Aspects of Mothers’ Parenting Independent and Specific INGLES

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/13/2019 Aspects of Mothers Parenting Independent and Specific INGLES

    1/15

    O R I G I N A L P A P ER

    Aspects of Mothers Parenting: Independent and SpecificRelations to Childrens Depression, Anxiety, and Social Anxiety

    Symptoms

    Natalie M. Scanlon Catherine C. Epkins

    Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

    Abstract Theories on childrens depression, anxiety, and

    social anxiety note aspects of parenting such as acceptance/rejection and behavioral control. Despite these theoretical

    relations and high rates of comorbidity among childrens

    internalizing symptoms, no studies have examined multiple

    aspects of parenting and childrens symptoms of depres-

    sion, anxiety, and social anxiety simultaneously. We

    examined mother- and child-reported mothers parenting

    behaviors (acceptance/rejection and behavioral control)

    and their combined, independent, and specific relations

    with childrens depression, anxiety, and social anxiety

    symptoms in a community sample of 124 motherchild

    dyads (children 1012 years old). Childrens report of

    maternal behavioral control was related to mothers reportof childrens anxiety problems. Importantly, childrens

    report of mothers acceptance/rejection was an independent

    predictor of all three child-reported symptom types, and

    child- and/or mother-report of maternal acceptance/rejec-

    tion was an independent predictor of mothers report of

    childrens anxiety and affective problems. After controlling

    for anxiety, social anxiety, and both together, childrens

    perceived maternal acceptance/rejection emerged as a

    specific and unique predictor of childrens depression

    symptoms. But, after controlling for depression, parenting

    behaviors were no longer related to childrens anxiety and

    social anxiety. Clinical and theoretical implications are

    discussed, as well as directions for future research.

    Keywords Childrens depressive symptoms

    Childrens anxiety Childrens social anxiety

    Maternal acceptance/rejection Maternal behavioral

    control

    Introduction

    Depression, anxiety, and social anxiety are common psy-

    chological symptoms and disorders (American Psychiatric

    Association2000; Kessler and Wang2008) that often begin

    in childhood (Abela and Hankin 2008; Cartwright-Hatton

    et al.2006; Kessler and Wang2008; Rapee et al.2009) and

    are quite impairing (Epkins and Heckler 2011; Muris and

    Meesters2002). In addition, these subthreshold symptoms

    have similar correlates and outcomes as clinical diagnoses(see Epkins and Heckler 2011, for a review of depression

    and social anxiety symptoms; see Wood et al. (2003), for a

    review of anxiety symptoms). Importantly, these symptoms

    and disorders tend to co-occur (Chavira et al. 2004; Se-

    ligman and Ollendick1998). This is especially true for the

    comorbidity of depression and social anxiety in particular,

    both for symptoms and disorders of depression and social

    anxiety (see Crawley et al. 2008; Essau et al. 1999; Viana

    et al.2008). In fact, children with comorbid depressive and

    anxiety disorders, in comparison to anxiety-disordered

    youth without depression, have been found to have more

    social anxiety symptoms (ONeil et al. 2010).

    Aspects of parenting have long been theorized to be

    related to childrens adjustment, including their depression,

    anxiety, and social anxiety (see Epkins and Heckler 2011,

    for a review of depression and social anxiety; see Rapee

    2012, for a review of anxiety). There is also substantial

    empirical support for the relations between parenting and

    childrens internalizing symptomatology, although few

    studies have examined aspects of parenting and multiple

    types of childrens internalizing symptoms simultaneously.

    N. M. Scanlon C. C. Epkins (&)Department of Psychology, Texas Tech University, Box 42051,Lubbock, TX 79409-2051, USAe-mail: [email protected]

    1 3

    J Child Fam Stud

    DOI 10.1007/s10826-013-9831-1

  • 8/13/2019 Aspects of Mothers Parenting Independent and Specific INGLES

    2/15

    Given the comorbidity of depression and anxiety, and

    especially between depression and social anxiety, exam-

    ining relations between aspects of parenting and only one

    type of internalizing symptom results in little knowledge

    about whether various parenting constructs are indepen-

    dently related, or specifically and uniquely related, to

    depression, anxiety, and/or social anxiety.

    In general, parenting behaviors may be viewed oncontinuums, one of which stems from PARTheory

    (Parental Acceptance and Rejection Theory; Rohner and

    Khaleque2005) and is the acceptance-rejection continuum.

    Acceptance (as defined by such terms as warmth and

    support) lies at the positive end of the continuum, and

    rejection (as defined primarily by the lack of warmth or

    support, and criticism) lies at the opposite, negative end of

    the continuum. A second continuum of parenting behaviors

    is that of autonomy versus control. Autonomy-granting, on

    one end of the continuum, is more or less the allowance by

    parents of childrens age-appropriate freedom while con-

    trol, on the other end of the continuum, is broadly definedas restriction or the lack of autonomy-granting (Drake and

    Ginsburg2012). Theorists have also discussed two types of

    control; behavioral control and psychological control.

    Barber (1996) defined both types: Psychological control

    refers to control attempts that intrude into the psychologi-

    cal and emotional development of the childBehavioral

    control, in contrast, refers to parental behaviors that

    attempt to control or manage childrens behavior (p.

    3296).

    Based on this definition, parents who are high in

    behavioral control attempt to directly control their chil-

    drens behavior by making rules and insisting that they arefollowed (Ballash et al. 2006; McLeod et al. 2007b). Par-

    ents who are high in psychological control, on the other

    hand, attempt to control childrens psychological devel-

    opment and typically do so by means of more covert

    methods than behavioral control (e.g., shaming, guilt

    induction, love withdrawal; Soenens and Vansteenkiste

    2010). In this way, psychological control has been likened

    to criticism and a lack of emotional warmth, both of which

    are more in line with the construct of parental rejection

    (Settipani et al. 2013). Theoretically, parental rejection is

    related to childrens internalizing symptomatology, as

    Soenens and Vansteenkiste (2010) note, it seems likely

    that children of internally controlling [psychologically

    controlling or rejecting] parenting are more at risk for

    internalizing problems. These children would experience

    an inner conflict between complying with their parents

    requests and pursuing their own personally endorsed goals.

    This inner conflict would result in emotional distress (e.g.,

    anxiety and depression) (p. 83).

    More specifically, early family disruption implicated in

    interpersonal models of youth depression (Rudolph et al.

    2008) include parenting behaviors characterized by less

    acceptance and more rejection and behavioral control,

    thereby leading to negative self-evaluations on the part of

    the child. Khaleque (2012), in fact, explained that all

    humans need warmth, acceptance, and other positive

    responses from their primary caregivers. Without these

    positive responses and interactions, much can go wrong in

    the childs development that pertains to emotional stability(Rohner 1986/1999). For instance, if children perceive

    their caregivers to be rejecting and lacking in acceptance,

    they will likely develop negative thoughts and feelings

    about themselves and the future, which are symptoms of

    depression.

    Negative thoughts about the self are also in line with

    social anxiety, which involve excessive self-consciousness

    and worries about being judged harshly by others (Amer-

    ican Psychiatric Association 2000). Similar to youth

    depression, parental rejection and overprotection are also

    implicated in models of youth social anxiety (Kearney

    2005; Rapee and Spence2004). Moreover, Ollendick andBenoit (2012) have proposed a parentchild interactional

    model of youth social anxiety which includes parental

    rejection and overprotection (likely in the form of behav-

    ioral control), in addition to less acceptance. Thus, parental

    rejection and behavioral control are also implicated in

    models of childrens social anxiety, due to the very self-

    focused and interpersonal nature of these parenting

    constructs.

    As with theories of youth depression and social anxiety,

    theories of general anxiety have focused on similar aspects

    of parenting. Ginsburg and Schlossberg (2002), drawing

    upon past models of youth anxiety, primarily focus oncertain anxiety-enhancing parenting behaviors such as

    over-control (both behavioral and psychological), over-

    protection, lack of warmth, criticism, and rejection.

    Clearly, rejection is associated with increased conflict in

    the parentchild relationship, which may lead to a lack of

    self-competence and self-worth on behalf of the child and

    therefore more anxiety (Ginsburg and Schlossberg 2002;

    Rapee 1997). Additionally, the perception of parents as

    rejecting often leads children to believe that the world is

    unfriendly, hostile, or dangerous (Rohner 1986/1999),

    which are symptoms of more general anxiety. Finally, lack

    of parental warmth (associated with increased rejection)

    may create disruption and distress in a childs development

    (Chorpita and Barlow1998), as well as a lack of support or

    affiliation, both of which may increase a childs level of

    anxiety (Drake and Ginsburg 2012).

    However, the consequences of less acceptance and more

    parental rejection (i.e., lack of self-competence and self-

    worth; lack of support or affiliation) are likely more so

    related to childrens depression and social anxiety symp-

    toms versus anxiety symptoms more generally. Thus, most

    J Child Fam Stud

    1 3

  • 8/13/2019 Aspects of Mothers Parenting Independent and Specific INGLES

    3/15

    developmental and theoretical models of youth anxiety

    focus primarily on parental behavioral control, rather than

    on acceptance/rejection (Chorpita and Barlow1998; Drake

    and Ginsburg2012; Hudson and Rapee2009; Rapee2001).

    For example, children whose parents assume control over

    everything likely do not develop a strong sense of mastery

    or self-efficacy, both of which are related to anxiety

    (Chorpita and Barlow1998). Although parental behavioralcontrol may be well-intentioned, it often results in an

    increased perception of threat and a decreased perception

    of control on the part of the child. Thus, children learn that

    many situations are anxiety-provoking and cannot be

    handled, so their learned response is increased anxiety and

    avoidance (Hudson and Rapee 2009) rather than mastery

    and confidence (Drake and Ginsburg2012; Settipani et al.

    2013).

    The implication in many of these theoretical models of

    youth depression, anxiety, and social anxiety is that it is

    childrens perceptions of parenting behaviors that have the

    strongest relations with their overall development (andinternalizing symptoms more specifically). For the most

    part, childrens perceptions of parenting behaviors are

    more salient with regard to relations to internalizing

    symptoms. Thus, from both a theoretical (Rohner 1986/

    1999) and empirical perspective (Fentz et al. 2011; Greco

    and Morris 2002; Hale et al. 2006; Khaleque 2012;

    Khaleque and Rohner 2002a), childrens perceptions of

    parenting behaviors are important to consider, above and

    beyond parents report of their own parenting behaviors.

    Empirically, youth depression has been found to be related

    to parental acceptance/rejection (Burkhouse et al. 2012;

    Dallaireet al. 2006; Schwartz et al. 2012; Sheeber etal. 2007).In fact, a meta-analysis (McLeod et al. 2007a) found that the

    construct of parental rejection (more specifically, aversive

    behaviors and lack of warmth) was most strongly related to

    childrens depression. However, none of the studies men-

    tioned above controlledfor comorbid anxiety or social anxiety

    symptoms. Likewise, studies have found parental acceptance/

    rejection to be related to youth anxiety (Drake and Ginsburg

    2011; Ginsburg et al. 2004a; Hale et al.2006; Hudson et al.

    2009; Hudson and Rapee 2001; Siqueland et al. 1996) and

    social anxiety (Knappe et al.2012), but none of these studies

    have controlled for comorbid depression.

    With regard to parental behavioral control, relations

    have been found with youth anxiety (Drake and Ginsburg

    2011; Ginsburg et al. 2004a; Hudson and Rapee 2001;

    McLeod et al.2007b; Muris and Merckelbach1998; Wood

    et al. 2003) and social anxiety (Knappe et al. 2012; Rork

    and Morris2009; Rubin et al.1999). Again, however, none

    of these studies controlled for comorbid depressive symp-

    toms. There is some empirical support for the relation

    between parental behavioral control and youth depression

    (or self-criticism more specifically; Koestner et al. 1991),

    in addition to behavioral control being related to childrens

    internalizing problems more generally (Mills and Rubin

    1998).

    A few noteworthy studies have examined aspects of

    parenting and multiple types of internalizing symptoms

    simultaneously. These studies have had mixed results

    though, owing somewhat to different methodologies aswell as to the investigation of family variables more

    broadly (Johnson et al. 2005; Starr and Davila 2008) as

    opposed to behavioral control and acceptance/rejection

    more specifically. In terms of behavioral control, some

    studies found a significant relation between childrens

    anxiety (Beesdo et al. 2010) or social anxiety (Greco and

    Morris 2002) symptoms and parental behavioral control,

    even after controlling for comorbid youth depression.

    Moreover, youth with comorbid depression and anxiety

    disorders, in comparison to anxiety-disordered youth

    without depression, have been found to report more family

    dysfunction in general, but these two groups did not differon perceptions of parental behavioral control (ONeil et al.

    2010). Thus, theoretically and empirically, parental

    behavioral control seems to hold more specific relations

    with childrens anxiety versus depression symptoms.

    In terms of parental acceptance/rejection, some studies

    have found that significant relations between acceptance/

    rejection and childrens anxiety (Fentz et al. 2011), and

    social anxiety (Hutcherson and Epkins2009), become non-

    significant when controlling for depression symptoms. In

    contrast, some studies found a significant relation between

    childrens depression and parental acceptance/rejection,

    even after controlling for youth anxiety (Beesdo et al.2010) and social anxiety (Hutcherson and Epkins 2009)

    symptoms. Therefore, empirically (Greco and Morris2002;

    Hutcherson and Epkins 2009), youth social anxiety and

    general anxietyappearto have similar relations to parental

    acceptance/rejection.

    Thus, parental behavioral control and acceptance/rejec-

    tion seem to be related to childrens depression, anxiety,

    and social anxiety. However, there seem to be stronger

    relations between parental acceptance/rejection and youth

    depression, and between parental behavioral control and

    youth anxiety. Indeed, in a recent review on youth anxiety,

    Rapee (2012) stated, Although other parenting styles such

    as criticism or lack of warmth may also play a role in

    anxiety, their influence on anxiety specifically has been less

    consistently established (p. 74). Furthermore, it is also

    important to examine youth social anxiety because there is

    some evidence that the parenting constructs of overpro-

    tection, rejection, and low warmth are associated with

    adolescents social anxiety disorder but not other anxiety

    disorders (Knappe et al. 2012).

    J Child Fam Stud

    1 3

  • 8/13/2019 Aspects of Mothers Parenting Independent and Specific INGLES

    4/15

    The purpose of this study is to extend past research by

    examining specific aspects of parenting (acceptance/rejec-

    tion and behavioral control) and all three types of chil-

    drens internalizing symptoms (depression, anxiety, and

    social anxiety) simultaneously. This will allow for a test of

    existing theoretical models and an examination of the

    independent, as well as specific and unique, relations

    between various aspects of parenting and childrensdepression, anxiety, and social anxiety which are currently

    not well known (see Epkins and Heckler2011for a review;

    Gere et al.2012).

    We hypothesized, based on theory and past research,

    that low acceptance, high rejection, and high behavioral

    control would be related to increased childrens depression,

    anxiety, and social anxiety symptoms. With regard to

    independent relations, we hypothesized that low maternal

    acceptance/high rejection would be more strongly and

    independently related to childrens depressive symptoms

    relative to maternal behavioral control. On the other hand,

    behavioral control would be more strongly and indepen-dently related to childrens anxiety and social anxiety

    symptoms relative to maternal acceptance/rejection. We

    include both mother- and child-reports of parenting to

    assess if childrens perceptions of parenting behaviors are

    more independently related to their internalizing symptoms

    (c.f., Khaleque2012).

    Importantly, with regard to specific and unique relations,

    after controlling for depression, we predicted that maternal

    acceptance/rejection would no longer be related to chil-

    drens symptoms of anxiety and social anxiety. But, after

    controlling for anxiety and social anxiety symptoms, we

    expected that acceptance/rejection would remain related tochildrens depression. We also predicted that maternal

    behavioral control would no longer be related to childrens

    depression symptoms after controlling for anxiety and

    social anxiety, but that behavioral control would remain

    related to anxiety and social anxiety symptoms after con-

    trolling for depression.

    Method

    Participants

    The sample consisted of 124 motherchild dyads (94.4 %

    biological mothers) from the community. Seven children

    were not biologically related to their mothers, but all but

    one of them had lived with their mother for a period of at

    least 5 years, if not for their entire life. The majority of

    mothers (72 %) were married, and approximately half of

    the child participants were girls (50.8 %). Children ranged

    in age from 10 to 12 years (M age 10.75 years,

    SD = .93 years). After Institutional Review Board

    approval, mothers and children were recruited through

    community events and activities such as camps, back-to-

    school events, sports, various seasonal events, etc. Only

    one child per household was allowed to participate,

    mothers had to be legal guardians of the children, and the

    participating children and their mothers were entered into a

    raffle to win one of ten $15 gift cards.The majority of the children were identified by their

    mothers as Caucasian (66.9 %), although a substantial

    percentage (33.1 %) was identified as being of a different

    ethnicity: Hispanic (17.7 %), Black (10.5 %), Asian

    (1.6 %), and Other (3.2 %). Using Hollingsteads Index

    (1975), the majority of families socioeconomic status

    (SES) levels fell into category 4 (44.1 %) and category 5

    (23.2 %), thereby reflecting a middle-upper level of SES.

    According to mother-report, 10.5 % of the children had

    been diagnosed with a psychological disorder in the past,

    and 18.5 % had a current diagnosis. With regard to

    treatment, 22 % of the children had previously been insome type of therapy/counseling (not including medica-

    tion) and 9 % were at the time of the study. For the

    mothers, 21 % had been diagnosed with a psychological

    disorder in the past, and 17 % had a current diagnosis.

    With regard to treatment, 47 % of the mothers had pre-

    viously been in some type of therapy/counseling (not

    including medication) and 10.5 % were at the time of the

    study.

    Procedure

    When permission to recruit at local community events wasgranted, caregivers (primarily mothers) were approached

    about participating in the study. If they were interested in

    participating (or at least agreed to be contacted about

    participating), mothers provided their name and contact

    information. Typically, they were telephoned within

    1 week of providing their contact information for the study.

    If they then agreed to participate, they scheduled a time

    (approximately 1 h) for themselves and their children to

    complete the measures either at the laboratory or in their

    home (79.8 % of the motherchild dyads completed the

    measures in their homes). Procedures were identical in the

    home and lab settings.

    Written informed consent from the mothers and assent

    from the children were obtained prior to participation in

    the study. Mothers and children independently completed

    two randomly-ordered packets of measures; one packet

    contained measures about themselves and the other con-

    tained measures about the other (the order of administra-

    tion of these packets was counterbalanced across child

    sex).

    J Child Fam Stud

    1 3

  • 8/13/2019 Aspects of Mothers Parenting Independent and Specific INGLES

    5/15

    Childrens Symptom Measures

    Beck Depression Inventory for Youth (BDI-Y)

    The BDI-Y (Beck et al. 2005) is a commonly-used and

    well-validated measure that assesses depressive symptoms.

    The BDI-Y contains 20 items, and responses for each item

    range from 0 (Never) to 3 (Always), with a higherscore reflecting more severe depression symptoms. The

    internal consistency has been found to exceed .90 in a

    number of samples (Beck et al. 2005; Stapleton et al.

    2007). Good testretest reliability (r = .79.92) was

    demonstrated with a sample of 105 children (ages 718)

    over a period of 78 days (Beck et al. 2005). Convergent

    validity has been demonstrated with questionnaire mea-

    sures as well as diagnostic interviews (Beck et al. 2005;

    Stapleton et al. 2007). Discriminant validity has also been

    demonstrated as youth diagnosed with depressive disorders

    have been found to have higher BDI-Y scores than youth

    with anxiety disorders, other disorders, and no disorders(Beck et al. 2005; Stapleton et al. 2007). The BDI-Y had

    good internal consistency in the current sample (a =.93).

    Beck Anxiety Inventory for Youth (BAI-Y)

    The BAI-Y (Beck et al. 2005) consists of 20 items that

    assess childrens fear, worry, and physiological aspects of

    anxiety. Like the BDI-Y, items are rated on a scale from 0

    (Never) to 3 (Always), with a higher score reflecting

    more anxiety symptoms. Good internal consistency (a s

    ranging from .89 to .91) and 1 week testretest reliabilities

    (rs ranging from .77 to .93) in 7 to 14-year-old youth havebeen demonstrated (Beck et al. 2005). Convergent and

    divergent validity have been demonstrated with question-

    naires as well as diagnostic interviews (Beck et al. 2005).

    The BAI-Y had good internal consistency in the current

    sample (a = .91).

    The Social Anxiety Scale for Children-Revised (SASC-R)

    The SASC-R (La Greca 1999) is a 22-item self-report

    measure designed for completion by preadolescent chil-

    dren. It is comprised of 18 items (and 4 filler items) that are

    each rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at

    all) to 5 (all of the time). Adequate convergent validity has

    been demonstrated between total scores on the SASC-R

    and the Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory for Children

    (SPAI-C), with Pearson correlation coefficients ranging

    from .63 to .81 (Epkins 2002; Morris and Masia 1998).

    Divergent validity has also been demonstrated between the

    two measures (Sanna et al. 2009) with the SASC-R able to

    distinguish between boys and girls with and without social

    phobia. Finally, predictive validity has also been

    demonstrated for the SASC-R (Reijntjes et al. 2007). Good

    internal consistency for the SASC-R total score (a = .91)

    was demonstrated in the current sample.

    Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)

    The CBCL (Achenbach and Rescorla2001) is completed by

    parents and it assesses childrens behavioral problems. TheDSM-Oriented Affective Problems and Anxiety Problems

    scales were used in this study. As Achenbach and Rescorla

    (2001) describe, these scales comprise items that were

    judged by experts as being very consistent with the

    respective DSM-IV diagnostic categories. Like the empiri-

    cally-derived CBCL scales, the CBCL DSM-Oriented

    scales were also normed on national samples with respect to

    child age and gender, and T-scores were used in the current

    study. In demographically-matched referred and nonre-

    ferred samples, internal consistency of the Affective and

    Anxiety Problems scales has been demonstrated

    (alphas = .82 and .72, respectively), as has 8-day testretestreliability (rs =.84 and .80, respectively; Achenbach and

    Rescorla2001). The Affective and Anxiety Problems DSM-

    Oriented scales have also been found to: (1) discriminate

    between referred children and demographically-matched

    nonreferred children; and (2) be related to interview

    administered DSM-IV checklists (rs =.63 and .43,

    respectively) and the presence or absence of professionally

    assessed clinical diagnoses in each category in a clinical

    sample (point biserial rs = .39 and .45, respectively;

    Achenbach and Rescorla2001).

    Parenting Measure

    Parental Acceptance and Rejection/Control Questionnaire

    (PARQ/Control)

    The PARQ/Control (Rohner and Khaleque 2005) is com-

    prised of the standard 60-item PARQ, in addition to a

    13-item control subscale. Both the Parent PARQ/Control

    and the Child PARQ/Control forms were used in this study.

    The standard PARQ yields an overall Total score of

    parental acceptance/rejection and includes items such as,

    My mother does not really love me; my mother lets me

    know I am not wanted. The control subscale yields a

    separate score and includes items such as, My mother is

    always telling me how I should behave; my mother

    believes in having a lot of rules and sticking to them.

    Each form (i.e., Parent and Child) uses a four-point Likert-

    type scale, and responses to items range from Almost

    always true to Almost never true. Total scores on the

    standard PARQ range from 60 to 240, with lower scores

    indicating an accepting parenting style, and higher scores

    indicating a more rejecting style. Scores on the control

    J Child Fam Stud

    1 3

  • 8/13/2019 Aspects of Mothers Parenting Independent and Specific INGLES

    6/15

    subscale range from 13 to 52, with higher scores reflecting

    more restrictive or strict behavioral control.

    Meta-analyses and numerous other studies support the

    reliability, validity, and factor structure of the Child and

    Parent PARQ in a wide-range of diverse samples (Khale-

    que and Rohner2002a,b; Rohner and Khaleque2005). The

    Parent PARQ Total Score has acceptable 3-week testret-

    est reliability (r = .84; McGuire and Earls 1993) andinternal consistency (alphas ranging from .78 to .90 in

    diverse samples; Khaleque and Rohner 2002b). The Child

    PARQ Total Score also has good internal consistency

    (alphas ranging from .69 to .95) and testretest reliability

    up to 10 years (Khaleque and Rohner 2002a,b). Both the

    Parent and Child PARQ Total Scores had good internal

    consistency in the current study (a = .92, a = .93,

    respectively).

    Regarding the Control subscale, adequate internal con-

    sistency was demonstrated in a meta-analysis with alphas

    acrossthe informant versions ranging from .49 to .91, with

    a weighted mean alpha coefficient of .73 (Rohner andKhaleque2003). Rohner and Khaleque (2003) also repor-

    ted alphas on the Child version ranged from .49 to .81 and

    on the Parent version ranged from .62 to .74; with weighted

    mean alphas for the Child and Parent versions reported to

    be .71 and .69, respectively. Construct validity has also

    been established across five culturally distinct samples,

    with factor analyses finding two correlated yet distinct

    factors: strictness and permissiveness (Rohner and Khale-

    que 2003). Internal consistency for both the Parent and

    Child Control subscales in the current study was as = .62

    and .67, respectively.

    Results

    Preliminary Analyses

    Table1contains the means, standard deviations, skewness,

    and kurtosis values for all mother and child measures.

    Childrens self-reported depression and anxietyT-scores on

    the BDI-Y and BAI-Y in our sample are consistent with

    those for community samples, as reported in the instrument

    manual (Beck et al. 2005). Mothers reports on childrens

    DSM-Oriented Affective and Anxiety problems on the

    CBCL were also consistent with T-scores for non-referred

    samples of boys and girls (Achenbach and Rescorla2001).

    Childrens social anxiety on the SASC-R in our sample

    was similar to other similar-aged school (La Greca 1999;

    Reijntjes et al. 2007) and community (Epkins2002) sam-

    ples. Regarding mothers and childrens overall accep-

    tance/rejection scores on the PARQ, in the US, most scores

    typically fall between 90 and 110 with youth having higher

    scores than parents (Rohner and Khaleque2005). Thus, our

    sample is similar to others. Additionally, mothers and

    childrens scores on the control subscale lie in between

    moderate control (2739) and firm control (4045; Rohner

    and Khaleque2005).

    Outliers on the measures and subscales were identified,

    and these scores were set to one unit higher (or lower) than

    the next highest (or lowest) score in the dataset, as rec-ommended by Tabachnick and Fidell (2007). Additionally,

    the assumptions of linearity, homoscedasticity, and nor-

    mality of errors were all examined. On the normal proba-

    bility plot of the residuals, points fell along the diagonal

    line and skewness and kurtosis values were all between -3

    and 3; thus, the assumption of normality of errors was met.

    Regarding linearity, scatterplots of the predicted stan-

    dardized values and actual standardized residuals were

    examined. This assumption too was met, as most of the

    points clustered around the imaginary line drawn at zero.

    Finally, with regard to homoscedasticity, variability in

    scores for one variable was approximately the same at all

    values of another variable, as the data met the assumption

    of normality of errors. This was further supported by

    examining the scatterplots of predicted standardized values

    and actual standardized residuals (Tabachnick and Fidell

    2007).

    The correlations among the parenting scales and chil-

    drens symptom measures can be found in Table 2. As seen

    in Table2, neither mother- nor child-reported maternal

    behavioral control was significantly related to either

    Table 1 Means and standard deviations on mothers and childrensmeasures

    Measure M SD Skewness Kurtosis

    M-REJECTION 85.38 13.36 .64 -.07

    M-CONTROL 39.81 4.01 -.30 .20

    C-REJECTION 94.10 22.05 1.13 .97

    C-CONTROL 37.60 5.54 .18 -.02C-DEPRESSION 11.85

    (47.93)8.08

    (10.41)1.06 .99

    C-ANXIETY 16.30(49.90)

    9.11(10.70)

    .81 .64

    C-SOCIALANXIETY

    41.94 13.74 .63 .26

    M-ANXIETY 54.04 5.84 1.66 2.02

    M-AFFECTIVE 54.44 6.29 1.75 2.87

    M-REJECTION = mother report of maternal acceptance/rejection;M-CONTROL =mother report of maternal control; C-REJEC-TION =child report of maternal acceptance/rejection; C-CON-TROL =child report of maternal control; C-DEPRESSION =scores

    on the BDI-Y; C-ANXIETY = scores on the BAI-Y; C-SOCIALANXIETY =scores on the child-reported SASC-R; M-ANXI-ETY =scores on the Anxiety Problems subscale of the CBCL;M-AFFECTIVE =scores on the Affective Problems subscale of theCBCL

    J Child Fam Stud

    1 3

  • 8/13/2019 Aspects of Mothers Parenting Independent and Specific INGLES

    7/15

    informants report of childrens symptoms with oneimportant exception: child-reported maternal behavioral

    control was significantly related to mother-reported child

    anxiety problems on the CBCL. In our main analyses, we

    controlled for potentially important demographic variables

    that past research finds are often related to childrens

    internalizing symptoms and parenting behaviors (c.f.,

    Anderson and Mayes 2010; Beck et al. 2005; Deater-

    Deckard et al. 2011; La Greca 1999). We controlled for

    childrens race (i.e., Caucasian vs. Non-Caucasian), family

    SES, childrens sex, treatment status for both children and

    mothers (i.e., past or current treatment vs. none), and

    mothers relationship to children (i.e., biological vs. non-biological).

    Main Analyses

    We examined the independent and specific relations of

    both child- and mother-reported maternal acceptance/

    rejection and behavioral control to each of childrens

    depression, anxiety and social anxiety symptoms, both

    before and after controlling for comorbid symptoms.

    Hierarchical multiple regression analyses were conducted

    where control variables (childrens sex, ethnicity, rela-

    tionship to mother, family SES, and both child and mothertherapy status) were entered in step one. Then, the four

    parenting constructs were entered simultaneously in step

    two. In controlling for comorbid symptoms, the comorbid

    symptoms were entered in step two and the parenting

    constructs simultaneously entered in step three. We

    examine statistical significance and report effect sizes

    regarding the magnitude of significant unique relations;

    withsr2 of .01, .09, and .25 indicating small, medium, and

    large effect sizes (Cohen et al.2003).

    Childrens Self-Reported Symptoms

    As shown in Table3, after entering the six control variables

    in step one, when considered together in step two, aspects of

    mothers parenting were related to childrens depression

    symptoms, and only child-reported maternal acceptance/

    rejection was an independent predictor (sr2 = .14, a med-

    ium effect size). Likewise, regarding childrens anxiety,

    when considered together in step two, aspects of mothers

    parenting were related to childrens anxiety, and only child-

    reported maternal acceptance/rejection was an independent

    predictor (sr2 = .11, a medium effect size; see Table3). For

    the regression on social anxiety, in step 2, aspects ofmothers parenting considered simultaneously were not, as a

    group, significantly related to childrens social anxiety, yet

    child-reported maternal acceptance/rejection was an inde-

    pendent predictor of social anxiety in the final model

    (sr2 = .04, a small to medium effect size). In all three

    regressions, race was a significant control variable, with

    non-Caucasian children reporting more internalizing

    symptoms than Caucasian children.

    After considering the effects of the six control variables,

    the four parenting variables collectively accounted for an

    additional 17, 13, and 6 % of the variance in childrens self-

    reported depression, anxiety, and social anxiety symptoms,respectively. Importantly, more child-reported maternal

    rejection was significantly and independently associated

    with children self-reporting more depression, anxiety, and

    social anxiety symptoms, with larger effect sizes for chil-

    drens depression and anxiety (sr2 = .14 and .11, respec-

    tively) compared to childrens social anxiety (sr2 = .04).

    Neither child-perceived nor mother-reported maternal

    behavioral control emerged as a significant or independent

    predictor of childrens self-reported symptoms, and these

    Table 2 Correlations among parenting subscales and childrens internalizing symptoms

    Measure 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9.

    1. M-REJECTION

    2. M-CONTROL .04

    3. C-REJECTION .11 -.09

    4. C-CONTROL .23** .05 .28***

    5. C-DEPRESSION .16* -.01 .38*** .13

    6. C-ANXIETY .16* -.02 .37*** .12 .85***

    7. C-SOCIAL ANXIETY .10 -.01 .27*** .14 .67*** .72***

    8. M-ANXIETY .28*** .04 .21** .16* .13 .20* .12

    9. M-AFFECTIVE .25** .01 .14 .08 .20* .17* .17* .58***

    M-REJECTION =mother report of maternal acceptance/rejection; M-CONTROL =mother report of maternal control; C-REJEC-TION =child report of maternal acceptance/rejection; C-CONTROL =child report of maternal control; C-DEPRESSION = scores on theBDI-Y; C-ANXIETY = scores on the BAI-Y; C-SOCIAL ANXIETY = scores on the child-reported SASC-R; M-ANXIETY =scores on theAnxiety Problems subscale of the CBCL; M-AFFECTIVE =scores on the Affective Problems subscale of the CBCL

    * p\ .05; ** p\ .01; *** p\ .001

    J Child Fam Stud

    1 3

  • 8/13/2019 Aspects of Mothers Parenting Independent and Specific INGLES

    8/15

    parenting constructs did not contribute much to the

    regressions.

    Childrens Self-Reported Symptoms, Controlling

    for Comorbid Symptoms

    Table4presents the above results after also controlling for

    comorbid symptoms in step 2. With regard to childrensdepression, after controlling for childrens anxiety in step

    2, aspects of mothers parenting considered simultaneously

    were not, as a group, significantly related to childrens

    depression in step 3, yet child-reported maternal accep-

    tance/rejection remained a specific and unique predictor of

    childrens depression in the final model (sr2 = .01, a small

    effect size). After controlling for childrens social anxiety

    in step 2, when considered together in step three, aspects of

    mothers parenting were significantly related to childrens

    depression, and only child-reported maternal acceptance/

    rejection was a specific and unique predictor (sr2 = .06, a

    small to medium effect size). After controlling for bothchildrens anxiety and social anxiety in step 2, child-

    reported maternal rejection remained a specific and unique

    predictor in the final model (sr2 = .01, a small effect size).

    Thus, more child-reported maternal rejection was specifi-

    cally and uniquely related to children reporting more

    depressive symptoms after controlling for demographic

    variables and their anxiety (sr2 = .01), their social anxiety

    (sr2 = .06) and both their anxiety and social anxiety

    (sr2 =.01).

    As anticipated, a different pattern emerged on anxiety

    and social anxiety, as seen in Table4. After controlling for

    childrens depressive symptoms in step 2, the parenting

    variables collectively were, as a group, not related to

    childrens anxiety or to their social anxiety (both

    DR2 =.00). Moreover, none of the four parenting variables

    (including behavioral control) were specific or unique

    predictors of childrens anxiety or social anxiety aftercontrolling for childrens depression symptoms in the final

    models (see Table4).

    Mother-Reported Childrens Affective and Anxiety

    Problems

    Similar analyses were conducted on mother-reported child

    affective and anxiety problems on the CBCL. After con-

    trolling for the six relevant demographic variables, the four

    parenting constructs considered together were not signifi-

    cantly, as a group, related to mother-reported child affective

    problems in step 2, yet mother-reported maternal accep-tance/rejection was an independent predictor in the final

    model (sr2 = .04, a small to medium effect; see Table5).

    After controlling for mother-reported child anxiety problems

    in step 2, the parenting constructs considered as a group in

    step 3 were not significantly related to mother-reported child

    affective problems, and no parenting variables were found to

    be specific or unique predictors (see Table 5).

    With regard to mother-reported child anxiety problems on

    the CBCL, as seen in Table5, child treatment was a

    Table 3 Hierarchical regressions examining parenting behaviors predicting childrens report of depression, anxiety, and social anxiety

    Predictor Depression Anxiety Social anxiety

    R2

    DR2

    DF b sr2

    R2

    DR2

    DF b sr2

    R2

    DR2

    DF b sr2

    Step 1 .07 .07 1.55 .08 .08 1.65 .12 .12 2.50*

    Child treatment .06 -.01 -.12

    Mother treatment -.02 -.02 .04

    Relationship -.07 .02 -.04

    Child race .27** .05 .29** .06 .33** .08

    Child sex -.05 -.05 -.14

    SES .04 .02 -.01

    Step 2 .24 .17 6.15*** .21 .13 4.72*** .17 .06 2.01

    M-REJECTION .06 .07 .03

    C-REJECTION .41*** .14 .36*** .11 .22* .04

    M-CONTROL .01 -.03 -.03

    C-CONTROL .05 .03 .08

    Betas reported are at the step in which the variable was entered. Child/mother treatment = treatment status (past or current treatment vs. none);relationship =mothers relationship to children (biological vs. non-biological); child race = Caucasian versus non-Caucasian; SES =socio-economic status of family; M-REJECTION =mother report of maternal acceptance/rejection; C-REJECTION = child report of maternalacceptance/rejection; M-CONTROL =mother report of maternal control; C-CONTROL =child report of maternal control

    * p\ .05; ** p\ .01; *** p\ .001

    J Child Fam Stud

    1 3

  • 8/13/2019 Aspects of Mothers Parenting Independent and Specific INGLES

    9/15

    significant control variable, with mothers reporting more

    anxiety problems for children with past or current (vs. no)

    treatment. The four parenting variables considered together

    in step 2 were significantly related to childrens anxiety

    problems, and both child- and mother-reported maternal

    acceptance/rejection were independent predictors (sr2 = .03

    and .04, respectively; both small to medium effect sizes).

    Here, more child- and mother-reported maternal rejection

    were significantly associated with mothers reporting more

    anxiety problems in children. However, after controlling for

    mother-reported child affective problems in step 2, the par-

    enting constructs considered simultaneously in step 3 were

    not significantly related to mother-reported child anxiety

    problems, and no parenting variables were specific or unique

    predictors. Finally, it is important to notethat although child-

    reported maternal behavioral control was significantly cor-

    related with mothers reporting more anxiety problems in

    children (reported above), maternal behavioral control

    (according to either mother- or child-report) did not emerge

    as an independent (much less a specific or unique) predictor

    Table 4 Hierarchical regression analyses examining parenting behaviors predicting childrens report of depression, anxiety, and social anxiety(controlling for comorbid symptoms)

    Predictor Depression (controlling for anxiety) Predictor Anxiety (controlling for depression)

    R2

    DR2

    DF b sr2 R2 DR2 DF b sr2

    Step 1 .07 .07 1.55 Step 1 .08 .08 1.65

    Child treatment .06 Child treatment -.01

    Mother treatment -.02 Mother treatment -.02

    Relationship -.07 Relationship .02

    Child race .27** .05 Child race .29** .06

    Child sex -.05 Child sex -.05

    SES .04 SES .02

    Step 2 .69 .61 225.79*** Step 2 .69 .61 225.79***

    Anxiety .82*** .61 Depression .81*** .61

    Step 3 .70 .02 1.49 Step 3 .69 .00 .26

    M-REJECTION .00 M-REJECTION .03

    C-REJECTION .14* .01 C-REJECTION .03

    M-CONTROL .03 M-CONTROL -.04

    C-CONTROL .03 C-CONTROL -.01

    Predictor Depression (controlling for social anxiety) Predictor Social anxiety (controlling for depression)

    R2

    DR2

    DF b sr2 R2 DR2 DF b sr2

    Step 1 .07 .07 1.55 Step 1 .12 .12 2.50*

    Child treatment .06 Child treatment -.12

    Mother treatment -.02 Mother treatment .04

    Relationship -.07 Relationship -.04

    Child race .27** .05 Child race .33** .08

    Child sex -.05 Child sex -.14

    SES .04 SES .01

    Step 2 .48 .40 88.27*** Step 2 .50 .38 88.27***

    Social anxiety .67*** .40 Depression .64*** .38Step 3 .54 .07 4.09** Step 3 .50 .00 .23

    M-REJECTION .04 M-REJECTION .00

    C-REJECTION .28*** .06 C-REJECTION -.06

    M-CONTROL .03 M-CONTROL -.04

    C-CONTROL .00 C-CONTROL .05

    Betas reported are at the step in which the variable was entered. Child/mother treatment = treatment status (past or current treatment vs. none);relationship =mothers relationship to children (biological vs. non-biological); child race = Caucasian versus non-Caucasian; SES =socio-economic status of family; M-REJECTION =mother report of maternal acceptance/rejection; C-REJECTION = child report of maternalacceptance/rejection; M-CONTROL =mother report of maternal control; C-CONTROL =child report of maternal control

    * p\ .05; ** p\ .01; *** p\ .001

    J Child Fam Stud

    1 3

  • 8/13/2019 Aspects of Mothers Parenting Independent and Specific INGLES

    10/15

    of childrens anxiety problems in the regression analyses,

    and behavioral control did not contribute much to the

    regressions.

    Discussion

    Although aspects of parenting have long been theoretically

    and empirically linked to childrens internalizing symp-toms (Epkins and Heckler2011; Rapee2012), few studies

    have examined multiple types of internalizing symptoms or

    multiple aspects of parenting in the same study. No studies

    have examined multiple aspects of parenting and childrens

    symptoms of depression, anxiety, and social anxiety in the

    same study, despite similar parenting constructs noted in

    theoretical models for all three of these symptoms and high

    rates of comorbidity (Chavira et al. 2004; Seligman and

    Ollendick 1998), especially among depression and social

    anxiety (Crawley et al. 2008; Essau et al. 1999; ONeil

    et al. 2010; Viana et al. 2008). This results in little

    knowledge about the specificity of various aspects of par-

    enting to depression, anxiety, and social anxiety. We

    extended past research by examining the independent, as

    well as specific and unique, relations among aspects of

    mothers parenting behaviors (as assessed by both mother-

    and child-report) and all three types of childrens inter-

    nalizing symptoms.Consistent with theories of depression (Rudolph et al.

    2008), anxiety (Ginsburg and Schlossberg 2002), and

    social anxiety (Ollendick and Benoit2012), we found that

    childrens perceived maternal acceptance/rejection was

    related to each of childrens self-reported symptoms of

    depression, anxiety, and social anxiety; and mother-repor-

    ted acceptance/rejection was related to childrens self-

    reported depression and anxiety. Additionally, mothers

    report of acceptance/rejection was related to mothers

    Table 5 Hierarchical regression analyses examining parenting behaviors predicting mothers report of childrens affective and anxiety problems(including controlling for comorbid symptoms)

    Predictor Affective problems Anxiety problems

    R2

    DR2

    DF b sr2 R2 DR2 DF b sr2

    Step 1 .05 .05 1.05 .13 .13 2.80*

    Child treatment .17 .24* .05

    Mother treatment .08 .17

    Relationship .02 .02

    Child race -.01 .04

    Child sex .04 .03

    SES -.06 -.02

    Step 2 .11 .06 1.81 .20 .08 2.67*

    M-REJECTION .23* .04 .21* .04

    C-REJECTION .13 .19* .03

    M-CONTROL .02 .06

    C-CONTROL -.04 .03

    Predictor Affective problems (controlling for anxiety problems) Anxiety problems (controlling for affective problems)

    R2 DR2 DF b sr2 R2 DR2 DF b sr2

    Step 1 .05 .05 1.05 .13 .13 2.80*

    Step 2 .33 .28 48.56*** .39 .26 48.56***

    Comorbid problems .57*** .28 .52*** .26

    Step 3 .35 .01 .51 .41 .03 1.33

    M-REJECTION .12 .10

    C-REJECTION .03 .12

    M-CONTROL -.01 .05

    C-CONTROL -.05 .05

    Betas reported are at the step in which the variable was entered. Child/mother treatment =treatment status (past or current treatment versusnone); relationship =mothers relationship to children (biological vs. non-biological); child race = Caucasian versus non-Caucasian;

    SES = socioeconomic status of family; M-REJECTION = mother report of maternal acceptance/rejection; C-REJECTION =child report ofmaternal acceptance/rejection; M-CONTROL = mother report of maternal control; C-CONTROL =child report of maternal control

    * p\ .05; ** p\ .01; *** p\ .001

    J Child Fam Stud

    1 3

  • 8/13/2019 Aspects of Mothers Parenting Independent and Specific INGLES

    11/15

    report of childrens affective problems, and both mothers

    and childrens report of acceptance/rejection was related to

    mothers report of childrens anxiety problems. Further-

    more, childrens report of behavioral control was related to

    mothers report of childrens anxiety problems, although

    behavioral control was neither an independent nor a spe-

    cific predictor of childrens depression, anxiety, or social

    anxiety symptoms.In some respects, our findings on behavioral control are

    inconsistent with previous research (Chorpita and Barlow

    1998; Drake and Ginsburg2012; Ginsburg and Schlossberg

    2002; Hudson and Rapee2009; Rapee2001) that includes

    parental behavioral control in models of childrens anxiety.

    However, it is also important to consider that there was only

    one measure of this construct. Although the control subscale

    of the PARQ/Control has demonstrated good reliability and

    validity, recent studies (Settipani et al. 2013; Soenens and

    Vansteenkiste2010) have pointed to the difficulty of mea-

    suring this construct and differentiating it from psycholog-

    ical control (which is more akin to parental rejection). Arecent study that looked at both behavioral and psycholog-

    ical control actually found a stronger relation between

    childrens anxiety symptoms and parental psychological

    control versus behavioral control (Wijsbroek et al. 2011).

    Future studies would therefore benefit from incorporating

    other measures of behavioral control, including other self-

    report measures as well as observations and interviews.

    Finally, it is also possible that parental behavioral control

    simply is not an independent or specific predictor of chil-

    drens anxiety (Gere et al. 2012), but future studies on

    anxiety and other comorbid symptoms are needed regarding

    behavioral control and childrens internalizing symptoms.In line with the few empirical studies that have inves-

    tigated various parenting constructs in relation to multiple

    youth internalizing symptoms (Beesdo et al. 2010;

    Hutcherson and Epkins 2009), we found support for the

    construct of maternal acceptance/rejection being indepen-

    dently, as well as specifically and uniquely, related to

    childrens depression symptoms. We even extended past

    work by controlling for comorbid symptoms of anxiety,

    social anxiety, and both anxiety and social anxiety symp-

    toms together, and childrens perceived maternal rejection

    remained significantly related to childrens self-reported

    depression symptoms. Also in line with past studies (Fentz

    et al. 2011; Hutcherson and Epkins 2009), the relations

    between childrens perceived maternal rejection and chil-

    drens self-reported symptoms of anxiety and social anxi-

    ety became non-significant when controlling for childrens

    comorbid depression symptoms.

    Additional theoretical implications may be gleaned from

    the current study. For example, the construct of maternal

    rejection (akin to maternal psychological control; Soenens

    and Vansteenkiste 2010) was related to all three types of

    internalizing symptoms in children but more so to

    depression and anxiety, rather than social anxiety. Thus,

    relations between parental acceptance/rejection and chil-

    drens social anxiety may only emerge when children also

    have comorbid depression. Similarly, aspects of parenting

    (as a group) did show relations with childrens self-repor-

    ted depression and anxiety, as well as mothers report of

    childrens anxiety problems, but those relations may onlybe implicated in models of childrens social anxiety if there

    are comorbid depression symptoms. It is important to note

    here that although child-reported maternal behavioral

    control was related to mothers report of childrens anxiety

    problems, behavioral control did not contribute much to

    parenting in the regression analyses.

    Important clinical implications may also be gleaned

    from the current study. An improved understanding of the

    independent, as well as specific and unique relations,

    between various aspects of parenting and childrens

    depression, anxiety, and social anxiety symptoms would

    help inform childrens treatment. A recent review of elevenrandomized controlled trials found mixed results for the

    efficacy of parenting components in treatments for chil-

    drens anxiety (Breinholst et al. 2012). However, the

    authors note that one explanation for these mixed results is

    that treatments are not including the parenting constructs

    most relevant to childrens symptoms (e.g., acceptance/

    rejection as demonstrated in the current study), so more

    research is needed to identify the specific aspects of par-

    enting that should be targeted.

    Furthermore, even if various aspects of parenting are not

    strongly related to childrens internalizing symptoms

    (McLeod et al. 2007a, b), they may still be relevant tochildrens treatment outcome and eventual symptom

    reduction. For example, Festen et al. (2013) sought to

    examine the parenting constructs that might predict treat-

    ment outcome for anxiety-disordered youth. They found

    that less perceived maternal warmth (assessed pre-treat-

    ment) was related to a less favorable treatment outcome for

    anxious youth. Therefore, even if certain parenting con-

    structs (e.g., behavioral control) do not seem to be directly

    related to childrens internalizing symptoms, they may play

    a key role in childrens treatment outcomes.

    We must recognize the potential limitations of the cur-

    rent study. As mentioned above with regard to the construct

    of behavioral control, we relied solely on self-report

    measures for all of the symptoms and parenting constructs,

    and we found that childrens report of aspects of parenting

    were primarily related to their self-reported symptoms. On

    the one hand, this is an important finding as it indicates that

    childrens perceptions of parenting behaviors (particularly

    acceptance/rejection) are more independently related to

    their internalizing symptoms, relative to mothers report of

    parenting behaviors (Fentz et al. 2011; Festen et al. 2013;

    J Child Fam Stud

    1 3

  • 8/13/2019 Aspects of Mothers Parenting Independent and Specific INGLES

    12/15

    Greco and Morris2002; Hale et al. 2006; Khaleque2012;

    Khaleque and Rohner 2002a). However, by not including

    other methods of measurement (e.g., observations, inter-

    views, etc.), we may have had inaccurate self-reports of

    negative parenting behaviors (which can often happen,

    particularly with mothers; Rohner and Khaleque2005) and

    of childrens symptoms. Thus, future studies should

    include more observational measures in addition to othersreports such as those of fathers and other primary care-

    givers whose parenting behaviors have a substantial impact

    on children (Bogels and Perotti 2011; Greco and Morris

    2002).

    The oversimplified classification of childrens race

    (Caucasian vs. all others) is also a limitation of the current

    study. Different racial and ethnic groups might experience

    and display symptoms and parenting behaviors differently

    (Deater-Deckard et al. 2011; Stewart et al. 2012), so by

    including race as merely Caucasian versus all others,

    these important differences are collapsed and minimized.

    Additionally, although we controlled for current and pasttreatment status for both mothers and children, we failed to

    control for mothers psychopathology. This is an important

    variable to consider when examining childrens internal-

    izing symptomatology, as it is implicated in theoretical

    models of depression, anxiety, and social anxiety and has

    been found to be directly related to childrens symptoms

    or, at the very least, interacting with other environmental

    factors (such as parenting) related to childrens symptoms

    (see Drake and Ginsburg2012; Epkins and Heckler 2011,

    for a review). Our sample size also precluded analysis of

    any gender differences, though we recognized child sex as

    an important demographic variable and controlled for it inthe main analyses. Thus, the examination of racial/ethnic

    differences, sex differences, and the inclusion of parental

    psychopathology are all important avenues for future

    research in this area.

    Community children were sampled, and future studies

    might examine similar questions in clinical samples where

    stronger relations between parenting constructs and chil-

    drens internalizing symptoms might emerge. Finally,

    long-term prospective studies (ideally with more observa-

    tional measures) are needed to assess the relations among

    various aspects of parenting (i.e., acceptance/rejection,

    behavioral control, and others) and childrens symptoms of

    depression, anxiety and social anxiety over time. Indeed,

    stemming from interpersonal and self-determination theo-

    ries (Rudolph et al.2008; Soenens and Vansteenkiste2010)

    as well as Rapees (2001) developmental model of anxiety,

    there is some empirical support for reciprocal or bidirec-

    tional relations between aspects of parenting and childrens

    depression (Branje et al. 2010; Hipwell et al. 2008), anx-

    iety (Ginsburg et al. 2004b; Hudson et al. 2009; Settipani

    et al.2013; Silverman et al.2009; Wijsbroek et al. 2011),

    and social anxiety (Van Zalk and Kerr 2011). Thus, future

    longitudinal studies would allow for the elucidation of

    parenting constructs as antecedents, correlates, or conse-

    quences of childrens symptoms.

    In addition to parenting constructs, theoretical models of

    childrens internalizing symptoms have also implicated

    certain biological vulnerabilities such as genetics or tem-

    peramental factors, in addition to other psychosocial vul-nerabilities such as various learning experiences and peer

    victimization. Some studies have found these to be mod-

    erators of the relation between parenting constructs and

    childrens internalizing symptoms (see Epkins and Heckler

    2011, for a review; Kiff et al. 2011).

    Our results, nonetheless, indicate that perceived maternal

    rejection is related to childrens self-reported depression,

    anxiety, and social anxiety symptoms (more so to depression

    and anxiety). Furthermore, mothers report of rejection was

    related to mothers report of childrens affective problems,

    and both mothers and childrens report of rejection was

    related to mothers report of childrens anxiety problems.Importantly, childrens perceived maternal rejection

    remained a specific and unique predictor of childrens self-

    reported depressive symptoms after controlling for comor-

    bid symptoms. Thus, the construct of perceived parental

    acceptance/rejection is an important one to consider in

    treatments for childrens depression. Finally, although

    childrens report of maternal behavioral control was related

    to mothers report of childrens anxiety problems, behav-

    ioral control was neither an independent nor a specific

    predictor of childrens internalizing symptoms. Behavioral

    control may possibly be an important aspect of parenting to

    consider, particularly in treatments for childrens anxiety,but more research employing different methods and meth-

    odologies should be conducted in order to fully elucidate

    this construct with respect to childrens internalizing

    symptoms.

    Acknowledgments This paper is based, in part, on the masters-levelresearch project of the first author, directed by the second author. Wethank David Heckler, Matt Carroll, Shannon Kelly, and Jessica Clarkfor assistance with data collection and data management.

    Referenecs

    Abela, J. Z., & Hankin, B. L. (2008). Depression in children andadolescents: Causes, treatment, and prevention. In J. Z. Abela &B. L. Hankin (Eds.), Handbook of depression in children andadolescents(pp. 35). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBAschool-age forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University ofVermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.

    American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statisticalmanual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Amer-ican Psychiatric Association.

    J Child Fam Stud

    1 3

  • 8/13/2019 Aspects of Mothers Parenting Independent and Specific INGLES

    13/15

    Anderson, E. R., & Mayes, L. C. (2010). Race/ethnicity andinternalizing disorders in youth: A review. Clinical Psychology

    Review, 30, 338348. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.12.008.Ballash, N., Leyfer, O., Buckley, A. F., & Woodruff-Borden, J.

    (2006). Parental control in the etiology of anxiety. Clinical Childand Family Psychology Review, 9, 113133. doi:10.1007/s10567-006-0007-z.

    Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: Revisiting aneglected construct.Child Development, 67, 32963319. doi:10.

    2307/1131780.Beck, J. S., Beck, A. T., Jolly, J. B., & Steer, R. A. (2005). Beck youth

    inventories for children and adolescents manual (2nd ed.). SanAntonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.

    Beesdo, K., Pine, D. S., Lieb, R., & Wittchen, H. (2010). Incidenceand risk patterns of anxiety and depressive disorders andcategorization of generalized anxiety disorder. Archives ofGeneral Psychiatry, 67, 4757. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.177.

    Bogels, S. M., & Perotti, E. C. (2011). Does father know best? Aformal model of the paternal influence on childhood socialanxiety. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 20, 171181.doi:10.1007/s10826-010-9441-0.

    Branje, S. T., Hale, W., Frijns, T., & Meeus, W. J. (2010).Longitudinal associations between perceived parent-child rela-tionship quality and depressive symptoms in adolescence.

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 751763. doi:10.1007/s10802-010-9401-6.

    Breinholst, S., Esbjrn, B. H., Reinholdt-Dunne, M., & Stallard, P.(2012). CBT for the treatment of child anxiety disorders: Areview of why parental involvement has not enhanced outcomes.

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 26, 416424. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.12.014.

    Burkhouse, K., Uhrlass, D., Stone, L., Knopik, V., & Gibb, B. (2012).Expressed emotion-criticism and risk of depression onset inchildren. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,41, 771777.

    Cartwright-Hatton, S., McNicol, K., & Doubleday, E. (2006). Anxietyin a neglected population: Prevalence of anxiety disorders in pre-adolescent children. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 817833.doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2005.12.002.

    Chavira, D. A., Stein, M. B., Bailey, K., & Stein, M. T. (2004).Comorbidity of generalized social anxiety disorder and depres-sion in a pediatric primary care sample. Journal of Affective

    Disorders, 80, 163171. doi:10.1016/S0165-0327(03)00103-4.Chorpita, B. F., & Barlow, D. H. (1998). The development of anxiety:

    The role of control in the early environment. PsychologicalBulletin, 124, 321. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.124.1.3.

    Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Appliedmultiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sci-

    ences(3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Crawley, S. A., Beidas, R. S., Benjamin, C. L., Martin, E., & Kendall,

    P. C. (2008). Treating socially phobic youth with CBT:Differential outcomes and treatment considerations. Behavioural

    and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 36, 379389. doi:10.1017/S1352465808004542.Dallaire, D. H., Pineda, A. Q., Cole, D. A., Ciesla, J. A., Jacquez, F.,

    LaGrange, B., et al. (2006). Relation of positive and negativeparenting to childrens depressive symptoms. Journal of ClinicalChild and Adolescent Psychology, 35, 313322. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3502_15.

    Deater-Deckard, K., Lansford, J. E., Malone, P. S., Alampay, L.,Sorbring, E., Bacchini, D., et al. (2011). The association betweenparental warmth and control in thirteen cultural groups. Journalof Family Psychology, 25, 790794. doi:10.1037/a0025120.

    Drake, K. L., & Ginsburg, G. S. (2011). Parenting practices ofanxious and nonanxious mothers: A multi-method, multi-

    informant approach. Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 33,299321. doi:10.1080/07317107.2011.623101.

    Drake, K. L., & Ginsburg, G. S. (2012). Family factors in thedevelopment, treatment, and prevention of childhood anxietydisorders. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 15,144162. doi:10.1007/s10567-011-0109-0.

    Epkins, C. C. (2002). A comparison of two self-report measures ofchildrens social anxiety in clinical and community samples.Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31, 6979. doi:10.

    1002/jcop.1051.Epkins, C. C., & Heckler, D. R. (2011). Integrating etiological models

    of social anxiety and depression in youth: Evidence for acumulative interpersonal risk model. Journal of Clinical Childand Family Psychology Review, 14, 329376. doi:10.1007/s10567-011-0101-8.

    Essau, C., Conradt, J., & Petermann, F. (1999). Frequency andcomorbidity of social phobia and social fears in adolescents.

    Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 831843. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00179-X.

    Fentz, H. N., Arendt, M., OToole, M. S., Rosenberg, N. K., &Hougaard, E. (2011). The role of depression in perceivedparenting style among patients with anxiety disorders. Journal of

    Anxiety Disorders, 25, 10951101. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.07.008.

    Festen, H., Hartman, C. A., Hogendoorn, S., de Haan, E., Pier, P.,Reichart, C. G., et al. (2013). Temperament and parentingpredicting anxiety change in cognitive behavioral therapy: Therole of mothers, fathers, and children. Journal of Anxiety

    Disorders, 27, 289297. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.03.001.Gere, M. K., Villabo, M. A., Torgersen, S., & Kendall, P. C. (2012).

    Overprotective parenting and child anxiety: The role of co-occurring child behavior problems. Journal of Anxiety Disor-ders, 26, 642649. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.04.003.

    Ginsburg, G. S., Grover, R. L., & Ialongo, N. (2004a). Parentingbehaviors among anxious and non-anxious mothers: Relationwith concurrent and long-term child outcomes.Child & Family

    Behavior Therapy, 26, 2341. doi:10.1300/J019v26n04_02.Ginsburg, G. S., & Schlossberg, M. C. (2002). Family-based

    treatment of childhood anxiety disorders. International Reviewof Psychiatry, 14, 143154. doi:10.1080/09540260220132662.

    Ginsburg, G. S., Siqueland, L., Masia-Warner, C., & Hedtke, K. A.(2004b). Anxiety disorders in children: Family matters. Cogni-tive and Behavioral Practice, 11, 2843. doi:10.1016/S1077-7229(04)80005-1.

    Greco, L. A., & Morris, T. L. (2002). Paternal child-rearing style andchild social anxiety: Investigation of child perceptions and actualfather behavior. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral

    Assessment, 24, 259267. doi:10.1023/A:1020779000183.Hale, W., Engels, R., & Meeus, W. (2006). Adolescents perceptions

    of parenting behaviours and its relationship to adolescentGeneralized Anxiety Disorder symptoms. Journal of Adoles-cence, 29, 407417. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.002.

    Hipwell, A., Keenan, K., Kasza, K., Loeber, R., Stouthamer-Loeber,

    M., & Bean, T. (2008). Reciprocal influences between girlsconduct problems and depression, and parental punishment andwarmth: A six year prospective analysis. Journal of AbnormalChild Psychology, 36, 663677. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9206-4.

    Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four factor index of social status.Unpublished manuscript. New Haven, CT: Yale University.

    Hudson, J. L., Doyle, A. M., & Gar, N. (2009). Child and maternalinfluence on parenting behavior in clinically anxious children.

    Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 38,256262. doi:10.1080/15374410802698438.

    Hudson, J. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2001). Parent-child interactionsand anxiety disorders: An observational study. Behaviour

    J Child Fam Stud

    1 3

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.12.008http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-006-0007-zhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-006-0007-zhttp://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131780http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131780http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.177http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.177http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10826-010-9441-0http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9401-6http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9401-6http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.12.014http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.12.014http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2005.12.002http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(03)00103-4http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.1.3http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1352465808004542http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1352465808004542http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3502_15http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3502_15http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025120http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07317107.2011.623101http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0109-0http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jcop.1051http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jcop.1051http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0101-8http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0101-8http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00179-Xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00179-Xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.07.008http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.07.008http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.03.001http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.04.003http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J019v26n04_02http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09540260220132662http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1077-7229(04)80005-1http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1077-7229(04)80005-1http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1020779000183http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.002http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-007-9206-4http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-007-9206-4http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15374410802698438http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15374410802698438http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-007-9206-4http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-007-9206-4http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.002http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1020779000183http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1077-7229(04)80005-1http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1077-7229(04)80005-1http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09540260220132662http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J019v26n04_02http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.04.003http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.03.001http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.07.008http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.07.008http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00179-Xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00179-Xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0101-8http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0101-8http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jcop.1051http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jcop.1051http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0109-0http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07317107.2011.623101http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025120http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3502_15http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3502_15http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1352465808004542http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1352465808004542http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.1.3http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(03)00103-4http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2005.12.002http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.12.014http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.12.014http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9401-6http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9401-6http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10826-010-9441-0http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.177http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.177http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131780http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131780http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-006-0007-zhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-006-0007-zhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.12.008
  • 8/13/2019 Aspects of Mothers Parenting Independent and Specific INGLES

    14/15

    Research and Therapy, 39, 14111427. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00107-8.

    Hudson, J. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2009). Familial and socialenvironments in the etiology and maintenance of anxietydisorders. In M. M. Antony & M. B. Stein (Eds.), Oxfordhandbook of anxiety and related disorders (pp. 173189). NewYork, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Hutcherson, S. T., & Epkins, C. C. (2009). Differentiating parent- andpeer-related interpersonal correlates of depressive symptoms and

    social anxiety in preadolescent girls. Journal of Social andPersonal Relationships, 26, 875897. doi:10.1177/0265407509345654.

    Johnson, H. S., Inderbitzen-Nolan, H. M., & Schapman, A. M. (2005).A comparison between socially anxious and depressive symp-tomatology in youth: A focus on perceived family environment.

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19, 423442. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.04.004.

    Kearney, C. A. (2005). The etiology of social anxiety and socialphobia in youths. In C. A. Kearney (Ed.), Social anxiety andsocial phobia in youth: Characteristics, assessment, and psy-

    chological treatment(pp. 4970). New York, NY: Springer.Kessler, R. C., & Wang, P. S. (2008). The descriptive epidemiology

    of commonly occurring mental disorders in the United States.Annual Review of Public Health, 29, 115129. doi:10.1146/annurev.publhealth.29.020907.090847.

    Khaleque, A. (2012). Perceived parental warmth, and childrenspsychological adjustment, and personality dispositions: A meta-analysis. Journal of Child and Family Studies,. doi:10.I007/sI0826-012-9579-z.

    Khaleque, A., & Rohner, R. P. (2002a). Perceived parental accep-tance-rejection and psychological adjustment: A meta-analysisof cross-cultural and intracultural studies. Journal of Marriageand Family, 64, 5464. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2002.00054.x.

    Khaleque, A., & Rohner, R. P. (2002b). Reliability of measuresassessing the pancultural association between perceived parentalacceptance-rejection and psychological adjustment: A meta-analysis of cross-cultural and intracultural studies. Journal ofCross-Cultural Psychology, 33, 8799. doi:10.1177/0022022102033001006.

    Kiff, C. J., Lengua, L. J., & Bush, N. R. (2011). Temperamentvariation in sensitivity to parenting: Predicting changes indepression and anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,39, 11991212. doi:10.1007/s10802-011-9539-x.

    Knappe, S., Beesdo-Baum, K., Fehm, L., Lieb, R., & Wittchen, H.(2012). Characterizing the association between parenting andadolescent social phobia. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 26,608616. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.02.014.

    Koestner, R., Zuroff, D. C., & Powers, T. A. (1991). Family origins ofadolescent self-criticism and its continuity into adulthood.

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 191197. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.100.2.191.

    La Greca, A. M. (1999). Social anxiety scales for children andadolescents: Manual and instructions for the SASC, SASC-R,

    SAS-A (adolescents), and parent versions of the scales. Miami,FL: University of Miami, Department of Psychology.McGuire, J., & Earls, F. (1993). Exploring the reliability of measures

    of family relations, parental attitudes, and parent-child relationsin a disadvantaged minority population.Journal of Marriage andthe Family, 55, 10421046. doi:10.2307/352785.

    McLeod, B. D., Weisz, J. R., & Wood, J. J. (2007a). Examining theassociation between parenting and childhood depression: Ameta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 9861003.doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.03.001.

    McLeod, B. D., Wood, J. J., & Weisz, J. R. (2007b). Examining theassociation between parenting and childhood anxiety: A meta-

    analysis.Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 155172. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.002.

    Mills, R. S., & Rubin, K. H. (1998). Are behavioural and psycho-logical control both differentially associated with childhoodaggression and social withdrawal? Canadian Journal of Behav-ioural Science, 30, 132136. doi:10.1037/h0085803.

    Morris, T. L., & Masia, C. L. (1998). Psychometric evaluation of thesocial Phobia and Anxiety Inventory For Children: Concurrentvalidity and normative data. Journal of Clinical Child Psychol-ogy, 27, 452458. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp2704_9.

    Muris, P., & Meesters, C. (2002). Symptoms of anxiety disorders andteacher-reported school functioning of normal children. Psycho-logical Reports, 91, 588590. doi:10.2466/PR0.91.6.588-590.

    Muris, P., & Merckelbach, H. (1998). Perceived parental rearingbehaviour and anxiety disorders symptoms in normal children.Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 11991206. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00153-6.

    ONeil, K. A., Podell, J. L., Benjamin, C. L., & Kendall, P. C. (2010).Comorbid depressive disorders in anxiety-disordered youth:Demographic, clinical, and family characteristics. Child Psychi-atry and Human Development, 41, 330341. doi:10.1007/s10578-009-0170-9.

    Ollendick, T. H., & Benoit, K. E. (2012). A parentchild interactionalmodel of social anxiety disorder in youth. Clinical Child andFamily Psychology Review, 15, 8191. doi:10.1007/s10567-011-0108-1.

    Rapee, R. M. (1997). Potential role of childrearing practices in thedevelopment of anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychology

    Review, 17, 4767. doi:10.1016/S0272-7358(96)00040-2.Rapee, R. M. (2001). The development of generalized anxiety. In M.

    W. Vasey & M. R. Dadds (Eds.), The developmental psycho-pathology of anxiety (pp. 481503). New York, NY: OxfordUniversity Press.

    Rapee, R. M. (2012). Family factors in the development andmanagement of anxiety disorders. Clinical Child and FamilyPsychology Review, 15, 6980. doi:10.1007/s10567-011-0106-3.

    Rapee, R. M., Schniering, C. A., & Hudson, J. L. (2009). Anxietydisorders during childhood and adolescence: Origins and treat-ment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 5, 311341.doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.032408.153628.

    Rapee, R. M., & Spence, S. (2004). The etiology of social phobia:Empirical evidence and an initial model. Clinical Psychology

    Review, 24, 737767. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2004.06.004.Reijntjes, A., Dekovic, M., & Telch, M. J. (2007). Support for the

    predictive validity of the SASC-R: Linkages with reactions to anin vivo peer evaluation manipulation. Journal of Anxiety

    Disorders, 21, 903917. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.10.007.Rohner, R. P. (1986/1999). The warmth dimension: Foundations of

    parental acceptance-rejection theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: SagePublications, Inc. (Reprinted 1999; Storrs, CT: RohnerResearch).

    Rohner, R. P., & Khaleque, A. (2003). Reliability and validity of theParental Control Scale: A meta-analysis of cross-cultural and

    intracultural studies. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34,643649. doi:10.1177/0022022103255650.Rohner, R. P., & Khaleque, A. (Eds.). (2005).Handbook for the study

    of parental acceptance and rejection (4th ed.). Storrs, CT:Rohner Research Publications.

    Rork, K. E., & Morris, T. L. (2009). Influence of parenting factors onchildhood social anxiety: Direct observation of parental warmthand control. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 31, 220235.doi:10.1080/07317100903099274.

    Rubin, K. H., Nelson, L. J., Hastings, P., & Asendorpf, J. (1999). Thetransaction between parents perceptions of their childrensshyness and their parenting styles. International Journal of

    J Child Fam Stud

    1 3

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00107-8http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00107-8http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407509345654http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407509345654http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.04.004http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.04.004http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.publhealth.29.020907.090847http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.publhealth.29.020907.090847http://dx.doi.org/10.I007/sI0826-012-9579-zhttp://dx.doi.org/10.I007/sI0826-012-9579-zhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2002.00054.xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022102033001006http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022102033001006http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9539-xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.02.014http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.100.2.191http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.100.2.191http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/352785http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2007.03.001http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.002http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.002http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0085803http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp2704_9http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/PR0.91.6.588-590http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00153-6http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00153-6http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10578-009-0170-9http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10578-009-0170-9http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0108-1http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0108-1http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(96)00040-2http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0106-3http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.032408.153628http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2004.06.004http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.10.007http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022103255650http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07317100903099274http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07317100903099274http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022103255650http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.10.007http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2004.06.004http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.032408.153628http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0106-3http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(96)00040-2http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0108-1http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0108-1http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10578-009-0170-9http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10578-009-0170-9http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00153-6http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00153-6http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/PR0.91.6.588-590http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp2704_9http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0085803http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.002http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.002http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2007.03.001http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/352785http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.100.2.191http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.100.2.191http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.02.014http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9539-xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022102033001006http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022102033001006http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2002.00054.xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.I007/sI0826-012-9579-zhttp://dx.doi.org/10.I007/sI0826-012-9579-zhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.publhealth.29.020907.090847http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.publhealth.29.020907.090847http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.04.004http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.04.004http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407509345654http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407509345654http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00107-8http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00107-8
  • 8/13/2019 Aspects of Mothers Parenting Independent and Specific INGLES

    15/15

    Behavioral Development, 23, 937958. doi:10.1080/016502599383612.

    Rudolph, K. D., Flynn, M., & Abaied, J. L. (2008). A developmentalperspective on interpersonal theories of youth depression. In J.Z. Abela & B. L. Hankin (Eds.), Handbook of depression inchildren and adolescents(pp. 79102). New York, NY: GuilfordPress.

    Sanna, K., Rachel, P., Hanna, E., Tuula, H., Leena, J., Marja-Leena,M., et al. (2009). Psychometric evaluation of social Phobia and

    Anxiety Inventory for Children (SPAI-C) and Social AnxietyScale for Children-Revised (SASC-R). European Child and

    Adolescent Psychiatry, 18, 116124. doi:10.1007/s00787-008-0712-x.

    Schwartz, O. S., Dudgeon, P., Sheeber, L. B., Yap, M. H., Simmons,J. G., & Allen, N. B. (2012). Parental behaviors during familyinteractions predict changes in depression and anxiety symptomsduring adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40,5971. doi:10.1007/s10802-011-9542-2.

    Seligman, L. D., & Ollendick, T. H. (1998). Comorbidity of anxietyand depression in children and adolescents: An integrativereview. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 1,125144. doi:10.1023/A:1021887712873.

    Settipani, C. A., ONeil, K. A., Podell, J. L., Beidas, R. S., & Kendall,P. C. (2013). Youth anxiety and parent factors over time:Directionality of change among youth treated for anxiety.

    Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 42,921. doi:10.1080/15374416.2012.719459.

    Sheeber, L. B., Davis, B., Leve, C., Hops, H., & Tildesley, E. (2007).Adolescents relationships with their mothers and fathers:Associations with depressive disorder and subdiagnostic symp-tomatology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 144154.doi:10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.144.

    Silverman, W. K., Kurtines, W. M., Jaccard, J., & Pina, A. A. (2009).Directionality of change in youth anxiety treatment involvingparents: An initial examination. Journal of Consulting andClinical Psychology, 77, 474485. doi:10.1037/a0015761.

    Siqueland, L., Kendall, P. C., & Steinberg, L. (1996). Anxiety inchildren: Perceived family environments and observed familyinteraction. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 25, 225237.doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp2502_12.

    Soenens, B., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2010). A theoretical upgrade of theconcept of parental psychological control: Proposing newinsights