17
This article was downloaded by: [University of Western Ontario] On: 21 October 2014, At: 12:32 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK European Journal of Cognitive Psychology Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pecp20 Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study Natasha Tokowicz a & Tessa Warren a a Departments of Psychology and Linguistics and Learning Research & Development Center , University of Pittsburgh , Pittsburgh, PA, USA Published online: 15 Jul 2010. To cite this article: Natasha Tokowicz & Tessa Warren (2010) Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study, European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22:7, 1092-1106, DOI: 10.1080/09541440903325178 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09541440903325178 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content.

Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

  • Upload
    tessa

  • View
    213

  • Download
    1

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

This article was downloaded by: [University of Western Ontario]On: 21 October 2014, At: 12:32Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH,UK

European Journal of CognitivePsychologyPublication details, including instructions for authorsand subscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pecp20

Beginning adult L2 learners'sensitivity to morphosyntacticviolations: A self-paced readingstudyNatasha Tokowicz a & Tessa Warren aa Departments of Psychology and Linguistics andLearning Research & Development Center , Universityof Pittsburgh , Pittsburgh, PA, USAPublished online: 15 Jul 2010.

To cite this article: Natasha Tokowicz & Tessa Warren (2010) Beginning adult L2learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study, EuropeanJournal of Cognitive Psychology, 22:7, 1092-1106, DOI: 10.1080/09541440903325178

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09541440903325178

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all theinformation (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform.However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make norepresentations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, orsuitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressedin this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not theviews of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content shouldnot be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sourcesof information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions,claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilitieswhatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connectionwith, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content.

Page 2: Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes.Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expresslyforbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

este

rn O

ntar

io]

at 1

2:32

21

Oct

ober

201

4

Page 3: Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

Beginning adult L2 learners’ sensitivity to

morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

Natasha Tokowicz and Tessa Warren

Departments of Psychology and Linguistics and Learning Research &

Development Center, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA

This study investigated beginning adult second language (L2) learners’ sensitivity toL2 morphosyntactic violations as a function of cross-language similarity. Onlinesensitivity was indexed by self-paced reading times at: (1) the critical word at which theviolation could first be detected, (2) the post-critical word, and (3) the sentence-finalword. In conditions in which morphosyntactic marking systems were similar ordifferent in L1 and L2, reading times on the critical word were slower when it cueda violation than when it did not; however, this sensitivity was not apparent in aconstruction unique to L2. Slower reading times to violations spilled over onto thepost-critical word. Cross-language similarity also influenced sentence-final wordreading times. Despite this online sensitivity, post-sentence grammaticality judge-ments were generally poor. However, these judgements were influenced by morpho-syntactic markings on words after the critical word, suggesting that learners can makeuse of this information.

Keywords: L2 learning; Morphosyntactic violations; Competition Model; Self-

paced reading; Grammaticality judgements.

How do adults learn a second language (L2)? Answering this question requires

understanding how processing changeswith proficiency (e.g., McDonald, 1987).

The present study provides a snapshot of processing at the beginning of

Correspondence should be addressed to Natasha Tokowicz, Learning Research & Development

Center, 3939 O’Hara St., Room 634, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, USA.

E-mail: [email protected]

During the writing of this manuscript, NT was supported by NSF-BCS 0745372 and TW

was supported by NIH-HD053639. We thank Angels Rusinol for her assistance in designing the

experiment, and for creating the stimuli and programming the experiment. We also thank the

members of the PLUM lab at the University of Pittsburgh, especially Rhonda McClain, Alison

Phillips, and Courtney Smith, for testing participants and coding data. An earlier version of

these findings was reported at the 47th annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Houston,

TX, USA. We thank Wouter Duyck and an anonymous reviewer for comments on an earlier

version of this manuscript.

EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY

2010, 22 (7), 1092�1106

# 2010 Psychology Press, an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business

http://www.psypress.com/ecp DOI: 10.1080/09541440903325178

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

este

rn O

ntar

io]

at 1

2:32

21

Oct

ober

201

4

Page 4: Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

proficiency development and investigates the following questions: To what kinds

of morphosyntactic information are beginning adult L2 learners sensitive? Are

beginning adult L2 learners sensitive to morphosyntactic violations at the

earliest point at which they can be detected? What is the timecourse of this

sensitivity? What factors influence this timecourse (e.g., cross-language

similarity, amount of agreeing/disagreeing information in the sentence)?

Knowing what kinds of information beginning learners are sensitive to and

when they are sensitive to it is necessary to understand how L2 processing

changes with proficiency, and could have implications for instruction.

Previous research suggests that beginning learners use first language (L1)

cues in L2 comprehension. In particular, according to the Competition

Model (MacWhinney, 1997; see also MacWhinney, 2005), cues in the

language input are more or less available to the learner, and more or less

valid as indicators of function. Availability and validity determine the

strength of individual cues. Because an adult L2 learner’s L1 is entrenched,

initial L2 processing will follow L1 cues. Therefore, the Competition Model

predicts that beginning adult learners should easily process grammatical

patterns that are similar in L1 and L2, but have difficulty with ones that

differ in L1 and L2, because relying on L1 cues will lead to errors in this case.

In an experiment similar to the present one, Tokowicz and MacWhinney

(2005) examined online sensitivity to L2 morphosyntactic violations in

beginning adult learners using event-related potentials (ERPs). They tested

three cross-language similarity conditions: (1) similar, in which both

languages licensed a verb form under similar circumstances (verb progressive

aspect licensing), (2) different, in which one language did and the other did

not mark agreement in the lexical system under investigation, but for which

both languages did mark this kind of agreement in similar lexical systems

(possessive and definite determiner-noun number agreement), and (3)

unique, in which one language had an agreement system the other lacked

(determiner-noun gender agreement). Following the Competition Model,

they predicted that learners would be most sensitive in the similar condition

because of positive transfer and least sensitive in the different condition

because of conflicting cues; predictions for the unique condition depend on

input exposure because this system doesn’t exist in L1.

Similar: Su abuela cocina/*cocinando muy bien.

His/Her grandmother cooks/*cooking very well.

Different: Las/*La gomas estan en la bolsa.

ThePL/*TheSING erasers are in the bag.

MORPHOSYNTACTIC VIOLATIONS IN L2 1093

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

este

rn O

ntar

io]

at 1

2:32

21

Oct

ober

201

4

Page 5: Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

Unique: Mi amiga tiene un/*una apartamento grande.

My friend has aMASC/*aFEM large apartmentMASC.

Tokowicz and MacWhinney (2005) used the P600 ERP component, which

is typically more positive in response to ungrammatical than grammatical

sentences (e.g., Osterhout & Mobley, 1995), to measure online sensitivity.

Post-sentence grammaticality judgements indexed offline sensitivity. Con-

sistent with Competition Model predictions, P600s were more positive to

ungrammatical than grammatical sentences for the similar and unique

constructions, but not for the different construction. Despite this online

sensitivity, grammaticality judgements were at chance.

Other ERP studies have also examined sensitivity to L2 (morpho)syn-

tactic violations in adult learners. Some indicate that moderately-

proficient learners are sensitive to certain violations (e.g., Rossi, Gugler,

Friederici, & Hahne, 2006), whereas others suggest that sensitivity holds

only for more-proficient speakers (e.g., Hahne, Mueller, & Clahsen, 2006;

see van Hell & Tokowicz, in press, for a review). Recent work in this

area is increasingly focused on the influence of cross-language similarity

(e.g., Sabourin & Stowe, 2008; see, for reviews, Kotz, 2009; Tolentino &

Tokowicz, 2009).

The present study extended that of Tokowicz and MacWhinney (2005)

using a converging method: self-paced reading. This is important because

their stimulus presentation rate (300 ms per word; 350 ms blanks between

words) may have hampered beginning learners’ sentence comprehension

because of its speed. Self-paced reading avoids this concern and provides a

more complete measure of processing across the entire sentence, including

immediate disruptions, spillover effects, and/or wrap-up effects at sentence-

final words (Just & Carpenter, 1980). Additionally, the present study

explored an issue that McClain and Tokowicz (2006) raised in a reanalysis

of Tokowicz and MacWhinney, namely that grammaticality judgements were

related to the number of words following the critical word that agreed with it

in number and/or gender. This relationship suggests that beginning L2

learners’ offline judgements may be influenced not only by local agreement

between elements, but also by an accumulation of evidence across the entire

sentence.

We investigated L2 grammatical processing in native English speakers in

the first four semesters of university Spanish. We included four conditions

that represent three levels of similarity between morphosyntactic agreement

or licensing patterns in English and Spanish: Tokowicz and MacWhinney’s

(2005) three conditions and a second similar condition, demonstrative

determiner-noun number agreement.

1094 TOKOWICZ AND WARREN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

este

rn O

ntar

io]

at 1

2:32

21

Oct

ober

201

4

Page 6: Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

Similar2: Esa/*Esas clase empieza al mediodıa.

This/*These class begins at noon.

Participants read sentences with or without violations in a self-paced

word-by-word paradigm. After each, they judged its grammaticality and

rated their confidence in their judgement. Online sensitivity to violations was

evaluated by comparing reading times on the word at which the violationbecame evident in the ungrammatical conditions (critical word) to reading

times on the analogous word in the grammatical conditions. The word

following the critical word (n�1) was analysed to capture spill-over effects;

the sentence-final word was analysed to investigate sentence wrap-up. In an

exploratory manipulation, participants read either silently or aloud.

We expected participants to show online sensitivity to violations in similar

and unique constructions, given Tokowicz and MacWhinney’s (2005) findings

in a similar population. However, Jiang (2004, 2007) found that proficient L2learners failed to demonstrate sensitivity to morphosyntactic plurality cues in

self-paced reading. This insensitivity seems most likely due to properties of the

learners’ L1 (Chinese), but if learners in general are insensitive to the

morphosyntax of plurality, they should not detect violations in the different

and similar2 conditions. Tokowicz and MacWhinney’s learners did not show

sensitivity to violations in different constructions. If this was because learners

are slow to resolve competition between L1 and L2 cues, our self-paced

methodology might allow time for resolution and we might observe sensitivity.If learners’ processing is slow overall, sensitivity to violations might appear

only in later measures like spillover and final-word reading times. Alterna-

tively, if L2 learners’ processing is not slow, but fragile, such that newly

processed material harms or overwrites previously processed material, later

measures might show less sensitivity to violations than earlier measures.

METHOD

Design

We used a 2 modality (silent, aloud)�4 cross-language similarity (similar1,

similar2, different, unique)�2 grammaticality (grammatical, ungrammatical)

within-participants design.

Participants

Participants were 59 students enrolled in university Spanish courses (semesters

1�4) in Pittsburgh. They were required to be native English speakers with no

MORPHOSYNTACTIC VIOLATIONS IN L2 1095

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

este

rn O

ntar

io]

at 1

2:32

21

Oct

ober

201

4

Page 7: Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

exposure to other languages before age 13. Data from nine participants who

did not meet these criteria were excluded; data from two participants were lost.

Analyses were thus conducted on data from 48 participants. Ten participants

were in the first semester of Spanish, 12 were in each of the second and third

semesters, and 14 were in the fourth. Participants were compensated $15.

Materials

Four Spanish conditions represented three levels of cross-language similarity

(see earlier examples). In ‘‘similar1’’, verbs appeared in either the simple

present tense (grammatical) or in a progressive form that was ungrammatical

because a required auxiliary verb was absent. In ‘‘similar2’’, demonstrative

determiners did or did not agree in number with their head noun. In

‘‘different’’, definite or possessive determiners did or did not agree in

number with their head noun. In ‘‘unique’’, a determiner did or did not agree

in gender with its head noun. Three English conditions served as a baseline.

The first was like the ‘‘similar1’’ Spanish condition, and had either a licensed

simple or unlicensed progressive verb form. In the second English condition,

a reflexive anaphor did or did not agree in number with its antecedent (some

adapted from Osterhout & Mobley, 1995). The third English condition

involved subject-verb person agreement.

Verb aspect licensing: The kitten might scratch/*scratching the child.

Reflexive anaphor agreement: Mark used to talk to himself/*themselves often.

Subject-verb person agreement: Karen likes/*like to go to the zoo.

The position of the critical word varied across items. In conditions

manipulating determiners, the following noun was the critical word, because

that was the point at which the ungrammaticality became evident. For the

conditions manipulating verb aspect, the verb was the critical word. In

addition to the critical conditions, one English and three Spanish filler

conditions were included to increase variability in the grammatical construc-

tions presented. Each condition had a total of 16 items.1 One condition of each

item was assigned to each of four lists in a Latin-square design; these lists were

counterbalanced with respect to grammaticality and modality so that no

participant saw an item in more than one condition.

1 One item in ‘‘unique’’ had a typo and was excluded. Four items in English verb aspect

licensing were eliminated because in the ungrammatical conditions the critical verb could have been

interpreted grammatically, as beginning a reduced relative clause.

1096 TOKOWICZ AND WARREN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

este

rn O

ntar

io]

at 1

2:32

21

Oct

ober

201

4

Page 8: Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

Procedure

Participants first completed a version of the Waters, Caplan, and Hildebrandt

(1987) working-memory task. On each trial, an English sentence waspresented visually and participants made a binary judgement about its

plausibility. Participants were instructed to attempt to remember the final

word of each sentence. After each set of between two and six sentences, they

typed the final words they remembered from that set (order not being

important); four sets of each size were presented. Performance was indexed by

two measures: (1) set size span, or the set size at which the participant correctly

recalled all of the words on at least two of the four sets of that size, and (2) total

span, or the total number of words correctly recalled (out of 80).Participants then completed four blocks of self-paced reading trials2 in

one of two orders: Spanish-silent, English-silent, Spanish-aloud, English-

aloud; or Spanish-aloud, English-aloud, Spanish-silent, English-silent.

Participants were tape recorded during all blocks to verify silent reading

and to transcribe aloud reading.

A fixation cross appeared at the centre of the screen; the spacebar initiated

each trial and progressed the sentence word-by-word; all words appeared at the

centre of the screen. At the end of each sentence, a screen appeared asking ‘‘Isthis sentence acceptable?’’, to which the participant indicated ‘‘yes’’ or ‘‘no’’ by

pressing a button; participants were instructed to use grammaticality as the basis

for this judgement. Then, a screen appeared asking ‘‘How sure are you about

your decision?’’, to which the participant responded 0, 25, 50, 75, or 100% by

moving the mouse over the response and selecting the appropriate option.

Following this task, participants completed a language history ques-

tionnaire (Tokowicz, Michael, & Kroll, 2004) that gathered demographic

information and self-ratings of L1 and L2 reading, writing, speaking, andcomprehension proficiency. Participants also indicated all languages to

which they had been exposed and under what circumstances (Table 1).

RESULTS

Coding and data trimming

Critical word length was confounded with grammaticality in the similar1

condition, and word length and reading times were significantly correlated in

all blocks, rs�.17, .18, .46, and .32, for the English-aloud, English-silent,

Spanish-aloud, and Spanish-silent blocks, respectively, psB.01. Therefore,

reading times were length-adjusted (Ferreira & Clifton, 1986; Trueswell,

Tanenhaus, & Garnsey, 1994). Specifically, individual regression analyses

2 We used Linger software created by Doug Rohde to present the self-paced reading task.

MORPHOSYNTACTIC VIOLATIONS IN L2 1097

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

este

rn O

ntar

io]

at 1

2:32

21

Oct

ober

201

4

Page 9: Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

were performed for each participant in each block. Word length was regressed

from reading time; residual values were used in all reading time analyses.

Furthermore, we excluded residual reading times longer than 2000 ms from

analyses, accounting for 0.26, 0.56, 1.62, and 2.77% of the data in the English-

aloud, English-silent, Spanish-aloud, and Spanish-silent blocks, respectively.

Coding of aloud responses indicated that participants very rarely corrected

grammatical violations (0.7% of trials in English and 1.1% of trials in Spanish).

These trials were removed from analyses because corrections may affect readingtimes. Data from all other trialswere analysed regardless of judgement accuracy,

because previous studies have demonstrated dissociations between accuracy

and online sensitivity to violations (e.g., Tokowicz & MacWhinney, 2005).

Self-paced reading

English

Critical word. Critical word reading times were faster for grammatical

(M�3.34) than ungrammatical sentences (M�27.50), F1(1, 47)�46.58,

MSE�25506.15, pB.01, F2(1, 41)�30.63, MSE�12331.84, pB.01.

Additionally, there were theoretically uninteresting baseline differences

among conditions, F1(2, 94)�4.78, MSE�20201.17, pB.05, F2(2, 41)�3.28, MSE�7991.83, pB.05. An interaction between condition and

grammaticality was marginally reliable by participants, F1(2, 94)�3.10,

MSE�11154.66, p�.06,3 F2B1; the reflexive anaphor agreement condi-

tion showed the largest grammaticality effect.

TABLE 1Language history questionnaire data

Measure Results

Age (years) 18.9 (1.5)

Age began L2 (years) 14.5 (1.9)

L2 reading ability 5.8 (1.7)

L2 writing ability 5.3 (1.9)

L2 conversation ability 4.7 (1.9)

L2 speech comprehension ability 5.6 (2.1)

Standard deviations are given in parentheses. Reading, writing, conversational, and speech

comprehension ability were rated on a 10-point scale, with 1 indicating the lowest level of ability

and 10 indicating the highest level of ability. Most participants reported having no L2 or Spanish

as an L2, with two exceptions (one Arabic and one Hebrew). All but seven participants indicated

that they were not able to speak, read, write, or understand any language other than English or

Spanish (these participants’ answers were: ASL; French; French and Arabic; Greek; Portuguese

and Latin; Swedish).

3 Reported p and MSE values correspond to the Greenhouse-Geisser nonsphericity correction

for effects that violated the sphericity assumption.

1098 TOKOWICZ AND WARREN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

este

rn O

ntar

io]

at 1

2:32

21

Oct

ober

201

4

Page 10: Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

Word n�1. The main effect of grammaticality spilled over onto word n�1, with faster reading for grammatical (M��20.88) than ungrammatical

sentences (M�28.39), F1(1, 47)�13.62, MSE�25664.30, pB.01, F2(1,

41)�32.51, MSE�2867.69, pB.01. Word n�1 was also read more quickly

aloud (M��7.65) than silently (M�8.69), F1(1, 47)�4.38, pB.05,

F2(1, 41)�5.77, MSE�3881.12, pB.05.

Final word. Self-paced reading times for sentence-final words showed a

reversed main effect of grammaticality, with slower reading for grammatical

(M�41.99) than ungrammatical sentences (M��56.21), F1(1, 47)�19.29,

MSE�72001.44, pB.01, F2(1, 41)�38.11, MSE�5831.33, pB.01 (Figure 1).

Furthermore, grammaticality and modality interacted, F1(1, 47)�7.89,

MSE�53952.48, pB.01, F2(1, 41)�26.92, MSE�5265.39, pB.01. Simple-

effects tests indicated that the effect of grammaticality was significant in both

analyses for the silent modality, Fs�18.08, psB.01, but only by participants in

the aloud modality, F1�4.16, pB.05, F2B1.

Summary. Grammatical violations in L1 drove immediate disruption

that spilled over onto word n�1. Interestingly, this reading time pattern

reversed for the sentence-final word, suggesting that readers may have

engaged in more wrap-up processing (Just & Carpenter, 1980; Warren,

White, & Reichle, 2009) for grammatical than ungrammatical sentences. We

now turn to self-paced reading analyses for L2 Spanish.

Spanish

Critical word. Reading times on critical words were longer in ungram-

matical than grammatical sentences, F1(1, 46)�20.43, MSE�108723.39,

pB.01, F2(1, 59)�24.21, MSE�29559.99, pB.01. This effect was qualified

Figure 1. Length-adjusted reading times for final words in English sentences as a function of

modality and grammaticality.

MORPHOSYNTACTIC VIOLATIONS IN L2 1099

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

este

rn O

ntar

io]

at 1

2:32

21

Oct

ober

201

4

Page 11: Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

by an interaction with similarity condition, F1(3, 138)�2.93, MSE�82443.00, pB.05, F2(3, 59)�2.79, MSE�29559.99, pB.05 (Figure 2).

Simple-effects tests indicated a grammaticality effect for the similar1,

similar2, and different conditions, Fs�5.07, psB.05, but not for the unique

condition, FsB1. In the item analysis, modality and grammaticality

interacted, F1(1, 46)�2.26, MSE�110904.50, p�.14, F2(1, 59)�5.65,

MSE�14752.07, pB.05. Simple-effects tests on item means demonstrated

a larger grammaticality effect in the silent condition, F2aloud�8.68, pB.01;

F2silent�24.37, pB.01. Finally, there were uninteresting baseline differences

among conditions in the participant analysis, F1(3, 138)�7.95, MSE�103987.15, pB.014, F2(3, 59)�1.62, MSE�182443.62, p�.20.

Word n�1. Word n�1 was read more slowly in ungrammatical (M��26.14) than grammatical sentences (M��62.63), F1(1, 46)�4.68, MSE�53497.45, pB.05, F2(1, 55)�5.78, MSE�14350.72, pB.05. There were

uninteresting baseline differences among conditions in the participant analysis,

F1(3, 138)�4.13, MSE�65132.00, pB.01, F2(3, 55)�1.16, MSE�82911.39,

p�.33.

Final word. On the sentence-final word, similarity and grammaticality

interacted, F1(3, 135)�5.88, MSE�149881.15, pB.01, F2(3, 59)�8.86,

MSE�39980.06, pB.01 (Figure 3). Simple-effects tests indicated that the

grammaticality effect was significant only for the similar2 condition, Fs�

17.41, psB.01 (other FsB1). This interaction qualified a main effect of

grammaticality, F1(1, 45)�6.01, MSE�136982.54, pB.05, F2(1, 59)�5.35, MSE�39980.06, pB.05, and a main effect of similarity that was

uninterpretable given variation among the similarity conditions, F1(3,

135)�11.29, MSE�125222.05, pB.01, F2(3, 59)�3.41, MSE�138570.68, pB.05. Modality and similarity interacted, F1(3, 135)�2.83,

MSE�144108.97, pB.05, F2(3, 59)�3.41, MSE�51450.76, pB.05.

Simple-effects tests indicated that sentences were read more quickly silently

than aloud in only the similar2 condition, Fs�14.92, psB.01 (other FsB1).

This effect qualified a main effect of modality in the item analysis, F1(1, 45)�2.83, MSE�211803.27, p�.10, F2(1, 59)�4.800, MSE�51450.76,

pB.05.

Summary. Learners showed immediate disruption to grammatical

violations in L2 for the similar and different but not unique conditions.

On word n�1, disruption was evident for all conditions. Cross-language

similarity also modulated sensitivity to violations as evidenced by sentence-final

4 See Footnote 3.

1100 TOKOWICZ AND WARREN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

este

rn O

ntar

io]

at 1

2:32

21

Oct

ober

201

4

Page 12: Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

word reading times, which were longer in grammatical than ungrammatical

sentences in only the similar2 condition. Modality generally had limited

effects, but responses were sometimes faster in the silent conditions. We now

turn to offline responses.

Offline responses

English

Because the behavioural responses in the Spanish blocks were yes-biased,

analyses were performed on d?, a measure of response sensitivity that

corrects for response biases. d? scores range from 0 (no sensitivity/at-chance

performance) to 6.2 (perfect sensitivity). To facilitate comparison, we used

the same measure for English. By participants, responses to English

sentences varied as a function of condition, F1(2, 94)�3.11, MSE�1.22,

Figure 3. Length-adjusted reading times for final words in Spanish sentences as a function of

similarity condition and grammaticality.

Figure 2. Length-adjusted reading times for critical words in Spanish sentences as a function of

similarity condition and grammaticality.

MORPHOSYNTACTIC VIOLATIONS IN L2 1101

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

este

rn O

ntar

io]

at 1

2:32

21

Oct

ober

201

4

Page 13: Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

pB.05, F2B1; sensitivity was highest for the verb aspect licensing condition

(M�5.87), intermediate for subject-verb person agreement (M�5.59), and

lowest for reflexive anaphor agreement (M�5.48); however, these contrasts

did not reach significance in follow-up Duncan’s multiple-range tests. No

other effects were significant.

Spanish

Sensitivity was low overall, but was reliably affected by similarity

condition, F1(3, 141)�16.93, MSE�6.63, pB.01,5 F2(3, 58)�10.59,

MSE�3.55, pB.01. Duncan’s multiple-range tests indicated that perfor-

mance in the unique condition (M�0.55) was reliably worse than in the

other three conditions (Ms�3.23, 2.17, and 2.15, for similar1, similar2, and

different, respectively). Participants had higher confidence in their accurate

judgements (80.4%) than their inaccurate judgements (70.0%), t(48)�6.49,

pB.01. Analyses relating modality and/or similarity condition to confidence

were not possible because of missing cells.

To investigate the relationship between confidence and accuracy and the

amount of agreement information marked on words that followed the

critical word (e.g., McClain & Tokowicz, 2006), we correlated the number of

pieces of agreeing information from the manipulated dimension (number or

gender) with the average accuracy and average confidence rating for that

sentence. The following two examples demonstrate how we calculated pieces

of agreeing information:

El/*Los camion esta en el garaje.

TheSING/*ThePL truck is in the garage.

La/*Las caja esta llena de libros.

TheSING/*ThePL box is full of books.

In both sentences, the verb esta is marked as singular to agree in number

with the critical noun camion and represents one piece of agreeing

information. In the second sentence, the adjective llena is also marked as

singular to agree with the critical noun and represents a second piece of

agreeing information. To increase power, the correlations with pieces of

information collapsed across similarity and modality. However, they were

performed separately for the grammatical and ungrammatical sentences

(thus, we used accuracy rather than d?) because the information may have

been used differently in the presence versus absence of a grammatical

....

.... .....

5 See Footnote 3.

1102 TOKOWICZ AND WARREN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

este

rn O

ntar

io]

at 1

2:32

21

Oct

ober

201

4

Page 14: Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

violation. Ungrammatical sentences were responded to more accurately

when more agreeing information followed the critical word, r�.33, pB.05.

Ungrammatical sentences showed no relationship between agreement

information and confidence. Grammatical sentences showed no relationship

between agreement information and either accuracy or confidence. Interest-

ingly, of the three constructions with agreement violations, the unique

condition had the least agreeing information, which could explain the low

d? in that condition.

To examine the role of working memory in sensitivity to grammatical

violations, we correlated the two working memory measures with the

difference between critical word RTs to ungrammatical and grammatical

sentences and d?. These correlations were performed separately for the

similarity conditions. None were significant.

DISCUSSION

In this study, beginning adult learners were sensitive to grammatical

violations during reading in both L1 and L2; when a critical word cued a

grammaticality violation, reading times on the critical word and the

immediately following word were longer in both languages. In L2, this

held for conditions with morphosyntactic systems that were also present in

L1, but not for the system unique to L2. In both L1 and L2, sentence-final

reading times were longer for grammatical than ungrammatical sentences; in

L2 this effect was driven by the similar2 (demonstrative determiner-noun

number agreement) condition. Offline judgements indicated that partici-

pants were sensitive to grammaticality in L1 (M�5.64), but relatively

insensitive in L2 (M�2.12), particularly in the unique condition.

These results indicate that learners are immediately sensitive to violations

in morphosyntactic and verb aspect licensing systems common to L1 and

L2, but insensitive to violations in a system unique to L2*at least until word

n�1, although the effect there was only 18 ms for the unique construction.

Interestingly, immediate sensitivity in systems common to L1 and L2 is

present whether the systems implement agreement marking similarly or

differently. Sentence-final word reading times in the similar2 condition, but

no other L2 condition, showed the same distinction between grammaticality

and ungrammaticality present in the L1 conditions. This suggests that

learners process this morphological construction (demonstrative adjective-

noun number agreement) in a more native-like way than the other L2

constructions. However, the fact that offline grammaticality judgement

sensitivity was no better in this condition than two other L2 conditions, and

that judgement accuracy was generally poor, suggests that learners base their

MORPHOSYNTACTIC VIOLATIONS IN L2 1103

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

este

rn O

ntar

io]

at 1

2:32

21

Oct

ober

201

4

Page 15: Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

judgements on different information in the two languages, and that reading

times and end-of-sentence judgements measure different processes.The finding of reliably less sensitivity to L2 unique violations in offline

grammaticality judgements is consistent with previous research (e.g.,

Tokowicz & MacWhinney, 2005). One possible reason is that learners might

try to infer grammatical information from the gist representation available

after the sentence has been read (e.g., Potter & Lombardi, 1990). Because

grammatical gender violations in the unique condition, unlike the number or

aspect violations in all other conditions, have no impact on the sentence’s

semantic/conceptual representation, they should be least available from agist representation.

Learners’ immediate sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations, particu-

larly ones involving plurals, contradicts Jiang’s (2004, 2007) self-paced

reading findings in which native Chinese speakers did not show such

sensitivity in English (L2). Jiang argued that this finding was not related to

the rarity of plural morphology in Chinese, but rather indicated that plural

morphology is not an integratable L2 structure, despite generally early

instruction. The present findings weigh against this interpretation andsuggest that Jiang’s results were likely due to the fact that, for his Chinese

participants, plural morphology is essentially unique to English.

Because of concerns that binary grammaticality judgements may not be

sensitive enough to detect small differences in offline sensitivity to different

kinds of grammatical violations, we gathered learners’ confidence ratings in

those judgements. Although confidence was higher for correct judgements,

neither confidence nor accuracy correlated with the magnitude of sensitivity

to violations at the critical word. Interestingly, accuracy was higher whenthere was additional information in the sentence that agreed with the critical

word, suggesting that early learners used this information to inform their

judgements.

The current experiment included an exploratory manipulation of aloud

versus silent reading. There was no support for the hypothesis that reading

aloud increases monitoring and thus sensitivity to violations. Reading

modality did not affect offline judgement sensitivity, but aloud reading was

slower than silent reading for the similar2 condition at the final word, andgrammaticality effects were larger in the silent than aloud condition at the

critical word. Because reading aloud has an additional vocal component,

and requires coordination between button presses, speaking, and language

processing, it is not entirely surprising that sensitivity was reduced and reading

was slower in this modality, especially for the condition that was most

sensitive to grammaticality at the final word.

It is interesting to compare the findings of the current study to those of

Tokowicz and MacWhinney (2005). Both studies found online sensitivity toviolations in similar L2 conditions and very low offline sensitivity in the

1104 TOKOWICZ AND WARREN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

este

rn O

ntar

io]

at 1

2:32

21

Oct

ober

201

4

Page 16: Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

unique condition. However, whereas Tokowicz and MacWhinney also found

online sensitivity to the unique condition, the current study found it in thedifferent condition. A possible reason for the discrepancy in the unique

condition is that Tokowicz and MacWhinney’s critical nouns were more likely

to include an overt cue to their gender (the last letter ‘‘o’’ or ‘‘a’’) than those in

the current study. The discrepancy with respect to the different condition

could be related to the speed at which stimuli were presented. In the present

study, there was no external time pressure, perhaps allowing the online

measure to be more sensitive.

The results of the current experiment were not entirely consistent with theCompetition Model. This model predicts that sensitivity to violations should

be highest in the similar conditions, because there would be no competition

between languages, and lowest in the different condition, because competition

would impede processing. In the current study, participants showed online

sensitivity to violations regardless of whether they occurred in morphosyn-

tactic systems that were similar or different in L1 and L2. These findings

highlight the fact that, although broad generalisations about across-language

similarity are useful, other aspects of the Competition Model such as cuevalidity and reliability, exposure, and the kinds of information that cues carry

should be taken into account in concert with cross-language similarity.

Original manuscript received June 2009

Revised manuscript received September 2009

First published online July 2010

REFERENCES

Ferreira, F., & Clifton, C., Jr. (1986). The independence of syntactic processing. Journal of Memory

and Language, 25, 348�368.

Hahne, A., Mueller, J., & Clahsen, H. (2006). Morphological processing in a second language:

Behavioural and event-related potential evidence for storage and decomposition. Journal of

Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 121�134.

Jiang, N. (2004). Morphological insensitivity in second language processing. Applied Psycholin-

guistics, 25, 603�634.

Jiang, N. (2007). Selective integration of linguistic knowledge in adult second language learning.

Language Learning, 57, 1�33.

Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension.

Psychological Review, 87, 329�354.

Kotz, S. A. (2009). A critical review of ERP and fMRI evidence on L2 syntactic processing. Brain

and Language, 109, 68�74.

MacWhinney, B. (1997). Second language acquisition and the Competition Model. In A. M. B. de

Groot & J. F. Kroll (Eds.), Tutorials in bilingualism: Psycholinguistic perspectives (pp. 113�142).

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

MacWhinney, B. (2005). A unified model of language acquisition. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B.

De Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 49�67). Oxford,

UK: Oxford University Press.

MORPHOSYNTACTIC VIOLATIONS IN L2 1105

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

este

rn O

ntar

io]

at 1

2:32

21

Oct

ober

201

4

Page 17: Beginning adult L2 learners' sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study

McClain, R., & Tokowicz, N. (2006, July). Measuring sensitivity to Spanish gender and number

agreement: Overt judgments of grammaticality. Poster presented at the Pittsburgh Science of

Learning Center Summer intern poster session, Pittsburgh, PA.

McDonald, J. L. (1987). Sentence interpretation in bilingual speakers of English and Dutch.

Applied Linguistics, 8, 397�413.

Osterhout, L., & Mobley, L. A. (1995). Event-related brain potentials elicited by failure to agree.

Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 739�773.

Potter, M. C., & Lombardi, L. (1990). Regeneration in the short-term recall of sentences. Journal of

Memory and Language, 29, 633�654.

Rossi, S., Gugler, M. F., Friederici, A. D., & Hahne, A. (2006). The impact of proficiency on

syntactic second-language processing of German and Italian: Evidence from event-related

potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 2030�2048.

Sabourin, L., & Stowe, L. A. (2008). Second language processing: When are first and second

languages processed similarly? Second Language Research, 24, 397�430.

Tokowicz, N., & MacWhinney, B. (2005). Implicit and explicit measures of sensitivity to violations

in second language grammar: An event-related potential investigation. Studies in Second

Language Acquisition, 27, 173�204.

Tokowicz, N., Michael, E. B., & Kroll, J. F. (2004). The roles of study-abroad experience and

working-memory capacity in the types of errors made during translation. Bilingualism:

Language and Cognition, 7, 255�272.

Tolentino, L. C., & Tokowicz, N. (2009). Across languages, space, and time: The role of cross-

language similarity in L2 (morpho)syntactic processing as revealed by fMRI and ERP.

Manuscript under review.

Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Garnsey, S. M. (1994). Semantic influences on parsing: Use

of thematic role information in syntactic disambiguation. Journal of Memory and Language,

33, 285�318.

Van Hell, J. G., & Tokowicz, N. (in press). Event-related brain potentials and second language

learning: Syntactic processing in late L2 learners at different L2 proficiency levels. Second

Language Research.

Warren, T., White, S. J., & Reichle, E. D. (2009). Investigating the causes of wrap-up effects:

Evidence from eye movements and E-Z Reader. Cognition, 111, 132�137.

Waters, G., Caplan, D., & Hildebrandt, N. (1987). Working memory and written sentence

comprehension. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance XII: The psychology of

reading (pp. 531�555). Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd.

1106 TOKOWICZ AND WARREN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f W

este

rn O

ntar

io]

at 1

2:32

21

Oct

ober

201

4