day 2, all rats began behavioraltesting. Behavioral testing occurred weekly throughout the study atapproximately the same time of day.

2.3.2. Cylinder testThe cylinder test was utilized to detect forelimb-use asymmetry

[18]. The rat was placed in a clear plexiglass cylinder, 39 cm high, witha diameter of 21 cm (PLAS BY LABS, Lansing, MI) and forepaw place-ments were counted. A paw placement was defined as a forepawtouching the side of the apparatus while the rat balanced on its two rearpaws. Sessions concluded once a combined total of 20 left and right pawplacements were made or when 10min had elapsed, whichever oc-curred first. To account for right, left, and both paw placements, thefollowing formula was used to calculate a laterality index [19]:

=

Laterality IndexLeft paw placements Right paw placementsTotal left right and both paw placements, ,

Cylinder testing occurred at approximately 0830 h on day 2, 9, 16,23, 30, 37, and 44 (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1. Diagram of experimental timeline.

M.M.M. Andrews et al. Behavioural Brain Research 356 (2019) 1–7

2

Page 3: Behavioural Brain Research Lab Doc… · In addition, work in our lab and those of others have indicated that operant techniques can be sufficiently sensitive to detect subtle, long-term

2.3.3. Limb-placement testThe limb-placement test (a variation of [20]) was employed to as-

sess asymmetry in limb placement. All testing occurred on an elevatedsurface with 90° corners between the top and sides of the structure (i.e.a table top). The rats underwent experimenter manipulations and theleft and right paws were scored on a 0–2 scale. If the limb was notplaced, it was scored as a 0, complete and immediate placing wasscored as a 2, and a 1 was given for anything in between such as de-layed placement or flailing (Fig. 2). The total score was calculated witha maximum score of 16 points. The limb-placement test took place atapproximately 0930 h on day 2, 9, 16, 23, 30, 37, and 44 (Fig. 1).

2.3.4. Rotorod testRotorod testing was used to assess gross motor movement [21]. Rats

were placed opposite of rotation on a 7.3 cm diameter rod elevated70 cm above the floor rotating 4 rpm and accelerating to 40 rpm over180 s (Columbus Instruments, Columbus, Ohio). Once the rat fell, orwas removed, from the rod, the time was recorded as latency to fall. Therat was removed from the rod at 180 s, or if the rat clung to the rodthrough two rotations. Three trials were conducted each testing day,with a 10-minute inter-trial interval. Rotorod testing occurred at ap-proximately 1600 h on day 2, 9, 16, 23, 30, 37, and 44 (Fig. 1).

2.4. Operant testing

On day 24, rats were reduced to approximately 85% of their freefeeding weight and were maintained at this weight through the re-mainder of the study via daily supplemental feedings (Fig. 1).

2.4.1. Operant apparatusOperant chambers were housed inside sound-attenuating cubicles

(from Med Associates Inc., St. Albans, VT) which contained fans forwhite noise to block out extraneous stimuli. Operant chambers mea-sured 53× 35 X 21 cm with a wire-mesh floor surrounded by 2 plex-iglass walls, 2 metal walls, and a plexiglass ceiling. The back wallcontained a single centered house light 13 cm above the floor, whichwas illuminated during all operant procedures. Centered on the frontmetal wall, 2 cm from the floor, was the pellet dispenser which dis-tributed 45-mg sucrose dustless precision pellets (Bio-Serv, Flemington,NJ) as the reinforcer. Equidistant on each side of the pellet dispenser,6.5 cm above the floor, were two levers, each protruding 2.5 cm into thechamber with a stimulus light 6 cm above each. Med PC for WindowsVersion IV software was used to program experimental contingenciesand to gather data.

2.4.2. Operant trainingBeginning on day 31, rats were magazine trained on a 60-second

variable time schedule. Levers were absent from the operant chambersand pellets were delivered, on average, every 60 s for a total of 30min.The following day, the levers were inserted into the operant chambers

and the rats trained to lever-press on a fixed ratio 1 (FR1) schedule ofreinforcement, whereby a lever-press on either lever delivered a re-inforcer. The FR1 training sessions completed once 20 reinforcers wereearned or an hour had elapsed, whichever occurred first. After learningto respond on a FR1 schedule, the rats were placed on a FR5 schedule ofreinforcement, which required 5 consecutive lever presses on eitherlever to earn a reinforcer. Sessions continued until 20 reinforcers wereearned or 1 h had elapsed, whichever occurred first. Training sessionstook place throughout week 5 at approximately 1000 h each day(Fig. 1).

2.4.3. Behavioral flexibility taskBeginning on day 37, rats began the behavioral flexibility operant

task. The behavioral flexibility operant task was conducted to assessability to learn new reinforcement paradigms [13]. During a behavioralflexibility session, reinforcers were earned by completing 5 consecutivelever presses on the active lever (either the left or the right lever aschosen initially at random). No stimulus was associated with the activelever. Responses on the inactive lever had no programmed con-sequence. At the completion of 5 consecutive active lever-presses, areinforcer was delivered, and the alternate lever became active. Aftereach earned reinforcer, the active lever was alternated until 40minelapsed and the session ended. Reinforcers earned, along with activeand inactive lever presses, were recorded during each session. Sessionswere conducted at approximately 1000 h every day for 7 continuousdays.

2.5. Histology

Rats were euthanized on day 45 with an overdose (120mg/kg) ofsodium pentobarbital (Fatal Plus, Vortech Pharmaceuticals, Dearborn,MI) and transcardially perfused with 0.1M phosphate buffered saline(PBS, pH 7.4) and then 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA; dissolved in 0.1MPBS, pH 7.4). Brains were collected and submerged in 4% PFA over-night at 4 °C and then moved to a sucrose solution (30% w/v; FisherScientific, Waltham, MA) at 4 °C until the tissue was saturated. Tissuewas then frozen with anhydrous methylbutane (Sigma-Aldrich, St.Louis, MO) packed in dry ice and stored at −80 °C until sectioned.Using a cryostat (Vibratome, St. Louis, MO), brains were sectionedcoronally at 20 μm and directly mounted onto positive-charged slides(Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA). Thirteen equally spaced re-gions of interest were collected throughout the striatum, beginning atapproximately+ 2.20mm from bregma and ending at approximately-4.16 mm from bregma.

Hematoxylin and eosin (H&E) staining was utilized to visualize thedamaged area of the brain. The tissue was heat-fixed to the slide at∼90 °C for 2.5min. Slides were brought back to room temperature andthen submerged for 60 s in tap water. Slides were submerged in 0.1%Meyers Hematoxylin (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) for 8min beforegoing into running tap water for 10min. The tissue was then immersed

Fig. 2. Scoring for the limb-placement test.

M.M.M. Andrews et al. Behavioural Brain Research 356 (2019) 1–7

3

Page 4: Behavioural Brain Research Lab Doc… · In addition, work in our lab and those of others have indicated that operant techniques can be sufficiently sensitive to detect subtle, long-term

in a 0.05% ethanol eosin (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) for 2min.Directly following the H&E staining the slides went into a 50% ethanolsolution for 30 s (all alcohols from Deacon Laboratories, Inc.,Allentown, PA), a 70% ethanol solution for 30 s, a 90% ethanol solutionfor 30 s, and a 100% ethanol solution for 1min. Tissue was then sub-merged in xylene (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) and immediately coverslipped using Eukitt mounting medium (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis MO).Hematoxylin and eosin images were scanned using a Nikon Coolscan IVscanner (Nikon, Melville, NY) and analyzed with Image J software(NIH, Rockville, MD). As described previously [12], tissue was assessedfor damage and reported as a percentage of the contralateral hemi-sphere.

2.6. Statistical analysis

All statistics were calculated in SPSS (IBM, Armonk, NY) with analpha level of p < 0.05. Behavioral baselines and infarct size betweengroups were analyzed using independent samples t-tests while beha-vioral tasks following MCAo were analyzed using repeated-measuresanalysis of variance (ANOVA). If Mauchly’s test of sphericity was foundsignificant, a Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied. Infarct size inthe MCAo group was correlated with the last session of each behavioralmeasure using Pearson correlation.

3. Results

3.1. Infarct size

An independent samples t-test showed an increase in damaged areain the 60-minute MCAo group as compared to the sham group, t(18)=-3.21, p=0.005 (Fig. 3). Infarct size in the 60-minute MCAo groupdid not significantly correlate with any behavioral measure except forthe limb-placement task (Fig. 4A) which was negatively correlated toinfarct size (r=-0.727, n=10, p=0.017; Fig. 4B).

3.2. Cylinder task

The laterality index was calculated and averaged by group for eachsession (Fig. 5). No significant difference was found during baseline, t(18)= 0.039, p=0.970. The laterality index was compared betweengroups across sessions using a 2 x 7 (2 groups x 7 sessions) mixedANOVA. Mauchly’s test of sphericity was not significant (p=0.214) sono correction was applied. There was a significant difference in later-ality index between groups (F(1,16)= 34.077; p<0.001) although nosignificant difference was found over time (F(6,96)= 0.836; p=0.545)and there was no significant group x session interaction effect (F(6,96)= 1.713; p=0.126).

3.3. Limb-placement task

The total score obtained for the left side (side of deficit) for each ratwas used for statistical analysis (Fig. 6). Baseline data showed no

significant difference between groups, t(18)= 0.849, p=0.407. Limb-placement scores were compared between groups across sessions usinga 2 x 7 mixed ANOVA. Mauchly’s test of sphericity was not found to besignificant (p=0.075) and no correction was applied. There was nosignificant difference across sessions (F(6,108)= 0.705; p=0.647)although there was a significant difference between groups (F(1,18)= 7.802; p=0.012), and a significant group x session interac-tion (F(6,108)= 2.365; p=0.035).

3.4. Rotorod task

The average of all three trials within each rotorod session was usedfor statistical analysis (Fig. 7). Baseline data revealed no significantdifference between groups, t(18)= 0.983, p=0.339. A 2 x 7 mixedANOVA revealed Mauchly’s test of sphericity not significant(p=0.053) so no correction was used. There was a significant differ-ence between groups (F(1,18)= 9.219; p=0.007) but no differencewas found in latency to fall across sessions (F(6,108)= 1.909;p=0.086) and no group x session interaction effect was observed (F(6,108)= 0.717; p=0.637).

3.5. Operant measures

There were two sham rats that never contacted the alternating levercontingency (i.e. never earned more than a single reinforcer in a ses-sion) which were excluded from the analyses. With the exclusion cri-teria in place, the number of reinforcers earned, the total number oflever presses, and the percentage of incorrect lever presses was com-pared across all 7 operant sessions between the sham (n= 8) and 60-minute (n=10) MCAo group.

The total number of lever presses was examined between groupsacross sessions using a 2×7 mixed ANOVA (Fig. 8A). Mauchley’s testof sphericity was significant (p < 0.001) and the Greenhouse-Geissercorrection was used. Total number of lever presses significantly in-creased across sessions (F(2.500,39.993)= 9.100; p<0.001), and wassignificantly different between groups (F(1,16)= 9.978; p=0.006). Nosignificant group x session interaction was observed (F(2.500,39.993)= 0.681; p=0.543).

The number of reinforcers earned was examined using a 2×7mixed ANOVA to indicate the ability of rats to learn the alternatingactive lever pattern (Fig. 8B). Mauchley’s test of sphericity was found tobe significant (p < 0.001) so the Greenhouse-Geisser correction wasused. The number of reinforcers earned significantly increased acrosssessions (F(1.428,22.849)= 27.509; p<0.001) and differed sig-nificantly between groups (F(1,16)= 12.264; p=0.003). There wasalso a significant group x session interaction effect (F(1.482,22.849)= 4.222; p=0.039).

Since a significant difference was found between groups in the totalnumber of lever presses, the number of incorrect lever presses (pressesmade on the inactive lever) were represented as a percentage of overalllever presses to diminish any confounding effects of overall motoricdifferences (Fig. 8C). The percentage of incorrect lever presses was

Fig. 3. Hematoxylin and eosin staining of a (A) sham and (B) 60-minute MCAo subject.

M.M.M. Andrews et al. Behavioural Brain Research 356 (2019) 1–7

4

Page 5: Behavioural Brain Research Lab Doc… · In addition, work in our lab and those of others have indicated that operant techniques can be sufficiently sensitive to detect subtle, long-term

analyzed using a 2×7 mixed ANOVA, and Mauchley’s test of spheri-city was found to be significant (p < 0.001) so the Greenhouse-Geissercorrection was used. The percentage of incorrect lever presses wasshown to significantly decrease over time (F(2.125,33.997)= 23.034;p<0.001), and was shown to be significantly different between groups(F(1,16)= 10.607; p=0.005). No group x session interaction effectwas observed (F(2.125,33.997)= 1.353; p=0.273).

Fig. 4. Correlational measures between infarct percentage and behavioral task. (A) Pearson’s correlational values for behavioral measures correlated with percentageof damaged area. (B) Scatterplot of the correlation between percentage of damaged area and scoring on the limb-placement task. *p<0.05.

Fig. 5. Average laterality index for the cylinder task by group across sessions.Laterality index was scored between -1 and 1. A score of 0 represented equal leftand right paw placements and a score of -1 represented exclusive right pawplacements. The 60-minute MCAo group used their right paw significantly morethan the sham group. *p<0.05, error bars represent standard error of themean.

Fig. 6. Average limb-placement scores by group across sessions. Limb-place-ment scores ranged from 0-16, 16 indicated a perfect score. The 60-minuteMCAo group scored significantly lower than the sham group. *p<0.05, errorbars represent standard error of the mean.

Fig. 7. Average latency to fall during the rotorod task by group across sessions.Sham rats stayed on the rod significantly longer than 60-minute MCAo rats.*p<0.05, error bars represent standard error of the mean.

Fig. 8. Operant measures across 7 sessions for each group. The total number oflever presses during the behavioral flexibility operant task (A), the number ofreinforcers earned during the behavioral flexibility operant task (B), and thepercentage of incorrect lever presses during the behavioral flexibility operanttask (C) significantly changed across sessions and was significantly differentbetween groups. *p<0.05 for sham compared to 60-minute MCAo, error barsrepresent standard error of the mean.

M.M.M. Andrews et al. Behavioural Brain Research 356 (2019) 1–7

5

Page 6: Behavioural Brain Research Lab Doc… · In addition, work in our lab and those of others have indicated that operant techniques can be sufficiently sensitive to detect subtle, long-term

4. Discussion

All behavioral tasks conducted within this study (cylinder, limb-placement, rotorod, and operant testing), revealed significant differ-ences across groups over seven weeks of testing. The infarct size, asshown by H&E staining, also revealed a significant difference at 45 daysafter surgery. The infarct size was also negatively correlated with thelimb-placement task. Taken together, it appears that when using thisMCAo model in rats, these specific tasks are sufficient to identify defi-cits up to seven weeks after stroke. The behavioral flexibility operanttask was used in our study to determine the ability of each rat to adaptto changing environmental contingencies. We found that it provided along-term, sensitive, cognitive measure that successfully detected long-term deficits in a rodent stroke model. While behavioral flexibility canbe tested in a variety of ways, such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting taskin humans [22], our use of a simple alternating contingency operanttask that required rats to learn to alternate back and forth on two leversto earn reinforcers proved successful in distinguishing rats with strokesfrom those given sham surgery. This task was a modified version of theone used by Linden et al. [13] whereby mice given MCAo were tested30 days after surgery and showed deficits. Linden et al. [13] concludedthat the disruption of behavioral flexibility is due to damage that occursexplicitly in the dorsomedial striatum. This conclusion is supported byothers who have shown that damage to the dorsomedial striatum pro-duces deficits in other measures of behavioral flexibility as well[23,24].

Behavioral flexibility can be measured in a variety of ways, like theoperant task we used here where the percentage of incorrect leverpresses is the representative measure of flexibility. However, there areother constructs that this specific operant task may be measuring, in-cluding motor deficits, differences in food motivation, or even a deficitin general learning of the lever pressing contingency.

While the lower number of total lever presses in the MCAo groupcompared to the sham group could support the argument that deficits inthe operant task are due to motor deficits rather than deficits in be-havioral flexibility, it would be unlikely given that lever presses can bemade using multiple topographies (the rat can use the contralaterallimb, the ipsilateral limb, their teeth, etc.) and does not require the useof the impaired limb. The number of incorrect lever presses was alsoshown as a percentage of overall lever presses, further suggesting thatmotor deficits did not confound the results obtained on this task. It isalso unlikely that these results are due to a difference in food motiva-tion, as all rats were similarly motivated during training to earn foodpellets. During the same training period, all rats learned to lever pressfor food pellets within the time allotted and consumed all food pelletsthat were delivered. The successful training of all rats also suggest thatour results are a product of deficits in behavioral flexibility deficit ra-ther than a general learning impairment. Also, the data obtained fromany rat that did not contact the alternating lever contingency (ie., neverearned two reinforcers within a single session) was not used in theanalyses of the operant task. This was done to ensure all rats beinganalyzed were being assessed for their inability to adapt to changingenvironmental contingencies (i.e., the alternating active lever).

To our knowledge, this is the first time that the behavioral flexibilityoperant task was used to assess long-term (6-weeks post-MCAo) cog-nitive deficits in this rat model of stroke. The findings confirmed thoseof Linden et al. [13] who found deficits at 3-weeks post-MCAo. In bothof these studies, the sham-operated rats earned more reinforcers andmade less incorrect lever presses than those given MCAo. These findingssupport the contention that the behavioral flexibility operant taskprovides a promising protocol for detecting stroke deficits, since it isobjective, only takes a week of testing, can detect deficits 30+ daysfollowing stroke, has numerous measurable parameters, and is com-parable to human tasks (such as the Wisconsin card sorting task). Whilethe behavioral flexibility operant task in this situation was sufficientlysensitive to detect deficits in MCAo rats, it also proved to be so

challenging for a few rats that never learned the task. Though the ab-sence of learning is still valuable information, changing how the task iscompleted (making it easier, running it longer than seven days, orconducting more rigorous training) may provide a means of refining thetask.

4.1. Conclusions

In conclusion, we found that the behavioral flexibility operant taskwas sufficiently sensitive to detect deficits in rats for up to seven weeksfollowing stroke and that a 60-minute MCAo was adequate to produceclear deficits. Pairing the behavioral flexibility task with rotorod, limb-placement, and cylinder, gave a well-rounded battery of behavioraltasks that revealed significant chronic deficits in the rat MCAo stokemodel, which should translate well to the human condition and providea useful model to test potential therapies.

Declaration of interest

None.

Funding

This work was supported by the Office of Research and SponsoredPrograms, the College of Medicine, the College of Humanities andSocial & Behavioral Sciences, and the John G. Kulhavi Professorship atCentral Michigan University, as well as the William F. AllinderEndowment for Stroke Research at the Field Neurosciences Institute.

References

[1] D. Mozaffarian, E.J. Benjamin, A.S. Go, D.K. Arnett, M.J. Blaha, ... M.B. Turner,Executive summary: heart disease and stroke statisitcs—2016 update, A report fromthe American Heart Association, Circulation 133 (2016) 47–454.

[2] M.J. Reeves, S. Arora, J.P. Broderick, M. Frankel, J.P. Heinrich, S. Hickenbottom, ...P. Weiss, Acute stroke care in the US: results from 4 pilot prototypes of the PaulCoverdell National Acute Stroke Registry, Stroke 36 (2005) 1232–1240.

[3] L.H. Schwamm, G.C. Fonarow, M.J. Reeves, W. Pan, M.R. Frankel, E.E. Smith, ...K.A. LaBresh, Get with the guidelines—stroke is associated with sustained im-provement in care for patients hospitalized with acute stroke or transient ischemicattack, Circulation 119 (2009) 07–115.

[4] S. Xu, S. Pan, The failure of animal models of neuroprotection in acute ischemicstroke to translate to clinical efficacy, Med. Sci. Monit. Basic Res. 19 (2013) 37–45.

[5] C.I. Ryan, T.A. Doucette, D.A. Gill, K.D. Langdon, Y. Liu, M.A. Perry, R.A. Tasker,An improved post-operative care protocol allows detection of long-term functionaldeficits following MCAo surgery in rats, J. Neurosci. Methods 154 (2006) 30–37.

[6] M. Fisher, G. Feuerstein, D.W. Howells, P.D. Hurn, T.A. Kent, S.I. Savitz, E.H. Lo,Update of the stroke therapy academic industry roundtable preclinical re-commendations, Stroke 40 (2009) 2244–2250.

[7] S.C. Cramer, Repairing the human brain after stroke: I. Mechanisms of spontaneousrecovery, Ann. Neurol. 63 (2008) 272–287.

[8] S.S. Kessner, U. Bingel, G. Thomalla, Somatosensory deficits after stroke: a scopingreview, Top. Stroke Rehabil. 23 (2016) 136–146.

[9] T. Schallert, Behavioral tests for preclinical intervention assessment, NeuroRx 3(2006) 497–504.

[10] K.M. Sicard, N. Henninger, M. Fisher, T.Q. Duong, C.F. Ferris, Long-term changes offunctional MRI-based brain function, behavioral status, and histopathology aftertransient focal cerebral ischemia in rats, Stroke 37 (2006) 2593–2600.

[11] A. Ferrara, S.E. Bejaoui, S. Seyen, E. Tirelli, J.C. Plumier, The usefulness of operantconditioning procedures to assess long-lasting deficits following transient focalischemia in mice, Behav. Brain Res. 205 (2009) 525–534.

[12] T.J. Hudzik, A. Borrelli, P. Bialobok, D. Widzowski, S. Sydserff, A. Howell,P. Gerdron, ... G.C. Palmer, Long-term functional end points following middlecerebral artery occlusion in the rat, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 65 (2000)553–562.

[13] J. Linden, L. Fassotte, E. Tirelli, J.C. Plumier, A. Ferrara, Assessment of behavioralflexibility after middle cerebral artery occlusion in mice, Behav. Brain Res. 258(2014) 127–137.

[14] S.A. Lowrance, K.D. Fink, A. Crane, J. Matyas, N.D. Dey, J.J. Matchynski, ...G.L. Dunbar, Bone-marrow-derived mesenchymal stem cells attenuate cognitivedeficits in an endothelin-1 rat model of stroke, Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 33 (2015)579–588.

[15] A. Durukan, T. Tatlisumak, Animal models of ischemic stroke, Stroke 92 (2009)43–66.

[16] F. Fluri, M.K. Schuhmann, C. Kleinschnitz, Animal models of ischemic stroke andtheir application in clinical research, Drug Des. Devel. Ther. 9 (2015) 3445–3454.

M.M.M. Andrews et al. Behavioural Brain Research 356 (2019) 1–7

6

Page 7: Behavioural Brain Research Lab Doc… · In addition, work in our lab and those of others have indicated that operant techniques can be sufficiently sensitive to detect subtle, long-term

[17] E.Z. Longa, P.R. Weinstein, S. Carlson, R. Cummins, Reversible middle cerebralartery occlusion without craniectomy in rats, Stroke 17 (1986) 472–476.

[18] T. Schallert, S.M. Fleming, J.L. Leasure, J.L. Tillerson, S.T. Bland, CNS plasticity andassessment of forelimb sensorimotor outcome in unilateral rat models of stroke,cortical ablation, parkinsonism and spinal cord injury, Neuropharmacology 39(2000) 777–787.

[19] M. Balkaya, J. Krober, K. Gertz, S. Peruzzaro, M. Endres, Characterization of long-term functional outcome in a murine model of mild brain ischemia, J. Neurosci.Methods 213 (2013) 179–187.

[20] M. De Ryck, J.V. Reempts, M. Borgers, A. Wauquier, A.J. Janssen, Photochemicalstroke model: flunarizine prevents sensorimotor deficits after neocortical infarcts inrats, Stroke 20 (1989) 1383–1390.

[21] R.J. Hamm, B.R. Pike, D.M. O’Dell, B.G. Lyeth, L.W. Jenkins, The rotorod test: anevaluation of its effectiveness in assessing motor deficits following traumatic braininjury, J. Neurotrauma 11 (1994) 187–196.

[22] K. Heyder, B. Suchan, I. Daum, Cortico-subcortical contributions to executivecontrol, Acta Psychol. 115 (2004) 271–289.

[23] A. Castañé, D.E. Theobald, T.W. Robbins, Selective lesions of the dorsomedialstriatum impair serial spatial reversal learning in rats, Behav. Brain Res. 210 (2010)74–83.

[24] D.M. Eagle, T. Humby, S.B. Dunnett, T.W. Robbins, Effects of regional striatal le-sions on motor, motivational, and executive aspects of progressive-ratio perfor-mance in rats, Behav. Neurosci. 113 (1999) 718–731.

M.M.M. Andrews et al. Behavioural Brain Research 356 (2019) 1–7

7