16
Author Proof A Bioelectromagnetics 00:1 ^ 8 (2005) Effects of Radiofrequency Radiation Emitted By CellularTelephones, on the Cognitive Functions of Humans Ilan Eliyahu, 1 * Roy Luria, 2 Ronen Hareuveny, 1 Menachem Margaliot, 1 Nachshon Meiran, 2 and Gad Shani 3 1 Radiation Safety Division Soreq NRC,Yavne, Israel 2 Department of Behavioral Sciences, Ben-Gurion University, Beer-Sheva, Israel 3 Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-Gurion University, Beer-Sheva, Israel The present study examined the effects of exposure to Electromagnetic Radiation emitted by a standard GSM phone at 890 MHz on human cognitive functions. This study attempted to establish a connection between the exposure of a specific area of the brain and the cognitive functions associated with that area. A total of 36 healthy right-handed male subjects performed four distinct cognitive tasks: spatial item recognition, verbal item recognition, and two spatial compatibility tasks. Tasks were chosen according to the brain side they are assumed to activate. All subjects performed the tasks under three exposure conditions: right side, left side, and sham exposure. The phones were controlled by a base station simulator and operated at their full power. We have recorded the reaction times (RTs) and accuracy of the responses. The experiments consisted of two sections, of 1 h each, with a 5 min break in between. The tasks and the exposure regimes were counterbalanced. The results indicated that the exposure of the left side of the brain slows down the left-hand response time, in the second—later— part of the experiment. This effect was apparent in three of the four tasks, and was highly significant in only one of the tests. The exposure intensity and its duration exceeded the common exposure of cellular phone users. Bioelectromagnetics 00:1–8, 2005. ȣ 2005 Wiley-Liss, Inc. Key words: cognitive function ability; left hemisphere; right hemisphere; GSM INTRODUCTION The dramatic increase in cellular phones usage raises the question of the existence of possible biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although the effects of the utilized frequencies (0.9 and 1.8 GHz) have been studied before, two new developments in cellular technology warrant our attention: 1. Never before have so many people (especially children) been exposed to RFR, at non-negligible intensities with such proximity to the head (although within permitted levels according to IRPA/ICNIRP, 1998, FCC, 1996, CENLEC, 2001 exposure standards). 2. Most modern cellular systems operate in a pulsating mode in which the data is accumulated and transmitted in short pulses. Such modulated RFR at low average power has been reported to have effects on the central nervous system [Bawin et al., 1975; Blackman et al., 1979, 1980]. Several recent studies on the effects of exposure to RFR from cellular phones report that exposure to 900 MHz has an identifiable effect on electroencephalo- gram (EEG). Klaus and Joachim [1996] found changes in the EEG pattern of sleeping subjects during the exposure to GSM RFR. The radiation source in the said report had an 8 W output and the power density at the head was estimated to be 0.05 mW/cm 2 . The subjects were exposed for 8 h from one side only. Further studies [Wagner et al., 1998] tried, but failed, to replicate the results of this study. A study by Krause et al. [2000] tested the effects of GSM cellular phone on the EEG during an auditory ȣ 2005 Wiley-Liss,Inc. BEM/809M-04(20187) —————— *Correspondence to: Ilan Eliyahu, Radiation Safety Division Soreq NRC, Yavne 81800, Israel. E-mail: [email protected] Received for review 29 November 2004; Final revision received 30 June 2005 DOI 10.1002/bem.20187 Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com).

Author Proof Bioelectromagnetics00:1^8(2005 ...nmeiran/index_files/cell1 proof.pdf · biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    3

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Author Proof Bioelectromagnetics00:1^8(2005 ...nmeiran/index_files/cell1 proof.pdf · biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although

Author Proof

ABioelectromagnetics 00:1^8 (2005)

Effects of Radiofrequency Radiation Emitted ByCellularTelephones, on the Cognitive

Functions of Humans

Ilan Eliyahu,1* Roy Luria,2 Ronen Hareuveny,1 Menachem Margaliot,1

Nachshon Meiran,2 and Gad Shani3

1Radiation Safety Division Soreq NRC,Yavne, Israel2Department of Behavioral Sciences, Ben-GurionUniversity, Beer-Sheva, Israel

3Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-GurionUniversity, Beer-Sheva, Israel

The present study examined the effects of exposure to Electromagnetic Radiation emitted by astandard GSM phone at 890 MHz on human cognitive functions. This study attempted to establish aconnection between the exposure of a specific area of the brain and the cognitive functions associatedwith that area. A total of 36 healthy right-handed male subjects performed four distinct cognitive tasks:spatial item recognition, verbal item recognition, and two spatial compatibility tasks. Tasks werechosen according to the brain side they are assumed to activate. All subjects performed the tasks underthree exposure conditions: right side, left side, and sham exposure. The phones were controlled by abase station simulator and operated at their full power. We have recorded the reaction times (RTs) andaccuracy of the responses. The experiments consisted of two sections, of 1 h each, with a 5 min break inbetween. The tasks and the exposure regimes were counterbalanced. The results indicated that theexposure of the left side of the brain slows down the left-hand response time, in the second—later—part of the experiment. This effect was apparent in three of the four tasks, and was highly significant inonly one of the tests. The exposure intensity and its duration exceeded the common exposure ofcellular phone users. Bioelectromagnetics 00:1–8, 2005. � 2005 Wiley-Liss, Inc.

Key words: cognitive function ability; left hemisphere; right hemisphere; GSM

INTRODUCTION

The dramatic increase in cellular phones usageraises the question of the existence of possiblebiological effects of radio-frequency electromagneticradiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although the effectsof the utilized frequencies (�0.9 and �1.8 GHz) havebeen studied before, two new developments in cellulartechnology warrant our attention:

1. Never before have so many people (especiallychildren) been exposed to RFR, at non-negligibleintensities with such proximity to the head(although within permitted levels according toIRPA/ICNIRP, 1998, FCC, 1996, CENLEC, 2001exposure standards).

2. Most modern cellular systems operate in a pulsatingmode in which the data is accumulated andtransmitted in short pulses. Such modulated RFRat low average power has been reported to haveeffects on the central nervous system [Bawin et al.,1975; Blackman et al., 1979, 1980].

Several recent studies on the effects of exposure toRFR from cellular phones report that exposure to900 MHz has an identifiable effect on electroencephalo-gram (EEG). Klaus and Joachim [1996] found changesin the EEG pattern of sleeping subjects during theexposure to GSM RFR. The radiation source in the saidreport had an 8 W output and the power density at thehead was estimated to be 0.05 mW/cm2. The subjectswere exposed for 8 h from one side only. Further studies[Wagner et al., 1998] tried, but failed, to replicate theresults of this study.

A study by Krause et al. [2000] tested the effects ofGSM cellular phone on the EEG during an auditory

12005Wiley-Liss, Inc. BEM/809M-04(20187)

——————*Correspondence to: Ilan Eliyahu, Radiation Safety DivisionSoreq NRC, Yavne 81800, Israel. E-mail: [email protected]

Received for review 29 November 2004; Final revision received30 June 2005

DOI 10.1002/bem.20187Published online in Wiley InterScience(www.interscience.wiley.com).

Page 2: Author Proof Bioelectromagnetics00:1^8(2005 ...nmeiran/index_files/cell1 proof.pdf · biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although

Author Proof

Amemory test. A standard GSM phone (at 902 MHz) wasattached to the right posterior temporal region of thehead under two exposure conditions: on or off. Theirfindings indicated that EMF decreased the theta bandactivity only during their memory retrieval task, andincreased the alpha band activity. However, a repli-cation study by Krause et al. [2004] did not confirmthese findings.

An epidemiological study by Oftedal et al. [2000]in Sweden and Norway looked for symptoms such asheadaches, feelings of discomfort or warmth behind/around the ear in GSM users and NMT users. Theirresults indicated that mobile phone users experienced avariety of symptoms either during or shortly after aphone call. A study by Koivisto et al. [2001] comparedsubjective symptoms or sensations, such as head aches,dizziness, fatigue, itching, tingling of the skin, rednessand sensations of warmth on the skin in two groups of 48subjects, who were exposed to RFR for 30–60 min.Their results did not reveal significant differencesbetween the exposure and non-exposure conditions.

The effects of RFR on cognitive functions wereexamined in several studies. Preece et al. [1999]conducted tests on a variety of short-term and long-term memory tasks, and reaction time (RT) tests. Thesubjects were exposed to RFR at 915 MHz (1 and0.25 W powers). They reported a reduction in RT, withthe shortest response time when the subjects wereexposed to 915 MHz at 1 W.

Similar tests were conducted by Koivisto et al.[2000a]: 12 different RT tests were conducted under theexposure to 902 MHz at 0.25 W. In three of the tests, areduction in the RT was observed. Another study byKoivisto et al. [2000b] examined the effects on theworking memory, and revealed a reduction in RT underexposure to 902 MHz at 0.25 W.

In two replications and an extension study done byHaarala et al. 2003, 2004, 64 subjects in two differentlaboratories in Finland and Sweden performed double-blind cognitive and short-term memory tasks. Thephone was attached to the left side of the head. Nostatistically significant differences were found betweenlaboratories, and they did not replicate the Koivistoet al. [2000a,b] results.

The Present Study

The goal of the present study was to examinewhether RFR can affect cognitive functions. Thesubjects were asked to perform four different taskswhile being exposed to different RFR condition. Thetasks chosen were those that are known to have highhemisphere specificity, i.e., they activate mostly oneside of the brain (see Smith and Joindes, 1999 for areview article). The four tasks were a verbal item

recognition task (left side), a spatial item recognitiontask (right side), and two spatial compatibility tasks (theleft compatible stimuli activate the right side, while theright compatible stimuli activate the left side).

Subjects were exposed to RFR alternatively to theleft side, to the right side and sham exposed. To the bestof our knowledge this is the first study that compareseffects of exposure to the two head sides within thesame task. This allows us to test the hypothesis of thepresent work, that performance of specific tasks isaffected by one side exposures only.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Thirty-six healthy right-handed male subjectswere chosen. The mean age was 24 years, ranging from19 to 27. The experiment was approved by the Ben-Gurion University Medical School Ethical Committee(Beer–Sheva, Israel). The subjects gave an informedwritten consent. The subjects completed a question-naire concerning intakes of tea, coffee, alcohol and theamount of sleep they had prior to the experiment. Theparticipants reported adequate sleep in the night prior tothe experiment, and they did not drink excessively oruse CNS-affecting drugs.

Cognitive Tasks

The subjects were requested to perform fourdifferent tasks. The examined parameters were theresponse time (RTs) and the percentage of erroneousresponses made by the subjects. In all tasks, subjectswere instructed to react to the stimuli presented on acomputer screen by pressing given buttons (‘‘/’’—usingthe index finger of their right hand or ‘‘z’’—using theindex finger of their left hand) on the computerkeyboard as quickly and accurately as possible.

As differentiation between hemispheres was oneof the objects of the present study, the tasks chosen werethose for which hemisphere differentiation is wellestablished.

The tasks were coded as follows:

Spatial item recognition task—‘‘FACE’’. In thistask, three targets ‘‘faces’’ were presented sequentially,for 650 ms each, in three random locations on thescreen, chosen out of eight possible locations. After a3.5 s interval, another face appeared in a randomlocation. The subject had to decide whether the last facematched the location of any of the three target faces.They were instructed to press the ‘‘/’’ when there was amatch, or ‘‘z’’ when there was no match. This task isknown to activate a region in the right premotor cortex[Smith et al., 1998; Smith and Jonides, 1999]. Figure 1illustrates this experiment.

2 Eliyahu et al.

Page 3: Author Proof Bioelectromagnetics00:1^8(2005 ...nmeiran/index_files/cell1 proof.pdf · biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although

Author Proof

AVerbal item recognition task—‘‘LETTER’’. In thistask, a small set of target uppercase letters waspresented simultaneously for 0.5 s, followed by a singlelowercase probe letter after a delay interval of 3 s. Thesubject had to decide whether the probe matched any ofthe target letters by pressing ‘‘/’’ when there was amatch or ‘‘z’’ when there was no match. This task isknown to activate the left posterior parietal cortex, threefrontal sites, and the left supplementary motor andpremotor areas [Smith et al., 1998; Smith and Jonides,1999]. Figure 2 illustrates this experiment.

Spatial compatibility—‘‘SPAT’’. In this task, a letterwas presented either on the left or on the right side of afixation letter. The subject had to activate the left or theright hand according to the side of the letter. This task isassumed to activate the right posterior parietal cortex,when the letter is in the right side of the fixation letter,and the left posterior parietal cortex when the letter is onthe left side of the fixation point [Peri and Zeki, 2000].Figure 3 illustrates this experiment.

Spatial compatibility—‘‘SIMON’’. In this task, thesubject had to respond to stimuli that appeared either on

the left or the right side of a fixation letter. When thesymbols P or y appeared, the subject had to respondwith his left or right hand, respectively. Previous studiesindicate that [Simon and Rudell, 1967; Simon, 1990]when the side of the stimulus’ presentation matches theresponding hand (the compatible condition), responseswere faster than when there is no match between theside of the stimulus and the responding hand (theincompatible condition). This is called the Simon effect[Simon and Rudell, 1967; Simon, 1990]. Figure 4illustrates this experiment.

The test was divided into two sessions: a first 1-hseries of tasks, a 5 min break, and then another hour oftasks. Before the first hour the subjects performed a 5-min training session of the four tasks employed in theexperiment in order to minimize training effects.

All subjects performed all four tasks under either,left, right, or sham exposure conditions. This resulted in12 sub-sessions per subject. Each subject performed atotal of 1614 trials in all four experiments.

RF Exposure

Each subject had two standard NokiaTM 5110GSM cellular phones attached to his head by a speciallydesigned non-conductive frame. The phones wereplaced on both sides of the head, as shown in Figure 5.

We controlled the cellular phones power by usingan HP GSM test system model E6392B. The phoneswere operated with test SIM cards (Wavetek). This

Fig. 1. ‘‘FACE’’�spatialitemrecognitiontask�thesubjectstaskistodecidewhether theprobefaceisin thesamelocationasanyof the target faces.

Fig. 2. ‘‘LETTER’’�verbal item recognition task�the subjectstaskistodecidewhether theprobematchesanyofthetargetletters.

X Xα

X

500msec 180msec

1200msec

Waiting for response

Fig. 3. ‘‘SPAT’’�spatial compatibility�the subject has to activatetheleft or therighthandaccording to thesideofthe letter.

EffectsQ1of Radiofrequency Radiation 3

Page 4: Author Proof Bioelectromagnetics00:1^8(2005 ...nmeiran/index_files/cell1 proof.pdf · biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although

Author Proof

Asystem maintained the phones at either no transmissionor full power transmission (890.2 MHz, 33 dBm¼ 2 Wpeak power, using the typical GSM pulse duration of577 ms and duty cycle of 1/8, yielding 0.25 W averagepower). The communication between the phones andthe test system was wireless, at an extremely smalloutput power (0.01 mW peak output power, ascompared to the 2 W peak output of the phones); thus,we consider it negligible. During the experiment, thephones were battery operated. The phones weremounted on the subject’s heads before the first taskand dismounted after the final task.

At the end of the experiments, the subjects wererequested to assess whether and when the phonesoperated. They were unable to distinguish betweenexposure/sham-exposure situations, and between thesides of the exposures.

All tasks utilized the standard laboratory PC-based software (Micro Experimental Laboratory 2.0TM)and experiments were presented on 14 inch screens. Thesubjects used standard 104-key computer keyboards.

In case of an erroneous response, the computeremitted a 400 ms beep at 500 Hz. The exposure regimeand the order of the tasks were counterbalanced across

subjects according to a balanced Latin square design.Each subject served as his own control, namely, hisperformance without exposure was compared to hisown performance under exposure (a repeated measuresdesign). The RF exposure regime was single-blinded,i.e., the experiment manager was aware of the exposuremode, while the subjects were not, since the phoneswere silent all the time. An opaque partition was placedbetween the experiment manager and the subjectsduring the experiment.

RESULTS

In each task, trials with response times longer than3 s and shorter than 100 ms were screened out. Onlytrials in which the response was correct were included inthe response time analyses. In the present work,Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied to thedf’s, and corrected P-values were reported for allfactors (when df> 2). No speed-accuracy trade-off wasobserved in any of the tasks.

A repeated measures analysis of variance(ANOVA) including Exposure condition (right hemi-sphere, left hemisphere, and sham exposure), Session(part one or part two), and Responding Hand (right orleft) as within dependent variables on the average RT ineach condition was performed.

Results for the FACE Task

The triple interaction between exposure, sessionand side was significant (F(2, 62)¼ 3.40, P¼.037) seeFigure 6a and b. For most exposure conditions, weobserved a reduction in the response/RT from the first tothe second session (with either sham exposure, orexposure to the right side of the brain). This result isprobably due to training (see Fig. 6a). However, in theleft hemisphere exposure condition, a reversed patternwas observed when subjects responded with their lefthand: the RT in the second part of the experiment wassignificantly prolonged relative to the first part, from907 to 931 ms (F(1, 32)¼ 6.30, P¼.01) and this patternwas significantly different from the sham exposure andright side exposure averaged together (F(1, 32)¼ 5.17,P¼.02) see Fig. 6b.

There was also a significant main effect ofResponding Hand, (F(1,32)¼ 13.3, P<.001) indicat-ing that RT was 48 ms (from 864 to 912 ms) faster forthe right hand responses (recall that all subjects wereright handed).

Results for the LETTER Task

The main effect for the factor session (F(1, 29)¼4.79, P¼.036) and Hand (F(1, 29)¼ 26.74, P<.0001)was significant, as was the interaction between these

X

XΠ X

250msec180msec

1500mse

Waiting for response

Fig. 4. ‘‘SIMON’’�spatialcompatibility�whenthesymbolsPandy appeared, the subject had to presshis left or right hand respec-tively.

Fig. 5. A subject during the experiment, with the phone frameattached.

Color

4 Eliyahu et al.

Page 5: Author Proof Bioelectromagnetics00:1^8(2005 ...nmeiran/index_files/cell1 proof.pdf · biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although

Author Proof

Afactors, (F(1, 29)¼ 9.54, P<.005). Subject improve-ment from the first to the second part of the experimentwas limited to the right hand (60 ms improvement forthe right hand as opposed to 0 ms for the left hand—seeFig. 7). The results also indicate a trend similar to theFACE experiment—RTs in the left hand were increasedunder the left side exposure condition (by 13 ms—from1014 to 1027 ms) as opposed to a slight decease in RT inthe right side and sham exposure (2 and 9 msrespectively). This trend was not significant. It ispossible that the effect of RFR is less evident on theaverage RTanalysis, but it may still affect other parts ofthe RT distribution [Ratcliff, 1979]. In order to verifythis point, we reanalyzed the data, this time using the25th (fast RT) percentile as the independent variable.The left-side exposure condition slowed left handresponses by 14 ms as opposed to acceleration for theright side and sham exposure conditions (15 and 29 ms,respectively). For left hand responses, the left exposurecondition was significantly slower than sham exposurein the second part of the experiment (F(1, 29)¼ 4.28,P¼.04). This difference was not significant in the firstpart of the experiment, F< 1.

Results for the SPAT Task

The only significant observable effect was themain effect for the factor of responding hand (F(1,

29)¼ 7.75, P<.01). Right hand responses were 9 msfaster than left hand responses (340 and 349 ms,respectively). Importantly, the same trend for the leftside exposure condition appeared also in this experi-ment (see Fig. 8): left hand responses were slowed downby 5 ms from the first to the second part of theexperiment (from 349 to 354 ms), but under the rightside and the sham exposure conditions, the left handresponses was accelerated (2 and 9 ms, respectively).The difference between the left side exposure conditionand the sham exposure condition was significant in thesecond part of the experiment (F(1, 30)¼ 6.43,P¼.01), but this difference was not significant in thefirst part of the experiment, F< 1.

Results for the SIMON Task

The ANOVA was the same as in the previousanalysis, but it included the variable compatibility(compatible vs. incompatible responses) as an addi-tional independent variable. The main significant effectwas the difference between the examination sessions(F(1, 29)¼ 10.86, P<.005), namely: the first part of theexperiment was on the average, 24 ms faster than thesecond part.

Another effect observed was the so-called Simoneffect [Simon and Rudell, 1967; Simon, 1990]—thecompatibility between the visual stimulus side and the

Fig. 6. Response times in milliseconds�left hand response (b) and right hand response (a)�‘‘FACE’’ task.Triple interactionbetweenexposure, session, andsidewassignificant.For threeof theexposureconditions,areductionintheresponsetimefromthefirst tosecondsessionwasobserved.Only in the setup in which the exposure was to the left side and the left hand responded was theresponse timeprolonged.

Color

EffectsQ1of Radiofrequency Radiation 5

Page 6: Author Proof Bioelectromagnetics00:1^8(2005 ...nmeiran/index_files/cell1 proof.pdf · biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although

Author Proof

Aresponding hand (F(1, 29)¼ 7.06, P¼.01), namely:compatible responses were 16 ms faster than incompat-ible responses (from 499 to 515 ms). There was noindication of any effects of RFR either in the average RTanalysis or in the fast RT analysis.

DISCUSSION

The scientific literature dealing with the effects oflow intensity radio waves emitted by cellular handsetsshows a growing interest in the existence of cognitive

Fig. 7. Response timesinmilliseconds�first andsecondsessions�LETTER task.

Fig. 8. Response timesinmilliseconds�first andsecondsessions�SPAT task.

Color

Color

6 Eliyahu et al.

Page 7: Author Proof Bioelectromagnetics00:1^8(2005 ...nmeiran/index_files/cell1 proof.pdf · biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although

Author Proof

Aeffects. This is due to the assumption that cognitivefunctions might express very weak basic effects, if theyexist, in an observable manner, because of CNS amp-lification.

Of these effects, the linkage between RFR expo-sure and RT to external stimuli has been previouslyexamined. In particular, Preece et al. [1999] andKoivisto et al. [2000a,b] reported a shortening of theRT. Some work on verification of these reports failed toconfirm their findings [Haarala et al., 2003, 2004].

In these studies only one side of the head wasexposed: the left side [Preece et al., 1999; Koivisto et al.,2000a,b; Haarala et al., 2003, 2004] and the right side[Krause et al., 2000]. The responding hand was the righthand in some works [Koivisto et al., 2000a,b; Haaralaet al., 2003, 2004], while in the other publications thiswas not clearly specified.

In the present work, we thus added specificexaminations of possible differences between right/leftside exposures and right/left responding hand.

We considered these details to be of relevance, dueto the differentiation between the right/left brainhemispheres functions.

The present work is, to the best of our knowledge,the first attempt to examine directly these two res-ponding conditions. To achieve these purposes, we usedtwo phones, placed simultaneously on both sides of thehead, and in fact, our results indicated that the effect ofRFR was evident only in the left head side exposure andleft hand responding combined condition.

The statistically significant finding was a slowingeffect in left hand responses under left side RFRexposure condition only (found in the spatial itemrecognition task—‘‘FACE’’). This effect became evi-dent only in the second part of the experiment (namely:after an hour of test, of which 40 min were under fullpower exposure). The same trend was also observed inthe spatial compatibility task—‘‘SPAT’’, but was signi-ficant only in the specific comparison analysis.

While the effects of RFR were not evident in allexperiments, it is possible that the dependent variable ofaverage RT is not sensitive enough to detect thoseeffects [Ratcliff, 1979]. In this respect we have shownthat at least in one task (Verbal item recognition—‘‘LETTER’’) the effects were not apparent in the usualaverage RT but were detectable in other parts of the RTdistribution. Specifically, we found changes on the 25thpercentile (the fast part of the RT distribution).

It is noteworthy that although all the detectedeffects were expressed in left hand slowing, andappeared under left side exposure, we cannot statethat a hemisphere dependence was detected, as thefunctions affected are related to activities of bothhemispheres.

The origin of the differences between our resultsand the former studies is unclear, and could result formseveral reasons such as: the exposure methodology—right and left hemispheres, the responding hand—left orright, the exposure time, and the differences in thecognitive tasks. These differences seem to be significantand should be examined in future studies.

It can be concluded that more work is required toverify our results and to reconcile the differencesbetween our study, and the former studies in which anopposite effect, or no effect were found. In addition, theinvolvement of confounding factors must be examinedfurther.

REFERENCES

Bawin SW, Kaczmarek LK, Adey WR. 1975. Effects of modulatedVHF fields on the central nervous system. Ann NY AcadSciQ2 247.

Blackman CF, Elder JA, Weil CM, Benane SG, Eichinger DC,House DE. 1979. Induction of calcium-ion efflux from braintissue by radiofrequency radiation, effects of modulationfrequency and field strength. Radio Sci 14:93.

Blackman CF, Elder JA, Lampe JA, Benane SG, Faulk JM, HouseDE. 1980. Induction of calcium-ion efflux from brain tissueby radiofrequency radiation effects of sample number andmodulation frequency on the power density window.Bioelectromagnetics 1:35.

CENELEC. 2001. European Committee for ElectrotechnicalStandardization. Product standard to demonstrate the com-pliance of mobile phones with the basic restrictions related tohuman exposure to electromagnetic fields (300 MHz–3GHz).

FCC/Federal Communications Commission. 1997. Evaluatingcompliance with FCC Guidelines for human exposure toradiofrequency electromagnetic fields. OET Bulletin 65Edition 97-01. August 1997.

Haarala C, Bjornberg L, Ek M, Laine M, Revonsuo A, KoivstoM, Hamalainen H. 2003. Effect of a 902 MHz electro-magnetic field emitted by mobile phones on human cognitivefunction: A replication study. Bioelectromagnetics 24:283–288.

Haarala C, Ek M, Bjornberg L, Laine M, Revonsuo A, Koivsto M,Hamalainen H. 2004. 902 MHz mobile phone does not affectshort term memory in humans. Bioelectromagnetics 25:452–456.

IRPA/ICNIRP. 1998. Guidelines for limiting exposure to time-varying electric magnetic and electromagnetic fields (up to300 GHz). Health Phys 74(4):494.

Klaus M, Joachim R. 1996. Effects of pulsed high frequencyelectromagnetic fields on human sleep. Neuropsychobiology33:41–47.

Koivsto M, Revonsuo A, Krause MC, Haarala C, Sillanmak L, LaineM, Hamalainen H. 2000a. Effects of 902 MHz electro-magnetic field emitted by cellular telephones on responsetimes in humans. Cognitive Neurosci Neuropsychol II(2):413–415.

Koivsto M, Krause MC, Revonsuo A, Laine M, Hamalainen H.2000b. The effects of electromagnetic field emitted by GSMphones on working memory. Cognitive Neurosci II(8):1641–1643.

EffectsQ1of Radiofrequency Radiation 7

Page 8: Author Proof Bioelectromagnetics00:1^8(2005 ...nmeiran/index_files/cell1 proof.pdf · biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although

Author Proof

AKoivsto M, Haarala C, Krause MC, Revonsuo A, Laine M,

Hamalainen H. 2001. GSM phone signal does not producesubjective symptoms. Bioelectromagnetics 22:212–215.

Krause CM, Sillanmaki L, Koivisto M, Haggqvist A, SaarelaCarina, Revonsuo A, Laine M, Hamalainen H. 2000. Effectsof electromagnetic field emitted by cellular phones on theEEG during a memory task. Cognitive Neurosci II(4):761–764.

Krause CM, Haarala C, Sillanmaki L, Koivisto M, Alanko K,Revonsuo A, Laine M, Hamalainen H. 2004. Effects ofelectromagnetic field emitted by cellular phones on the EEGduring an auditory memory task: A double blind replicationstudy. Bioelectromagnetics 25:33–40.

Oftedal G, Wilen J, Sandstrom M, Mild KH. 2000. Symptomsexperienced in connection with mobile phone use. OccupMed 50(4):237–245.

Peri R, Zeki S. 2000. The neurology of saccades and covert shifts inspatial attention. An event-related fMRI study. Brain 123:2273–2288.

Preece AW, Iwi G, Davies-Smith A, Wesnes K, Butler S, Lim E,Varey A. 1999. Effect of a 915 MHz simulated mobile phone

signal on cognitive function in man. Int J Radiat Biol75(4):447–456.

Ratcliff R. 1979. Group reaction time distributions and an analysisof distribution statistics. Psychol Bull 86:446–461.

Simon JR. 1990. The effects of an irrelevant directional humaninformation processing. In: Proctor RW, Revve TG, Editors.Stimulus response compatibility: an integrated perspective.Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp 31–88.

Simon JR, Rudell AP. 1967. The effect of an irrelevant cueon information processing. J Appl PsychologiesQ3 300–304.

Smith E, Jonides J. 1999. Storage and executive process in thefrontal lobes. Science 283:1657–1661.

Smith E, Jonides J, Christy M, Robert AK. 1998. Components ofverbal working memory: Evidence from neuroimaging. ProcNatl Acad Sci USA 95:876–882.

Stewart W. 2000. Mobile phones and health. Independent expertgroup on mobile phones.

Wagner P, Roschke J, Mann K, Hiller W, Frank C. 1998. Humansleep under the influence of pulsed radiofrequency electro-magnetic field. Bioelectromagnetics 19:199–202.

Q1: Au: Please check the short title.

Q2: Au: Please provide the volume number.

Q3: Au: Please provide the volume number.

8 Eliyahu et al.

Page 9: Author Proof Bioelectromagnetics00:1^8(2005 ...nmeiran/index_files/cell1 proof.pdf · biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although

1 1 1 R I V E R S T R E E T, H O B O K E N, N J 0 7 0 3 0

ELECTRONIC PROOF CHECKLIST, B IOE LECTROM A G N E T I C S

***IMMEDIATE RESPONSE REQUIRED***Please follow these instructions to avoid delay of publication.

READ PROOFS CAREFULLY• This will be your only chance to review these proofs.• Please note that the volume and page numbers shown on the proofs are for position only.

ANSWER ALL QUERIES ON PROOFS (Queries for you to answer are attached as the last page of your proof.)• Mark all corrections directly on the proofs. Note that excessive author alterations may ultimately result in delay of

publication and extra costs may be charged to you.

CHECK FIGURES AND TABLES CAREFULLY (Color figures will be sent under separate cover.)• Check size, numbering, and orientation of figures.• All images in the PDF are downsampled (reduced to lower resolution and file size) to facilitate Internet delivery.

These images will appear at higher resolution and sharpness in the printed article.• Review figure legends to ensure that they are complete.• Check all tables. Review layout, title, and footnotes.

COMPLETE REPRINT ORDER FORM• Fill out the attached reprint order form. It is important to return the form even if you are not ordering reprints. You

may, if you wish, pay for the reprints with a credit card. Reprints will be mailed only after your article appears inprint. This is the most opportune time to order reprints. If you wait until after your article comes off press, thereprints will be considerably more expensive.

RETURN PROOFSREPRINT ORDER FORMCTA (If you have not already signed one)

RETURN WITHIN 48 HOURS OF RECEIPT VIA FAX TO Patrick Snajder AT 201-748-6825

QUESTIONS? Patrick Snajder, Production EditorPhone: 201-748-8807E-mail: [email protected] to journal acronym and article production number(i.e., BEM 00-001 for BioElectroMagnetics 00-001).

Page 10: Author Proof Bioelectromagnetics00:1^8(2005 ...nmeiran/index_files/cell1 proof.pdf · biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although

CCOOPPYYRRIIGGHHTT TTRRAANNSSFFEERR AAGGRREEEEMMEENNTT

Date:

To:

Production/ContributionID#______________Publisher/Editorial office use only

Re: Manuscript entitled ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________ (the "Contribution")

for publication in __________________________________________________________________ (the "Journal")published by Wiley-Liss, Inc., a subsidiary of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. ("Wiley").

Dear Contributor(s):

Thank you for submitting your Contribution for publication. In order to expedite the publishing process and enable Wiley todisseminate your work to the fullest extent, we need to have this Copyright Transfer Agreement signed and returned to us assoon as possible. If the Contribution is not accepted for publication this Agreement shall be null and void.

A. COPYRIGHT1. The Contributor assigns to Wiley, during the full term of copyright and any extensions or renewals of that term, all

copyright in and to the Contribution, including but not limited to the right to publish, republish, transmit, sell, distributeand otherwise use the Contribution and the material contained therein in electronic and print editions of the Journaland in derivative works throughout the world, in all languages and in all media of expression now known or laterdeveloped, and to license or permit others to do so.

2. Reproduction, posting, transmission or other distribution or use of the Contribution or any material contained therein,in any medium as permitted hereunder, requires a citation to the Journal and an appropriate credit to Wiley asPublisher, suitable in form and content as follows: (Title of Article, Author, Journal Title and Volume/Issue Copyright [year] Wiley-Liss, Inc. or copyright owner as specified in the Journal.)

B. RETAINED RIGHTSNotwithstanding the above, the Contributor or, if applicable, the Contributor's Employer, retains all proprietary rights otherthan copyright, such as patent rights, in any process, procedure or article of manufacture described in the Contribution,and the right to make oral presentations of material from the Contribution.

C. OTHER RIGHTS OF CONTRIBUTORWiley grants back to the Contributor the following:

1. The right to share with colleagues print or electronic "preprints" of the unpublished Contribution, in form and contentas accepted by Wiley for publication in the Journal. Such preprints may be posted as electronic files on theContributor's own website for personal or professional use, or on the Contributor's internal university or corporatenetworks/intranet, or secure external website at the Contributor’s institution, but not for commercial sale or for anysystematic external distribution by a third party (e.g., a listserve or database connected to a public access server). Priorto publication, the Contributor must include the following notice on the preprint: "This is a preprint of an articleaccepted for publication in [Journal title] copyright (year) (copyright owner as specified in the Journal)". Afterpublication of the Contribution by Wiley, the preprint notice should be amended to read as follows: "This is a preprintof an article published in [include the complete citation information for the final version of the Contribution aspublished in the print edition of the Journal]", and should provide an electronic link to the Journal's WWW site,located at the following Wiley URL: http://www.interscience.Wiley.com/. The Contributor agrees not to update thepreprint or replace it with the published version of the Contribution.

2. The right, without charge, to photocopy or to transmit online or to download, print out and distribute to a colleague acopy of the published Contribution in whole or in part, for the Contributor's personal or professional use, for the

Page 11: Author Proof Bioelectromagnetics00:1^8(2005 ...nmeiran/index_files/cell1 proof.pdf · biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although

-2-

advancement of scholarly or scientific research or study, or for corporate informational purposes in accordance withParagraph D.2 below.

3. The right to republish, without charge, in print format, all or part of the material from the published Contribution in a

book written or edited by the Contributor.

4. The right to use selected figures and tables, and selected text (up to 250 words, exclusive of the abstract) from theContribution, for the Contributor's own teaching purposes, or for incorporation within another work by the Contributorthat is made part of an edited work published (in print or electronic format) by a third party, or for presentation inelectronic format on an internal computer network or external website of the Contributor or the Contributor's employer.

5. The right to include the Contribution in a compilation for classroom use (course packs) to be distributed to students at

the Contributor’s institution free of charge or to be stored in electronic format in datarooms for access by students atthe Contributor’s institution as part of their course work (sometimes called “electronic reserve rooms”) and for in-house training programs at the Contributor’s employer.

D. CONTRIBUTIONS OWNED BY EMPLOYER1. If the Contribution was written by the Contributor in the course of the Contributor's employment (as a "work-made-for-

hire" in the course of employment), the Contribution is owned by the company/employer which must sign thisAgreement (in addition to the Contributor’s signature), in the space provided below. In such case, thecompany/employer hereby assigns to Wiley, during the full term of copyright, all copyright in and to the Contributionfor the full term of copyright throughout the world as specified in paragraph A above.

2. In addition to the rights specified as retained in paragraph B above and the rights granted back to the Contributorpursuant to paragraph C above, Wiley hereby grants back, without charge, to such company/employer, its subsidiariesand divisions, the right to make copies of and distribute the published Contribution internally in print format orelectronically on the Company's internal network. Upon payment of the Publisher's reprint fee, the institution maydistribute (but not resell) print copies of the published Contribution externally. Although copies so made shall not beavailable for individual re-sale, they may be included by the company/employer as part of an information packageincluded with software or other products offered for sale or license. Posting of the published Contribution by theinstitution on a public access website may only be done with Wiley's written permission, and payment of anyapplicable fee(s).

E. GOVERNMENT CONTRACTS

In the case of a Contribution prepared under U.S. Government contract or grant, the U.S. Government may reproduce,without charge, all or portions of the Contribution and may authorize others to do so, for official U.S. Government purposesonly, if the U.S. Government contract or grant so requires. (U.S. Government Employees: see note at end).

F. COPYRIGHT NOTICEThe Contributor and the company/employer agree that any and all copies of the Contribution or any part thereofdistributed or posted by them in print or electronic format as permitted herein will include the notice of copyright asstipulated in the Journal and a full citation to the Journal as published by Wiley.

G. CONTRIBUTOR'S REPRESENTATIONSThe Contributor represents that the Contribution is the Contributor's original work. If the Contribution was preparedjointly, the Contributor agrees to inform the co-Contributors of the terms of this Agreement and to obtain their signature tothis Agreement or their written permission to sign on their behalf. The Contribution is submitted only to this Journal andhas not been published before, except for "preprints" as permitted above. (If excerpts from copyrighted works owned bythird parties are included, the Contributor will obtain written permission from the copyright owners for all uses as set forthin Wiley's permissions form or in the Journal's Instructions for Contributors, and show credit to the sources in theContribution.) The Contributor also warrants that the Contribution contains no libelous or unlawful statements, does notinfringe on the rights or privacy of others, or contain material or instructions that might cause harm or injury.

Page 12: Author Proof Bioelectromagnetics00:1^8(2005 ...nmeiran/index_files/cell1 proof.pdf · biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although

-3-

CHECK ONE:___________________________________ _____________________

[____]Contributor-owned work Contributor's signature Date

___________________________________________________________Type or print name and title

___________________________________ _____________________Co-contributor's signature Date

___________________________________________________________Type or print name and title

ATTACHED ADDITIONAL SIGNATURE PAGE AS NECESSARY

___________________________________ _____________________[____]Company/Institution-owned work Company or Institution (Employer-for-Hire) Date

(made-for-hire in thecourse of employment) ___________________________________ ______________________

Authorized signature of Employer Date

[____]U.S. Government work

NNoottee ttoo UU..SS.. GGoovveerrnnmmeenntt EEmmppllooyyeeeess

A Contribution prepared by a U.S. federal government employee as part of the employee's official duties, or which is an officialU.S. Government publication is called a "U.S. Government work," and is in the public domain in the United States. In such case,the employee may cross out Paragraph A.1 but must sign and return this Agreement. If the Contribution was not prepared aspart of the employee's duties or is not an official U.S. Government publication, it is not a U.S. Government work.

[____]U.K. Government work (Crown Copyright)

Note to U.K. Government Employees

The rights in a Contribution prepared by an employee of a U.K. government department, agency or other Crown body as part ofhis/her official duties, or which is an official government publication, belong to the Crown. In such case, the Publisher willforward the relevant form to the Employee for signature.

Page 13: Author Proof Bioelectromagnetics00:1^8(2005 ...nmeiran/index_files/cell1 proof.pdf · biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although

BIOELECTROMAGNETICSJOHN WILEY & SONS, INC.1 1 1 R I V E R S T R E E T , H O B O K E N , N J 0 7 0 3 0

Telephone Number: 201.748.8807 • Facsimile Number: 201.748.6825

To: Mr. Patrick Snajder

Company:

Phone:

Fax: 201-748-6825

From:

Date:

Pages including thiscover page:

Message:

Page 14: Author Proof Bioelectromagnetics00:1^8(2005 ...nmeiran/index_files/cell1 proof.pdf · biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although

E1

REPRINT BILLING DEPARTMENT •• 111 RIVER STREET, HOBOKEN, NJ 07030PHONE: (201) 748-8807; FAX: (201) 748-6825

E-MAIL: [email protected] REPRINT ORDER FORM

Please complete and return this form even if you do not wish to order any reprints together with the second page of the pagecharge form. Fill either the top or bottom section, whichever is applicable.BIOELECTROMAGNETIC VOLUME ISSUE ARTICLE NO. NO. OF PAGES

AUTHOR(S) ARTICLE TITLE

£ My institution does pay page charges. Please supply me with 100 reprints at no charge plus _____________ additional reprints of the above article for$_________________ plus shipping and handling charges. (Tax Exempt #:_________________________________)

No. of Pages 100 Reprints 200 Reprints 300 Reprints 400 Reprints 500 Reprints$ $ $ $ $

1-4 117 168 219 270 3215-8 156 241 326 411 496

9-12 195 314 433 552 67113-16 234 387 540 693 84617-20 273 460 647 834 1,02121-24 311 532 753 974 1,19525-28 349 603 857 1,111 1,36529-32 387 674 961 1,248 1,53533-36 425 745 1,065 1,385 1,70537-40 463 816 1,169 1,522 1,875

(Shipping charges and applicable sales taxes are additional)

£ My institution does not pay page charges. Please supply me with _________ reprints of the above article for $__________ plus shipping and handling charges. (TaxExempt #:_________________________________)

No. of Pages 100 Reprints 200 Reprints 300 Reprints 400 Reprints 500 Reprints$ $ $ $ $

1-4 203 376 522 668 7905-8 287 528 741 940 1,110

9-12 357 660 927 1,176 1,39013-16 429 792 1,116 1,416 1,67017-20 477 884 1,239 1,572 1,86521-24 547 1,010 1,419 1,800 2,12525-28 604 1,118 1,572 1,992 2,35529-32 665 1,230 1,725 2,188 2,59033-36 731 1,350 1,896 2,408 2,85037-40 797 1,474 2,073 2,628 2,995

(Shipping charges and applicable sales taxes are additional)Reprints are available only in lots of 100. IF YOU WISH TO ORDER MORE THAN 500 REPRINTS, PLEASE CONTACT OUR REPRINTSDEPARTMENT AT (201) 748-8807 FOR A PRICE QUOTE.

BILL TO: SHIP TO:Name Name

Institution Institution

Address Address

Phone FaxPurchase Order No. If “BILL TO” is an institution, order form must be accompanied or followed

by a purchase order made out to the Publisher, Wiley-Liss, Inc.

PLEASE NOTE: This form is sent to only one author of each article. If your co-authors will want reprints, be sure to order them on this form together with yours.Please complete and return this form within 48 hours of receipt. Reprints ordered after printing of an issue are more expensive.

Page 15: Author Proof Bioelectromagnetics00:1^8(2005 ...nmeiran/index_files/cell1 proof.pdf · biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although

2412 Cobblestone WayFrederick, MD 21702

PAGE CHARGE FORM

Bioelectromagnetics, Volume ________, Issue ________

Authors: ______________________________________________________________________________

Article number: __________

The Bioelectromagnetics Society request that the author’s institution pay a part of the costof publication in the form of a page charge of $80 per page. This charge entitles the institution to100 free reprints. There will be no discrimination against papers for which page charges are notpaid. Additional reprints may be ordered directly from the publisher on the enclosed form.

Number of pages: ________ @ $80 per page = $_________.

My institution will pay page charges: YES __________; NO __________.

If YES, enclosed is a check __________; purchase order __________.

Ship 100 reprints to:

Name: _______________________________________________________________________________

Address: _____________________________________________________________________________

_____________________________________________________________________________________

_____________________________________________________________________________________

Signature: _____________________________________________________ Date: _________________

Please fill in and return one copy along with a check or purchase order to the BioelectromagneticsSociety at the above address. Return another copy to the publisher, Wiley-Liss, Inc., 111 RiverStreet, Hoboken, NJ 07030, USA, together with your order for additional reprints and proofs.

Page 16: Author Proof Bioelectromagnetics00:1^8(2005 ...nmeiran/index_files/cell1 proof.pdf · biological effects of radio-frequency electromagnetic radiation (RFR) [Stewart, 2000]. Although

Softproofing for advanced Adobe Acrobat Users - NOTES toolNOTE: ACROBAT READER FROM THE INTERNET DOES NOT CONTAIN THE NOTES TOOL USED IN THIS PROCEDURE.

Acrobat annotation tools can be very useful for indicating changes to the PDF proof of your article.By using Acrobat annotation tools, a full digital pathway can be maintained for your page proofs.

The NOTES annotation tool can be used with either Adobe Acrobat 4.0, 5.0 or 6.0. Other annotation tools are also available in Acrobat 4.0, but this instruction sheet will concentrateon how to use the NOTES tool. Acrobat Reader, the free Internet download software from Adobe,DOES NOT contain the NOTES tool. In order to softproof using the NOTES tool you must havethe full software suite Adobe Acrobat 4.0, 5.0 or 6.0 installed on your computer.

Steps for Softproofing using Adobe Acrobat NOTES tool:

1. Open the PDF page proof of your article using either Adobe Acrobat 4.0, 5.0 or 6.0. Proofyour article on-screen or print a copy for markup of changes.

2. Go to File/Preferences/Annotations (in Acrobat 4.0) or Document/Add a Comment (in Acrobat6.0 and enter your name into the “default user” or “author” field. Also, set the font size at 9 or 10point.

3. When you have decided on the corrections to your article, select the NOTES tool from theAcrobat toolbox and click in the margin next to the text to be changed.

4. Enter your corrections into the NOTES text box window. Be sure to clearly indicate where thecorrection is to be placed and what text it will effect. If necessary to avoid confusion, you canuse your TEXT SELECTION tool to copy the text to be corrected and paste it into the NOTEStext box window. At this point, you can type the corrections directly into the NOTES textbox window. DO NOT correct the text by typing directly on the PDF page.

5. Go through your entire article using the NOTES tool as described in Step 4.

6. When you have completed the corrections to your article, go to File/Export/Annotations (inAcrobat 4.0) or Document/Add a Comment (in Acrobat 6.0).

7. When closing your article PDF be sure NOT to save changes to original file.

8. To make changes to a NOTES file you have exported, simply re-open the original PDFproof file, go to File/Import/Notes and import the NOTES file you saved. Make changes and re-export NOTES file keeping the same file name.

9. When complete, attach your NOTES file to a reply e-mail message. Be sure to include yourname, the date, and the title of the journal your article will be printed in.