10
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 19%, Vol. 22, No. 6,1354-1363 Copyright 19% by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/96/S3.00 Music-Dependent Memory: The Roles of Tempo Change and Mood Mediation William R. Balch The Pennsylvania State University, Altoona Benjamin S. Lewis The Pennsylvania State University, University Park Music-dependent memory was obtained in previous literature by changing from 1 musical piece to another. Here, the phenomenon was induced by changing only the tempo of the same musical selection. After being presented with a list of words, along with a piece of background music, listeners recalled more words when the selection was played at the same tempo than when it was played at a different tempo. However, no significant reduction in memory was produced by recall contexts with a changed timbre, a different musical selection, or no music (Experiments 1 and 2). Tempo was found to influence the arousal dimension of mood (Experiment 3), and recall was higher in a mood context consistent (as compared with inconsistent) with a given tempo (Experiment 4). The results support the mood-mediation hypothesis of music-dependent memory. Context-dependent memory (CDM) refers to a change in context or environment that causes some of the material learned in the original context to be forgotten. McGeoch (1932) first formally proposed the principle, calling it altered stimulus conditions. The CDM effect has been obtained with various context manipulations, including place (e.g., Godden & Baddely, 1975; Smith, 1979; Smith, Glenberg, & Bjork, 1978), olfactory cues (Cann & Ross, 1989; Schab, 1990), time of day (Holloway, 1978); gender of speaking voice (Geiselman & Glenny, 1977), alcohol or drug states (e.g., Eich, 1980), and mood states (e.g., Bower, Monteiro, & Gilligan, 1978; Eich, 1995b; Eich & Metcalfe, 1989; Lewis & Williams, 1989). Despite the variety and number of studies reporting CDM, however, the reliability of these effects has been questioned (Bjork & Richardson-Klavehn, 1989). For instance, Fernandez and Glenberg (1985) did not find place-dependent memory, and Bower and Mayer (1989) and Mueller, Grove, and Thompson (1991) did not report reliable mood-dependent memory effects. Background music has recently been found to affect memory, thus joining the list of CDM contexts (Balch, Bowman, & Mohler, 1992; Smith, 1985). In these studies, performed with typical CDM procedures, a series of words was presented along with a particular selection of music. Later, recall for the William R. Balch, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, Altoona; Benjamin S. Lewis, Department of Psychol- ogy, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park. We thank George Balch for assistance in the digitization and recording of the musical contexts used in this study; Paul Cornwell, Kevin Murnane, Lance Shotland, Mark Lafer, and Elaine Prestia for their help in securing student volunteers; John Johnson for helpful advice concerning the mood induction procedures of Experiment 4; Steven Smith, for useful suggestions regarding the manuscript; and Dave Myers, Amanda Yost, and Chris Papotto for conducting some of the experimental sessions. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to William R. Balch, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, 3000 Ivyside Park, Altoona, Pennsylvania 16601-3760. Electronic mail may be sent via Internet to [email protected]. words was tested. Participants recalled fewer words when a different piece as compared with the same piece was played. This type of CDM (i.e., music-dependent memory) is the particular concern of the present research. Our central pur- pose is to explain why music-dependent memory occurs. However, the key issues addressed in this study are also relevant to CDM effects obtained by the manipulation of other contexts, such as mood (e.g., Eich, 1995b) or place (e.g., Smith et al., 1978). The first issue addressed here is the identification and testing of specific contextual changes that might induce music- dependent memory. In previous studies, the CDM effect has generally been obtained with procedures involving a number of simultaneous changes in context. For instance, Balch et al. (1992) and Smith (1985) induced the effect by playing one musical selection—called the presentation or learning context— during presentation of the words and a different selection during recall. The presentation and recall contexts thus dif- fered in the sense of their overall identities and presumably in a multiplicity of specific musical features or dimensions. Some of these changes include differences in the tempo, the timbres of the instruments playing the music, the melodic phrases, the harmonic sequences, and probably the moods induced by the music. Compound differences have also been used in obtaining CDM effects with other contexts: In place-dependent memory studies, researchers have used different rooms with different furnishings (e.g., Smith, 1979), differently furnished rooms containing different olfactory cues (Dalton, 1993), or different geographical environments such as land versus underwater (e.g., Godden & Baddely, 1975), and in mood-dependent memory studies, researchers have used different pieces of music expressing different moods and accompanied by differ- ent mood instructions (e.g., Eich, Macaulay, & Ryan, 1994; Eich & Metcalfe, 1989). 1 1 Regarding the mood-dependent memory studies cited, multiple- feature differences are involved with respect to the procedures and stimuli used in establishing the moods, though not necessarily with respect to the internal mood states themselves. 1354

Balch Lewis

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Balch Lewis

Citation preview

  • Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition19%, Vol. 22, No. 6,1354-1363

    Copyright 19% by the American Psychological Association, Inc.0278-7393/96/S3.00

    Music-Dependent Memory:The Roles of Tempo Change and Mood MediationWilliam R. Balch

    The Pennsylvania State University, AltoonaBenjamin S. Lewis

    The Pennsylvania State University, University Park

    Music-dependent memory was obtained in previous literature by changing from 1 musical piece toanother. Here, the phenomenon was induced by changing only the tempo of the same musicalselection. After being presented with a list of words, along with a piece of background music,listeners recalled more words when the selection was played at the same tempo than when it wasplayed at a different tempo. However, no significant reduction in memory was produced by recallcontexts with a changed timbre, a different musical selection, or no music (Experiments 1 and 2).Tempo was found to influence the arousal dimension of mood (Experiment 3), and recall washigher in a mood context consistent (as compared with inconsistent) with a given tempo(Experiment 4). The results support the mood-mediation hypothesis of music-dependent memory.

    Context-dependent memory (CDM) refers to a change incontext or environment that causes some of the materiallearned in the original context to be forgotten. McGeoch(1932) first formally proposed the principle, calling it alteredstimulus conditions. The CDM effect has been obtained withvarious context manipulations, including place (e.g., Godden& Baddely, 1975; Smith, 1979; Smith, Glenberg, & Bjork,1978), olfactory cues (Cann & Ross, 1989; Schab, 1990), timeof day (Holloway, 1978); gender of speaking voice (Geiselman& Glenny, 1977), alcohol or drug states (e.g., Eich, 1980), andmood states (e.g., Bower, Monteiro, & Gilligan, 1978; Eich,1995b; Eich & Metcalfe, 1989; Lewis & Williams, 1989).Despite the variety and number of studies reporting CDM,however, the reliability of these effects has been questioned(Bjork & Richardson-Klavehn, 1989). For instance, Fernandezand Glenberg (1985) did not find place-dependent memory,and Bower and Mayer (1989) and Mueller, Grove, andThompson (1991) did not report reliable mood-dependentmemory effects.

    Background music has recently been found to affect memory,thus joining the list of CDM contexts (Balch, Bowman, &Mohler, 1992; Smith, 1985). In these studies, performed withtypical CDM procedures, a series of words was presentedalong with a particular selection of music. Later, recall for the

    William R. Balch, Department of Psychology, The PennsylvaniaState University, Altoona; Benjamin S. Lewis, Department of Psychol-ogy, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park.

    We thank George Balch for assistance in the digitization andrecording of the musical contexts used in this study; Paul Cornwell,Kevin Murnane, Lance Shotland, Mark Lafer, and Elaine Prestia fortheir help in securing student volunteers; John Johnson for helpfuladvice concerning the mood induction procedures of Experiment 4;Steven Smith, for useful suggestions regarding the manuscript; andDave Myers, Amanda Yost, and Chris Papotto for conducting some ofthe experimental sessions.

    Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed toWilliam R. Balch, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania StateUniversity, 3000 Ivyside Park, Altoona, Pennsylvania 16601-3760.Electronic mail may be sent via Internet to [email protected].

    words was tested. Participants recalled fewer words when adifferent piece as compared with the same piece was played.

    This type of CDM (i.e., music-dependent memory) is theparticular concern of the present research. Our central pur-pose is to explain why music-dependent memory occurs.However, the key issues addressed in this study are alsorelevant to CDM effects obtained by the manipulation of othercontexts, such as mood (e.g., Eich, 1995b) or place (e.g., Smithet al., 1978).

    The first issue addressed here is the identification andtesting of specific contextual changes that might induce music-dependent memory. In previous studies, the CDM effect hasgenerally been obtained with procedures involving a number ofsimultaneous changes in context. For instance, Balch et al.(1992) and Smith (1985) induced the effect by playing onemusical selectioncalled the presentation or learning contextduring presentation of the words and a different selectionduring recall. The presentation and recall contexts thus dif-fered in the sense of their overall identities and presumably ina multiplicity of specific musical features or dimensions. Someof these changes include differences in the tempo, the timbresof the instruments playing the music, the melodic phrases, theharmonic sequences, and probably the moods induced by themusic.

    Compound differences have also been used in obtainingCDM effects with other contexts: In place-dependent memorystudies, researchers have used different rooms with differentfurnishings (e.g., Smith, 1979), differently furnished roomscontaining different olfactory cues (Dalton, 1993), or differentgeographical environments such as land versus underwater(e.g., Godden & Baddely, 1975), and in mood-dependentmemory studies, researchers have used different pieces ofmusic expressing different moods and accompanied by differ-ent mood instructions (e.g., Eich, Macaulay, & Ryan, 1994;Eich & Metcalfe, 1989).1

    1 Regarding the mood-dependent memory studies cited, multiple-feature differences are involved with respect to the procedures andstimuli used in establishing the moods, though not necessarily withrespect to the internal mood states themselves.

    1354

  • MUSIC-DEPENDENT MEMORY 1355

    The use of these multiple-feature changes in CDM studies isconsistent with the theoretical view of context as the combinedcontributions of several sources of information (e.g., Bower,1972; Tulving, 1983). This view has recently been called themental-context hypothesis (Smith, 1995). One implication of thishypothesis is that the more contextual changes that are made,the greater the CDM effect. In regard to this point, Eich et al.(1994, p. 203) have suggested that simultaneous changes in twoeffective dimensions of mood (e.g., pleasantness and arousal)may increase mood-dependent memory, as compared with theeffect induced by either single-dimensional change. However,they note that so far this hypothesis has been based mainly oncorrelational evidence (Eich & Metcalfe, 1989; Eich et al.,1994).

    Although varying several aspects of context simultaneouslyis possibly the most effective method of inducing CDM, thisprocedure does not clarify what manipulations might besufficient causes of CDM. One such manipulation might be achange in tempo. For instance, Balch et al. (1992) found thatchanging to a different piece having a different tempo loweredrecall (compared with using the same musical selection).However, changing to a different piece having the same tempodid not induce this effect.

    Therefore, tempo may be a specific dimension closelyassociated with music-dependent memory. However, moredirect evidence for this hypothesis would be obtained if CDMcould be induced by an experimentally controlled tempochange. To this end, Experiments 1 and 2 both include (a) asame-context condition, in which the same piece is played at thesame tempo during recall and (b) a different-tempo context, inwhich the identical selection is played at a faster or slowertempo. A recall difference between these two conditions wouldshow that a controlled tempo change is sufficient to inducemusic-dependent memory. In other words, tempo-dependentmemory would be demonstrated.

    Several other contextual manipulations are also tested fortheir effects on recall. Experiment 1 includes a different-selection context, in which a different piece is played at the sametempo during recall. If recall is lower in this context than in thesame-context condition, then a change in overall musicalcontextwithout altering tempocan induce music-depen-dent memory. This type of change was not found to be effectiveby Balch et al. (1992) but is tested again here.

    In Experiment 2, a different-timbre condition is introduced toisolate and test the effect of a change in timbre on music-dependent memory. For instance, a selection originally playedwith a piano timbre is played with a brass timbre. As in thedifferent-tempo condition, the different-timbre context repre-sents a controlled change in a single musical dimension.However, tempo and timbre changes need not influence recallin the same way. Therefore, both tempo-dependent andtimbre-dependent memory are tested in Experiment 2.

    In addition, a no-context condition is included in this experi-ment. This type of context change is a test for what Smith(198S) has called contextual cuing, referring to superior recallobtained with a same-context condition as opposed to a nullrecall context. Although most CDM studies have reportedbetter memory for the same (as compared with a different)context, it has been more difficult to show that recalling in the

    same context serves as a facilitative recall cue and actuallyimproves memory relative to a no-context control (Rovee-Collier, Earley, & Stafford, 1989, p. 149; Ucros, 1989). Forbackground-music contexts, contextual cuing was found bySmith (1985) but not by Balch et al. (1992) or by Thaut and del'Etoile (1993).

    A second issue was motivated by our finding tempo-dependent memory in Experiments 1 and 2. This issue relatesto the mechanisms that might underlie the effect of tempochanges on recall. We focus on the hypothesis that tempochanges may induce mood alterations and that these mooddifferences may in turn induce CDM. A similar viewpoint hasbeen expressed in connection with other CDM effects and isknown as the mood-mediation hypothesis (Bower, 1981; Eich,1995a; Eich & Birnbaum, 1988). This hypothesis, along withother possible mechanisms, is tested in Experiments 3 and 4.

    Experiment 1Experiment 1 compared word recall after a 1-min retention

    interval under three recall-context conditions: same context(identical to the presentation context), different tempo (thesame piece played at a different tempo than during presenta-tion), and different selection (a different piece played at thesame tempo as during presentation). If fewer words wererecalled with the different-tempo context than with the samecontext, this result would demonstrate tempo-dependentmemory and indicate that changing tempo is a sufficientcondition for CDM. If fewer words were recalled with thedifferent-selection context than with same context, this findingwould suggest that changing a variety of other musical features(i.e., musical selection) can induce music-dependent memory.

    MethodParticipants. Volunteers for this experiment were 168 undergradu-

    ates enrolled in introductory psychology courses at the Altoona andUniversity Park campuses of The Pennsylvania State University. Equalnumbers of students from each campus made up each experimentalgroup, and all participants received a small amount of academic creditfor their services.

    Materials and apparatus. The words used for all conditions of thisexperiment were 24 common (A-frequency) two- and three-syllablenouns taken from Spreen and Schulz's (1966) norms (e.g., furniture,cousin, etc.). All words were originally presented together with one oftwo selections of background music: an excerpt (Mozart, 1953) fromthe Rondo (third movement) of a Mozart piano sonata in C major (K.309) or "Jazz Holiday," a piano composition (Nevin, 1957). Thesepieces are designated here as the classical and jazz selections,respectively. We chose pieces representing two different genresbecause we wished to use selections that were not likely to soundsimilar to participants in the different-selection condition. In addition,neither piece was considered likely to have been heard frequently bymost of the participants in the experiment.

    These selections were originally played by William R. Balch on aKorg SG-1D keyboard and digitized on a 386-SX computer withsequencing software (Master Tracks Pro 5). For both selections, apiano timbre was simulated by means of a Roland U-220 SoundModule (timbre program 001 on that unit). Each piece was played in Cmajor (requiring no transcription of the classical [C] piece butrequiring the jazz [J] piece to be transcribed one whole tone higher by

  • 1356 BALCH AND LEWIS

    computer). Two audiocassette recordings were made from eachselection, one at a slow (S) tempo (60 quarter notes per minute) andthe other at a fast (F) tempo (140 quarter notes per minute). Allrecordings had a duration slightly longer than the time required forword presentation (250 s). If the end of a selection occurred duringrecording, the computer continued playing from the beginning.

    Thus, four recordings were used in this experiment: SC, FC, SJ, andFJ. In comparing the fast and slow versions of either the classical (SCvs. FC) or the jazz (SJ vs. FJ) selection, note that each version wasidentical except for the tempo.

    Design. The first two independent variables were selection (classi-cal [C] or jazz [J]) and tempo (slow [S] or fast [F]). Together, thesevariables generated the four possible presentation contexts: SC, FC,SJ, or FJ. In other words, each presentation context was based oneither the classical or the jazz selection, played at either the slow or thefast tempo (60 or 140 quarter notes per minute, respectively).

    The third independent variable was recall context. This variableconsisted of three conditions: same context, different tempo, ordifferent selection. For the same-context condition, the same record-ing was used for both the presentation and recall contexts. For thedifferent-tempo condition, the recordings used for these contexts werebased on the same selection but different tempos. For the different-selection condition, on the other hand, presentation and recallcontexts were based on a different piece played at the same tempo.

    Fourteen different participants were randomly assigned to each ofthe 12 experimental groups. In terms of presentation and recallcontexts, the four same-context groups can be designated as SC-SC,FC-FC, SJ-SJ, and FJ-FJ. The four different-tempo groups wereSC-FC, FC-SC, SJ-FJ, and FJ-SJ. Finally, the four different-selectiongroups were SC-SJ, FC-FJ, SJ-SC, and FJ-FC.

    The dependent variable was the number of words (out of 24)correctly recalled during the recall phase of the experiment.

    Procedure. An individual session was conducted with each partici-pant, who was told that the purpose of the experiment was to ratewords for pleasantness. Participants were then given a pencil and abooklet for rating the words on a 6-point scale: 1 for very unpleasant, 2for moderately unpleasant, 3 for slightly unpleasant, 4 for slightlypleasant, 5 for moderately pleasant, and 6 for very pleasant. To make therating task more enjoyable, participants were told, background musicwould be played while the words were shown.

    Typed words were presented visually on index cards, 1 word percard. To help give the participants sufficient exposure to the material,two different random orders of the 24-word list were run consecutivelyto generate a complete 48-word sequence. During the instructions,participants had been informed that each word would be repeatedsomewhere in the sequence and that they should rate each presentedword according to their impression of its pleasantness at the moment.One of two different 48-word sequences was assigned to half theparticipants in each group.

    After the instructions, the experimenter started the recording of theassigned musical context. After 10 s, he or she began showing words atthe rate of one every S s. This presentation phase lasted about 250 s.The musical recording was stopped at the end of presentation, and theparticipant's rating booklet was taken.

    Next, a piece of "distraction music" was played: a techniqueadopted from Balch et al.'s (1992) study (their Experiment 3). Thisprocedure was intended to help equate the otherwise differentdistraction levels that might be involved in the various recall-contextconditions. Immediately changing to a different context (as in thedifferent-tempo and different-selection conditions) might have startledor distracted participants more than playing the identical recordingagain (as in the same-context condition). This extra distraction mighthave disrupted attention and impaired recall performance in a manneruninteresting from a memory standpoint. An intentionally distractingpiece inserted between presentation and recall, however, should have

    helped eliminate such differences in the degrees of distraction pro-duced by the recall contexts.

    The distraction piece was an excerpt from Shirabe-Sagaribe (1980).(The title is translated, "The Sound of Wind Through BambooLeaves.") This essentially atonal music was played on several orientalinstruments, including a bamboo flute, and was chosen to be distract-ingly different from both the classical and jazz contexts.

    After 30 s of distraction music, the recall-context recording wasstarted. Participants were handed a piece of paper and asked to writedown, in any order, as many of the presented words as they couldrecall. The interval between the end of presentation and the beginningof recall was about 1 min. Two minutes were allowed for the recallsession itself, and then participants were debriefed.

    Results and DiscussionAn alpha level of .05 was used for the statistical tests

    reported in all the present experiments.The results of Experiment 1 are illustrated in Table 1, which

    gives the mean recall scores and standard deviations for thevarious conditions. The table shows the scores for the threerecall contexts: the same context, the different-tempo context,and the different-selection context. Average recall across thepresentation conditions was highest for the same context(12.38), lowest for the different tempo (10.98), and intermedi-ate for the different selection (11.75). This outcome appears tobe consistent with the hypothesis that a tempo change per se issufficient to induce music-dependent memory.

    To check the statistical significance of these results, 2 x 2 x3 (Selection x Tempo x Recall Context) independent-groupsanalysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed. As expected,the main effect of recall context was significant, F(2, 156) =6.83, MSE = 3.99, p < .01. Neither of the other main effectsand none of the interactions were significant (ps > .10).

    The music-dependent memory effects can be assessed bymultiple Bonferroni comparisons between the same-contextmean and each of the other recall-context means. The 1.40

    Table 1Recall Scores for the Contexts in Experiment 1

    Presentationcontext"

    SlowMSD

    FastMSD

    SlowMSD

    FastMSD

    Samecontext

    Recall contextDifferent

    tempoClassical selection

    12.501.95

    13.071.69

    11.211.72

    10.351.45

    Jazz selection

    12.352.06

    11.571.60

    11.211.72

    11.072.13

    Differentselection

    11.641.60

    12.791.67

    11.861.84

    10.712.76

    Note. Each participant's recall score was the number of correctwords out of 24; n = 14 for each entry.aAll presentation contexts were played with the piano timbre.

  • MUSIC-DEPENDENT MEMORY 1357

    difference in recall between the same-context (12.38) anddifferent-tempo conditions (10.98) was significant, '(165) =3.73, p < .002. However, the 0.63 difference between recall inthe former-context and different-selection conditions (11.75)was not significant, f (165) = 1.73,p > .10.

    Thus, changing tempo induced music-dependent memory.However, changing to a different selection played at the sametempo did not. Tempo, in particular, appears to be a dimen-sion of musical context that has significant memory conse-quences. However, because Experiment 1 was designed to testthe individual effects of changing tempo or selection, nostatistical inference is made concerning the comparison be-tween the tempo-dependent and selection-dependent effects.

    Experiment 2In Experiment 2, word recall in the same-context condition

    of Experiment 1 was compared with recall under each of threechanged contexts. First, the different-tempo context, whichwas found to reduce recall in Experiment 1, was tested again.This time, however, a second controlled and single-dimen-sional change was also tested: different timbre (in which onlytimbre was altered). To test for contextual cuing, a furthercontext manipulation was introduced: a no-context condition(in which no musical selection was played during recall).

    A significant drop in recall under the different-tempocontext would replicate the finding obtained in Experiment 1that changing tempo is a sufficient cause for music-dependentmemory. Decreased recall under the different-timbre contextwould show an analogous effect for a timbre change. Finally, amemory decrement under the no-context condition woulddemonstrate contextual cuing.

    MethodParticipants. Participants were 128 undergraduates who took part

    in this experiment for extra credit. As in Experiment 1, half of theparticipants in each condition were from the Altoona campus of ThePennsylvania State University, and half were from the University Parkcampus.

    Materials and apparatus. The same word list, musical selections,and tempos used in Experiment 1 were applied again in Experiment 2.Likewise, the same four recorded contexts used in the previousexperimentall in the piano timbrewere used here. These contextswere designated SC-P, SJ-P, and so forth (P indicates the pianotimbre). An additional set of four comparable contexts was recorded ina brass timbre created by the Roland U-220 Sound Module (timbreprogram 042 on that unit): SC-B, SJ-B, and so forth (B indicates thebrass timbre).

    Design. Because of the introduction of the brass timbre, thepresentation-context variables were expanded from two in Experiment1 to three in Experiment 2: selection (classical [C] or jazz [J]), tempo(slow [S] or fast [F]), and timbre (piano [P] or brass [B]). Thus, eightpresentation contexts were generated: SC-P, SC-B, FC-P, FC-B,SJ-P, SJ-B, FJ-P, and FJ-B.

    The recall-context variable included two contexts from Experiment1 (same context and different tempo) and two additional contexts(different timbre and no context). These conditions were explained inthe introduction to Experiment 2 and are briefly illustrated now. Thepresentation-context/recall-context sequences were as follows: for theeight same-context groups: SC-P/SC-P, SC-B/SC-B, and so forth; forthe different-tempo context: SC-P/FC-P, SC-B/FC-B, and so forth;

    for the different-timbre context: SC-P/SC-B, SC-B/SC-P, and soforth; and for the no-context condition: SC-P/N, SC-B/N, and soforth (N designates no musical selection during recall).

    Four different participants were randomly assigned to each of the 32combinations of the eight presentation contexts and four recallcontexts.

    Procedure. All of the same procedures used for conducting experi-mental sessions in Experiment 1 were applied in Experiment 2. In theno-context condition, though, no selection was played during recall.The retention interval for all conditions was again 1 min, and the samedistraction music was used.

    Results and DiscussionA preliminary 2 x 2 x 2 x 4 (Selection x Tempo x

    Timbre x Recall Context) independent-groups ANOVA re-vealed that musical selection was not significant as a maineffect, nor was it included in any significant interactions withthe other variables (ps > .10). Therefore, the results werecollapsed across musical selections.

    Table 2 gives the mean recall scores and standard deviationsfor the conditions in Experiment 2. The table shows the scoresfor the four recall contexts: the same-context, the different-tempo context, the different-timbre context, and the no-context conditions. Average recall across the presentationconditions was higher for the same context (12.06) than for thedifferent-tempo context (10.19). These scores appear to bereasonably similar to the comparable numbers found in Experi-ment 1 (12.38 and 10.98, respectively). Again, changing onlytempo seems to be sufficient to induce music-dependentmemory. However, average recall in the different-timbre(12.59) and no-context conditions (12.53) was about the sameas that obtained for the same context (12.06). Therefore, thereseems to be no evidence for the timbre-dependent memory orcontextual-cuing effects.

    To test the statistical significance of the results, a 2 x 2 x 4(Tempo x Timbre x Recall Context) independent-groupsANOVA was performed. As expected, the main effect of recallcontext was significant, F(3, 112) = 10.49, MSE = 3.89, p < .01. However, recall in theactive mood was significantly better for the fast (10.75) than forthe slow (9.08) presentation tempo, F(l, 22) = 7.45, MSE =2.24,p < .05. (See Table 4.)

    This pattern of results is consistent with the hypothesis thattempo-dependent memory is mediated by mood. Apparently,

    Table 4Recall Scores for the Tempo and Mood Contexts in Experiment 4

    MoodRelaxed

    MSD

    ActiveMSD

    Slow

    12.411.989.081.08

    TempoFast

    9.332.61

    10.751.82

    Note. Words were presented in a musical-tempo context and recalledin a mood context; n = 12 for each condition.

  • MUSIC-DEPENDENT MEMORY 1361participants recall more when they are put into a mood thatmatches, rather than mismatches, the mood induced by thetempo of the musical presentation context. This experimentshows that the recall context need not be musical, as inExperiments 1 and 2. Thus, what underlies the CDM effectfound in Experiment 4 is mood, rather than musical variablessuch as phrase repetition or beats per minute. Note that thelatter was specified as the operative variable in the temporal-frame hypothesis.

    General DiscussionConsidered together, the present experiments support the

    mood-mediation hypothesis of music-dependent memory. Ex-periments 1 and 2 show that changing tempo, but not musicalselection (Experiment 1) or timbre (Experiment 2), signifi-cantly reduces recall. In Experiment 3, tempobut again, notselection or timbrewas found to influence the arousaldimension of mood. Experiment 4 linked tempo and moodtogether in terms of memory. Tempo was manipulated in themusical presentation context; however, in recall, a moodcontext was manipulated strictly by verbal means. Duringrecall, more was remembered in mood contexts that matchedthe moods of the original presentation tempos. Therefore, thisCDM effect was mediated by the one presumably commonproperty of the matching presentation and recall contexts:mood arousal.

    In explaining CDM effects, an alternative to the mood-mediation view is the mental-context hypothesis. According toSmith (1995), this latter hypothesis considers "mood, place,mental set and other factors as components of one's mentalcontext, any of which can serve to cue the representation ofmental context at test" (p. 309). According to this view, anyone or a combination of many specific contextual changescould be a sufficient cause of CDM.

    The tempo manipulation, tested in Experiments 1 and 2,significantly influenced recall. Thus, tempo change is clearly asufficient cause of music-dependent memory. However, we didnot induce CDM with the other contextual changes. Thus, ourresults provide no evidence that music-dependent memory canhave multiple causes, an idea expressed by the mental-contexthypothesis.

    We acknowledge that there may be causes of this CDMeffectother than tempothat we simply have not found.Even variations of the timbre or the selection changes that wetested here might have the potential to induce music-dependent memory. Though the brass and piano timbressounded quite different to us, for instance, there may be othercontrasting timbres that would have induced stronger CDM.Moreover, the two tempos used here were purposely chosen tobe very distinct (60 vs. 140 beats per minute), and the contrastsinvolved in our other musical variables may not have beenfunctionally equivalent in every relevant way. The 60 beats-per-minute pace may also have rendered the slow-tempo contextsless discriminable from each other, compared with the greaterdistinctiveness of each fast context. Note that the fast tempo iscloser to those tempos originally intended for the jazz andclassical compositions used here.

    However, we can cite one type of functional equivalence. In

    Experiment 3, all of the musical contexts were rated about thesame in the mood pleasantness they induced. Pleasantnessrepresents one type of encoding that could be relevant to thepresent memory task, especially because the words to beremembered were rated for pleasantness during presentation.Yet there were no obvious differences in the effects of tempo,timbre, or selection on the mood-pleasantness ratings of themusical contexts.

    Still, we acknowledge that there may be changes in timbre,selection, or other musical characteristics that would be morecomparable with the tempo manipulation we used. Therefore,we conclude only that the tempo change used here is a singleand sufficient manipulation that induces music-dependentmemory. We do not claim that this manipulation is necessaryto induce the effect. By the same token, we do not dismiss thenotion that several simultaneous contextual changes mightinduce greater CDM than any single change (Eich et al., 1994,p. 203). More research in the various CDM areas needs to beaddressed to systematic changes in context, both single andmultiple.

    Though tempo change may be an external cause of music-dependent memory, Experiments 3 and 4 show that this effectseems to be best explained in terms of an internal mediator:mood arousal. In addition, Experiment 4 helps resolve an issueraised by Smith (1995) concerning the mood-mediation hypoth-esis. He points out an ambiguity in Eich's (1995a) research onthe mediation of place-dependent memory by the happy-saddimension of mood (Eich's Experiment 3). Because Eich usedonly one presentation context (a pleasant place), recall in themismatched-mood context might have been worse because ofthe sad-mood procedure rather than because of the mis-matched mood (Smith, 1995, p. 309). However, in the presentExperiment 4, presentation tempos and recall moods werecounterbalanced. Thus, the CDM we found appears to arisefrom matched or mismatched moods rather than from theparticular recall contexts used.

    In light of the above consideration, our findings clarify andextend Eich's (1995a) support of the mood-mediation hypoth-esis. Note that Eich was concerned with place-dependentmemory and an evaluative dimension of mood (i.e., pleasant-happy vs. unpleasant-sad). Our own experiments have beenfocused on tempo change and the arousal dimension of mood.If mood mediation proves to be the key mechanism for avariety of CDM effects, it seems plausible that the particularmood dimension underlying any given effect will depend on thetype of CDM in question.

    The present findings also have some implications concerningthe question of reliability, which has plagued most CDMeffects (e.g., Bjork & Richardson-Klavehn, 1989). For in-stance, Eich (1995a) has summarized the phenomena ofplace-dependent and mood-dependent memory as having had"mercurial histories, with the mostly positive results reportedin the 1970s giving way to mostly negative results in the 1980s,leading theorists in the 1990s to wonder whether either PDM[place-dependent memory] or MDM [mood-dependentmemory] even exists" (p. 305).

    The mood-mediation view, which is supported by the pre-sent research, helps explain such reliability problems. Forinstance, several studies have not found place-dependent

  • 1362 BALCH AND LEWIS

    memory (e.g., Fernandez & Glenberg, 1985). However, in anearly study by Godden and Baddely (1975), a 46% difference inrecall between same- and different-place contexts was ob-tained with scuba divers as participants. This large effect maywell have been due to the choice of contexts: on land orunderwater. Because these two locations were likely to haveevoked very different moods, place-dependent memory couldhave been mediated by this difference. (See Eich, 1995a, for amore detailed discussion of this point.)

    Certain reliability problems have also surfaced in music-dependent memory research. For instance, contextual cuing(i.e., a recall difference between same-music and no-musiccontexts) was found by Smith (1985). However, no contextualcuing for background music was found in the present Experi-ment 2 or in the studies by Balch et al. (1992) and Thaut and del'Etoile (1993).

    Mood mediation may explain why contextual cuing has beendifficult to demonstrate with background music. Although adifferent-music condition could disrupt mood and induceCDM, the no-music recall context should not prevent partici-pants from mentally reinstating the mood they had experi-enced during the presentation music. Therefore, this nullcontextunlike a different-music contextshould not inter-fere with recall. In other words, contextual cuing would not beexpected. This interpretation seems plausible in light of recenttheoretical discussions of spontaneous context reinstatement(e.g., Bjork & Richardson-Klavehn, 1989).

    Reliability has generally not been a problem for music-dependent memory induced by different-music contexts. Thiseffect, obtained in the present Experiments 1 and 2, reinforcessimilar effects found by both Smith (1985) and Balch et al.(1992). Different-music contexts have apparently not inducedCDM only when the tempo was purposely kept the same asthat of the presentation music, as in the following cases: thedifferent-selection context of Experiment 1, the different-timbre context of Experiment 2, and the same-tempo contextin Balch et al.'s (1992) study in which a different selection wasplayed at the original presentation tempo (their Experiment2). The preceding observations are consistent with the viewthat tempo change and mood mediation figure importantly inmusic-dependent memory.

    To what degree is the present research congruent witheveryday examples of music-dependent memory? Our resultsseem generally consistent with several aspects of practicalmusical experience. For instance, consider the familiar notionthat old songs brings back associated memories. In this case,musical context appears to have memory consequences thatoccur in everyday life as well as in experimental studies. Insongs, music can also influence the recognition of lyrics(Serafine, Crowder, & Repp, 1984; Serafine, Davidson, Crow-der, & Repp, 1986), illustrating a related type of memoryconsequence.

    One aspect of musical experience is more difficult to explainin terms of experimental research. In some naturally occurringcases, music-dependent memory may involve the kind ofcontextual cuing not found in the present Experiment 2 or inmost CDM studies. As already mentioned, decreases inmemory due to an altered context have been easier todemonstrate than facilitation due to the same context. Yet

    sometimes a few bars of an old melody appear to cue memoriesthat might have been difficult to retrieve otherwise. If so,several influences might contribute to this phenomenon: manypairings of the musical context and the associated material tobe remembered, for instance, or the personal significance ofthe music and material to the individual involved. The range ofconditions possible in natural occurrences of CDM far exceedseven the broad variety now encountered in laboratory studies.

    ReferencesBalch, W. R., Bowman, K., & Mohler, L. A. (1992). Music-dependent

    memory in immediate and delayed recall. Memory & Cognition, 20,21-28.

    Bjork, R. A., & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (1989). On the puzzlingrelationship between environmental context and human memory. InC. Izawa (Ed.), Current issues in cognitive processes (pp. 313-344).Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Bower, G. H. (1972). Stimulus-sampling theory of encoding variability.In A. W. Melton & E. Martin (Eds.), Coding processes in humanmemory (pp. 85-124). Washington, DC: Winston.

    Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36,129-148.

    Bower, G. H., & Mayer, J. D. (1989). In search of mood-dependentretrieval. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 4, 133-168.

    Bower, G. H., Monteiro, K. P., & Gilligan, S. G. (1978). Emotionalmood as a context for learning and recall. Journal of Verbal Learningand Verbal Behavior, 17, 408-417.

    Cann, A., & Ross, D. A. (1989). Olfactory stimuli as context cues inhuman memory. American Journal of Psychology, 102, 91-102.

    Dalton, P. (1993). The role of stimulus familiarity in context-dependent recognition. Memory & Cognition, 21, 223-234.

    Eich, E. (1980). The cue-dependent nature of state-dependent re-trieval. Memory & Cognition, 8, 157-173.

    Eich, E. (1995a). Mood as a mediator of place dependent memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 293-308.

    Eich, E. (1995b). Searching for mood dependent memory. Psychologi-cal Science, 6, 67-75.

    Eich, E., & Birnbaum, I. M. (1988). On the relationship between thedissociative and evaluative properties of drugs. In G. M. Davies &D. M. Thomson (Eds.), Memory in context: Context in memory (pp.81-93). Chichester, England: Wiley.

    Eich, E., Macaulay, D., & Ryan, L. (1994). Mood dependent memoryfor events of the personal past. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 123, 201-215.

    Eich, E., & Metcalfe, J. (1989). Mood-dependent memory for internalvs. external events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,Memory, and Cognition, 15, 433-455.

    Fernandez, A., & Glenberg, A. M. (1985). Changing environmentalcontext does not reliably affect memory. Memory & Cognition, 13,333-345.

    Geiselman, R. E., & Glenny, J. (1977). Effects of imagining speakers'voices on the retention of words presented visually. Memory &Cognition, 5, 499-504.

    Godden, D. R., & Baddely, A. D. (1975). Context-dependent memoryin two natural environments: On land or underwater. British Journalof Psychology, 66, 325-331.

    Holloway, F. A. (1978). State-dependent retrieval based on time ofday. In B. T. Ho, D. W. Richards III, & D. L. Chute (Eds.), Drugdiscrimination and state-dependent learning (pp. 319-343). New York:Academic Press.

    Lewis, V. E., & Williams, R. N. (1989) Mood-congruent vs. mood-statedependent learning: Implications for a view of emotion. Journal ofSocial Behavior and Personality, 4, 157-171.

  • MUSIC-DEPENDENT MEMORY 1363

    McGeoch, J. A. (1932). Forgetting and the law of disuse. PsychologicalReview, 39, 352-370.

    Mozart, W. A. (1953). Rondo [Sheet Music]. Secaucus, NJ: WarnerBrothers Publications.

    Mueller, J. H., Grove, T. R., & Thompson, W. B. (1991). Mood-dependent retrieval and mood awareness. Cognition and Emotion, 5,331-349.

    Nevin, M. (1957). Jazz holiday [Sheet Music]. Paramount, CA:Schroeder and Gunther.

    Rovee-Collier, C , Earley, L., & Stafford, S. (1989). Ontogeny of earlyevent memory: III. Attentional determinants of retrieval at 2 and 3months. Infant Behavior & Development, 12, 147-161.

    Schab, F. R. (1990). Odors and the remembrance of things past.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,16, 648-655.

    Serafine, M. L., Crowder, R. G., & Repp, B. H. (1984). Integration ofmelody and text in memory for song. Cognition, 16, 285-303.

    Serafine, M. L., Davidson, J., Crowder, R. G., & Repp, B. H. (1986).On the nature of melody-text integration in memory for songs.Journal of Memory & Language, 25, 123-135.

    Shirabe-Sagaribe. (1980). On Japan Kabuki and other traditional music[Cassette]. New York: Elektra/Asylum/Nonesuch Records.

    Smith, S. M. (1979). Remembering in and out of context. Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 460-471.

    Smith, S. M. (1985). Background music and context-dependent memory.American Journal of Psychology, 98, 591-603.

    Smith, S. M. (1995). Mood is a component of mental context:Comment on Eich (1995). Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 124, 309-310.

    Smith, S. M., Glenberg, A., & Bjork, R. A. (1978). Environmentalcontext and human memory. Memory & Cognition, 6, 342-353.

    Spreen, O., & Schulz, R. W. (1966). Parameters of abstraction,meaningfulness, and pronunciability for 329 nouns. Journal of VerbalLearning & Verbal Behavior, 5, 459-468.

    Thaut, M. H., & de l'Etoile, S. K. (1993). The effects of music on moodstate-dependent recall. Journal of Musk Therapy, 30, 70-80.

    Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. Oxford, England:Clarendon Press.

    Ucros, C. G. (1989). Mood state-dependent memory: A meta-anarysis.Cognition and Emotion, 3, 139-167.

    Received July 14,1995Revision received February 21,1996

    Accepted February 28,1996

    Low Publication Prices for APA Members and AffiliatesKeeping you up-to-date. All APA Fellows, Members, Associates, and Student Affiliatesreceiveas part of their annual duessubscriptions to theAmerican Psychologist aadAPAMonitor. High School Teacher and International Affiliates receive subscriptions to the.i4.R4Monitor, and they may subscribe to the American Psychologist at a significantly reducedrate. In addition, all Members and Student Affiliates are eligible for savings of up to 60%(plus a journal credit) on all other APA journals, as well as significant discounts onsubscriptions from cooperating societies and publishers (e.g., the American Association forCounseling and Development, Academic Press, and Human Sciences Press).

    Essential resources. APA members and affiliates receive special rates for purchases ofAPA books, including the/>aZ)/;caftoMiwa/ ofthe American PsychologicalAssociation,and on dozens of new topical books each year.

    Other benefits of membership. Membership in APA also provides eligibility forcompetitive insuranceplans, continuing education programs, reduced APAconventionfees,and specialty divisions.

    More information. Write to American Psychological Association, Membership Services,750 First Street, NE, Washington, DC 20002-4242.