18
8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 1/18 Chapter Aspect: Problem  of lexicon and  morphology In  this chapter  we  outline several  of the  influential linguistic analyses  of aspect in the literature, and examine how these analyses treat grammatical aspect, lexical aspect, and the interaction between the two. We  will  begin by  looking  at  Comrie s  analysis  of  perfective  and imperfective  aspect, an d  then  proceed  to  Vendler s  categorization of verbs and times,  Smith s two-component theory  of  aspect,  and  Klein s  view  on  aspect  in  terms  of the relationship between topic time and situation time. We end  with  a discussion  of how  these various linguistic analyses bear  on  children s acquisition of aspect. 2 1 rammatical  aspect 2.1.1.  Perfective  and  imperfective The grammatical encoding of aspectual notions, which we call  grammati cal  aspect is  realized  in  different  languages  in  different  ways,  for  exam- ple, through the use of inflectional morphology, derivational morphol- ogy, auxiliary,  or  periphrastic constructions. This variation does  not mean, however, that grammatical aspect  is  wholly idiosyncratic  and language-specific in the way it is  encoded. Typological studies  of how languages of the world encode aspectual notions have uncovered recur- ring patterns of aspectual marking  (Comrie  1976; Bybee 1985;  Dahl 1985; Bybee and Dahl 1989; Bybee, Perkins, and Pagliuca  1994).  In this section,  we  characterize these grammatical aspectual patterns  and  show how  they  are  related  to  each other. The  most basic aspectual opposition that  is  often  encoded grammati- cally  is  that  of  perfective  and  imperfective.  As  noted  in  Chapter  1, perfective  aspect presents  a  situation  as an  unanalyzed whole (external view),  whereas imperfective aspect presents  a  situation  from  within (internal view).  In the  following example  from  English, (1)  John built  a  house. (2)  John was building a house. Unauthenticated Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 1/18

Chapter

 

Aspect: Problem

 of lexicon and

 morphology

In   this chapter

  we

  outline several

  of the

  influential l inguistic analyses

  of

aspect in the literature, and examine how these analyses treat grammatical

aspect, lexical aspect, and the interaction between the two. We   will   begin

by   looking   at   Comrie s   analysis   of   perfective   and imperfective   aspect,

an d   then   proceed   to   Ven dler s   categorization of verbs and times,   Smith s

two-component theory

  of

  aspect,

  and   Kle in s   view   on

  aspect

  in

  terms

  of

the relationship between topic time an d situation time. W e end

  with

  a

discussion

  of how

  these various linguistic analyses bear

  on

  ch i ld ren s

acquisition of aspect.

2 1 rammatical  aspect

2.1.1. Perfective  an d   imperfective

The grammatical encoding of aspectual notions, which we call  grammati

cal

  aspect is

  realized

  in

  different

  languages

  in

  different

  ways,

 for

  exam-

ple, throug h the use of inflectional morphology, derivation al m orp ho l-

ogy, auxiliary,

  or

  periphrastic constructions. This variation does

  not

mean, however, that grammatical aspect

  is

  wholly idiosyncratic

  and

language-specific in the way it is   encoded. Typo logical studies   of how

languages

  of the

  world encode aspectual notions have uncovered recur-

ring patterns of aspectual marking   (Comrie   1976; Bybee 1985;   Dah l

1985; Bybee and Dahl 1989; Bybee, Perkins, and Pagliuca  1994).   In this

section,

  we

  characterize these grammatical aspectual patterns

  and

  show

how

  they   are   related

  to

  each other.

The

  most basic aspectual opposition that

  is

  often

  encoded grammat i -

cally   is   that   of   perfective   an d   imperfective.   As   noted   in   Chapter   1,

perfective

  aspect presents

  a

  situation

  as an

  unan alyzed whole (external

view),  whereas imperfective aspect presents   a   situation   from   within

(internal view).  In the   following example   from   English,

(1 )

  John built  a   house.

(2 )  John was building a house.

Unauthenticated

Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Page 2: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 2/18

12   Aspect

sentence (1) is aspectually perfective, whereas sentence (2) is aspectually

imperfect ve. In

  sentence

  (1), the

  perfective aspect

  of the

  verb

  reports  th e

situation in its entirety, w ith the speaker presenting the situation in clu d-

ing

  it s  initial  and terminal points. Thus, sentence (1) entails completion:

John

  finished

  building a house. In contrast, the

  imperfective

  aspect in

sentence (2) communicates the internal structure of the event, without

regard

  to its

  beginning

  and end

  points. Thus

  th e

  meaning

  of the

  sentence

is

  non-committal

  as to

  whether

  or not

  John

  has

  finished building

  th e

house .

Comrie

  (1976:  25)  proposed  th e  follow ing hierarchical classification

of aspectual categories.

 

Perfective Imperfective

 

Habitual

  (Continuous)

(Non-progressive) Progressive

Figure 2.1.  Classification

 of

 aspectual opp ositions (adapted  from  Comrie 1976:

  25 )

Comrie

  divided imperfective aspect into habitual

  and

  continuous,

  and

further

  subdivided continuous into progressive

  and

  non-progressive.

  Let

us  characterize these categories  from  th e  bottom  up,  following  Comrie

(1976).

Progressive  aspect  is a category  of  imperfective that  has the  properties

of dynam icity and change as its defining features: typically it denotes a

dynamic, continuously changing action in progress, and is generally

incompatible with Stative predicates  *He  is  knowing  th e  answer).

Habitual

  aspect denotes

  a

  situation that spans

  an

 extended  period

  of

time, typic ally inv olv ing repetition of an action over multiple occasions.

English

  has a

  habitual aspect marker  used to)  which

  is

  used

  in

  past-time

reference only.

Imperfective  aspect, some times referred  to as  general  imperfective,

denotes both habitual

  and

  continuous  i.e.,  progressive

  and

  non-

progressive) qualities.

  For

  example, Romance languages grammatically

encode

  th e

  perfective-imperfective distinction

  in the

  past,

  and the

  imper-

fective  past  can  describe  a  past action  in  progress (i.e., progressive),  a

Unauthenticated

Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Page 3: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 3/18

Grammatical aspect

  13

past  state i.e.,

  non-progressive),

6

  and a past habit.  For  instance, the

Spanish imperfective past form can express the equivalents of

  he was

dancing

he

  loved Mary

and   he

  used

  to

  dance?

It

  is interesting to note here that

  Comrie

  failed  to explicitly character-

ize the  categories  of continuous and non-progressive. He defined  con-

tinuousness

  in the

  negative, that

  is, as

  imperfectivity  that

  is not

  habitual ,

and he did not define  non-progressive Bybee, Perkins, and Pagliuca

1994: 138). T his m ay be related to the  fact  that the aspectual markers

that

  specifically  denote  these categories

  are

  extremely

  rare.

8

  For

  this

reason, the two categories appear in parenthesis in Figure   2.1.

2.1.2. Grammaticization

 of

  aspect markers

A s

  noted

  in

  Chapter

  1,

 there

  is a

  tendency

  for the

  meanings

  and

  func -

tions  of tense-aspect markers to change over time. For example, it is

claimed that progressive aspect markers can develop into   more  general

imperfective aspect markers by generalizing their applicability to habit-

ual

  and

  non-progressive situations.

  In

  this subsection,

  we

  discuss gram-

maticization of tense-aspect markers to explain how   different  tense-

aspect

  markers

  are

  related

  to

  each other, both semantically

  and

  histori-

cally.

Building  on

  research

  from

  historical

  linguistics  and

  crosslinguistic

survey  of typologically diverse languages, Bybee, Perkins, and Pagliuca

  1994) proposed hypotheses concerning the gram m aticization of tense-

aspect-m odality markers. Pa rticularly relevant to our research are two

hypotheses concerning the universal paths of the developm ent of tense-

aspect markers. According to these authors,  first,  there is a unive rsal

tendency for  resultative  and  completive markers  to  grammaticize into

perfect

  markers, which

  in turn

  become perfective

  or

  simple past markers.

Second,  there is a tende ncy for a progressive ma rker to de velop into a

general imperfective marker

  by

  expanding

  it s

 reference

  to

  habi tual

  and

stative

  situations; in terms of Comrie's  hierarchy see Figure 2.1), imper-

fective

  aspect markers develop from  th e  bottom

  up.

9

Here,

  we

  would like

  to

  discuss

  th e

  perfective path

  in

  more detail,

primarily  to explicate the nature of the category  perfect.  Bybee, Perkins,

and

  Pagliuca  1994)  claimed that comp letive and resultative aspect

markers grammaticize into perfect markers. Completive markers denote

an  action performed completely

  and

  thoroughly ,

  and

  resultative

 denotes

a state that has been brought about by a prior action. These markers then

Unauthenticated

Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Page 4: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 4/18

14  Aspect

extend their meaning  and  become perfect markers, which denote

  past

action  with

  current relevance . English perfect  have  +  past participle)  is

a typical example of  this  category (Dahl 1985), wh ich has such uses as

  perfect of result , perfect of

 experience ,

  etc. Perfect  further  g ram-

maticizes  and  loses  its  curr ent releva nce restriction,  and  becomes  a

perfective

  aspect marker

  and/or

  a simple past tense marker.

It  is often  the case that a past tense marker also has an aspectual value.

For example, many European languages (e.g., French, Spanish, German,

Dutch) have  th e  perfect form gramm aticizing into  a  past

  tense  form

which  also has a perfective value, although the degree  of  grammaticiza-

tion  differs.  Thus,

 in

 these languages, they have

 two

 past tense ma rkers

  -

older past forms

  (often

  referred to as  preterite),  and more recent past

forms

  (auxiliary plus participle).  As  noted  in  Chapter  1, what  we see

 here

is a continuous development of tense-aspect markers, which  often  makes

it  very

  difficult

  to  explicitly determine whether  a  grammatical form  is a

tense marker or an aspect marker.

Interestingly, this historical developm ent charted for tense-aspect

markers has been  found  to have a parallel in language acquisition. In

Chapters 3 through 6, we will  discuss in more detail the claim that chil-

dren use

  past tense marking

  first  as a resultative

  aspect marker

  and

  then

as  a pure past tense marker, and that progressive markers  are  initially

restricted  to the  action-in-progress meaning  and  only later develop  more

varied meanings

  (e.g.,

  Antinucci

  and

  Miller 1976; Bloom, Lifter,

  and

Hafitz

  1980; Shirai

  an d

  Andersen  1995).  This claim about

  the

  acquisi-

tion  of tense-aspect morphology has also been discussed within the

larger issue of how and why there are parallel  processes  in language

acquisition  and  historical change (Slobin 1977, 1997;  Ziegeler 1997).

  Lexical Aspect:  Vendler Smith and   Comrie

Gram m atical aspect provides a certain am ount of inform ation for the

interpretation of the aspectual meaning of verbal predicates, but most

linguists

  accept that

 we

 need

 to

 consider another type

  of

  aspect

 -

  lexical

aspect. Lexical aspect (also known  as  inherent aspect, situation aspect,  or

Aktionsart) refers to the semantic characteristics inherent in the lexical

content of words, usually  verbs or verb phrases, that are defined in terms

of the temporal properties of given situations that the verbs describe.

Because lexical aspect deals with lexical semantics,

  it is

  generally  re-

Unauthenticated

Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Page 5: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 5/18

Lexical aspect

  15

garded

  as a

  lexical category

  as

  opposed

  to the

  grammatical category

  of

  grammatical

  aspect .

As a starting point for our illustration, Ve ndler s (195 7) four -w ay

classification represents an early attempt to

 categorize

  lexical aspect.

10

  He

classified

  verbs or verb phrases into

  four

  categories

  with

  respect to the

temporal properties that they encode:

  activities, accomplishments,

achievements,  and   states.  According to Vendler,

  activity

  verbs

  encode

situations as consisting of successive phases over tim e w ith no inh ere nt

endpoint, for example,  walk,  run,  an d   swim.  Accomplishment verbs also

characterize situations as hav ing successive phases, but they

  differ

  f rom

activity   verbs

  in

  that they encode

  a

  natural endpoint

  and

  often

  a

  change

of  state,  for example,   paint

  a picture, build a house,

  and

  run a mile.

Vendler illustrated the contrast between activities and accomplishments

by

  the difference between   ru n  and   run a  mile The sentence  John  w as

running

  entails that John ran, whereas

  the

  sentence

  John

  w as

  running

  a

mile does  not entail   that   John ran a mile, that is, John m igh t have stopped

halfway.   Like accomplishments, achievement verbs also   encode  a natural

endpoint,  but   they

  differ

  from   accomplishments   and   activities   in   that

they

  encode  events

  as

  punctual

  and

  instantaneous, that

  is, as

  having

  no

duration, such as in

  fall,

  win the

  race,

  and

  reach

  the

  summit.

  Strictly

speaking, every event occupies time.  But   speakers   can   construe given

verbs as denoting situations having no time duration. Finally, state verbs,

in

  contrast

 to the

  other three

  categories,

  encode situations

  as

 homogene-

ous, with

  no

  successive phases

  or

  endpoints, involving

  no

  dynamici ty ,

such

  as

  know

  and

  love Thus, state verbs cannot

  usually   be

  combined

with  progressive aspect that marks change and development   from   one

phase to the next  (e.g.,

  John  is know ing the story

  is odd).

1 1

  These

categories  can be  schematically represented   as  follows   (Andersen

  1990).

State

  love, contain, know

Activity  run,

 walk, swim

Accomplishment

 

paint

 a

 picture, build

 a

 house

Achievement  X   fall,

  drop, win the  race

In this

  schematization, a

  solid line

  is

  used

  to

  represent states, because

states have no apparent beginnin g point or endpoint and endure ind efi-

nitely unless some external force changes them. The   wavy   lines for

activities and accom plishments indicate the dyn am ic d uratio n of an

action, while   ÷ for   accomplishments   and   achievements represents   a

Unauthenticated

Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Page 6: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 6/18

16   Aspect

punc tua l  point

  of

  change

  of

  state, signa ling telicity (i.e., na tura l en d-

poin t ) .

Ve nd ler s analysis, now proba bly the most w idely accepted and the

best know n, has become an imp ortant starting point for subsequen t

research

  on

  lexical aspect.

  For

  example, Smith

  (1997)

  recently modified

this

  system

  and

  applied

  it to an

  analysis

  of

  English, French, Chinese,

Russian, and   Navajo. Smith s modification involved main ly   th e   addition

of  semelfactive verbs, such as

 cough

tap and   knock   in English and

their equivalents

  in

  other languages.

  She

  argued that semelfactives

resemble achievements   in   being punctual ,   but   differ   from achievements

in   that seme lfactives encode no endpoint. In particular, semelfactives and

achievements behave   differently with   progressive aspect ma rking: when

semelfactives are ma rked with progressive, they are interpreted as

 speci-

fying

  a repeated even t (e.g., coughin g or knoc king several times); wh en

achievements are marked   with   progressive, they are interpreted as ind i-

cating preliminary, detachable stages

  of the

  event rather than

  a  repeated

event (e.g.,  John was reaching the summit   means that John was at a stage

just prior to being at the summit, not that he arrived at the summit several

times).

  In

  addition

  to

  Sm ith, other researchers hav e extended V en dle r s

analysis

  in other ways. For exam ple, instead of u sing a categorical fo u r-

way   classification, some linguists have classified verbs

  as

  pairs

  of

  con -

trasts, such as stative versus dynamic verbs, telic versus atelic verbs, and

punctual versus durative verbs

  (Comrie  1976).  These

  pairs

  of

  contrast

have

  th e

  advantage

  of

  making

  the

  semantic features

  of

  lexical aspect

explicit

  and

  transparent. These contrasts

  can be

  reanalyzed

  as

  features,

for  example, [±dynamic], [±telic],

  and

  [±punctual] .   Table

  2.1

  presents

such

  an analysis, adapted   from   Smith

  (1991),

  to characterize

  Vendler's

four

  categories plus semelfactives.

Table  2.1.  Semantic

  features

  for the

  five

  categories  of

  lexical

  aspect

  (adapted

  from

Smith  1991:  30)

states

  activities acc om plishm ents semelfactives achievements

dynamic

  + + + +

punctual 

telic

  + - +

W e

  should point

  out a   confusion

  that

  has

  been

  in the

  literature

  on

lexical

  aspect since Vendler (1957). Vendler did not use the terms lexical

aspect, Aktion sart, situation aspect, or inherent aspect. He w as sp eaking

Unauthenticated

Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Page 7: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 7/18

Lexical aspect  17

of  time

  schemata .

  However,   it is not   clear whether time schemata   are

part of the lexical semantic contents of verbs, or part of the temporal

properties  of   situations   to   which   the   verbs refer. Admittedly,   it is not

always

 easy to separate lexical semantics from the objects and situation s

to

  which

  th e

  lexical items refer.

  But we

  would like

  to

  make this distinc-

tion here, for reasons that   will become  clear later on. Our intention in this

book  is to   treat lexical aspect   as   lexical  categories  according   to the   kinds

of temporal properties that the lexical items inherently encode, not as

situational  categories  according to the temporal  properties  that  the

situations  typically display.  W e   contend

  that

  situatio nal properties   are not

necessarily reflected directly in lexical semantics, and that the confusion

between the two often leads to negative consequences in the study of

language acquisition (see

  further

  discussion in Chapter 3).

The ab stract concepts of, for ex am ple, states and activities can be

viewed

  as

  universal semantic notions, independent

  of

  particular lan-

guages. In other words, every languag e may have lexical means to

encod e states and activities. How ever, w hic h verbs encode states and

which encode  activities

 i s

  languag e-specific,

  and can be

  determined only

within   the context of a given language. Acc ordingly, children have to

learn to   identify   wh ich verbs encode w hich tempo ral properties of a

situation in the language being acquired. An example   from   Comrie

(1976)  clearly demonstrates the need to disting uish between in her ent

verb  m eanings and temporal p roperties of situations. Acco rding to

Comrie, English

  and

  Portuguese treat perception verbs  (e.g.,  see

hear}

differently.  English treats them

  as

  stative,

  an d

  these verbs consequently

do not accept progressive marking , while Portuguese treats them as no n-

stative, so they can n atura lly accept progressive m ark ing . Take Japanese

fo r  another example. Japanese  does  not have a stative verb correspo nd-

ing   to the

  Engl ish

  know.  In

  order

  to

  express

  a

  notion like

  /   know him

Japanese speakers use the verb

  siru

  which means  'come  to

  k n o w

and

attach to it the durative aspect m arker -teiru to denote the resultative state

after

  coming

  to

  know him. Thus,

  in

  Japanese,

  siru

  is an

  achievement

verb. These examples show that verbs

  in

  different   languages

  m ay

  differ

with respect  to their lexical characteristics even tho ug h they refer to the

same situations. Such crosslinguistic differences show that there is no

one-to-one  mapping between given types of situations and given types of

lexical

  items.

Having emphasized

  the

  importance

  of

  this distinction,

  w e

  must

  ac-

knowledge that

 correspondences

 between  lexical  contents

  and

  situational

Unauthenticated

Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Page 8: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 8/18

18  spect

properties  are not  totally arbitrary. There  are  clear cases where languages

do not  differ.  For example, there is pro bab ly no lang uag e that treats the

verb referring to killing someone as stative, and the verb   jump in any

language has to somehow encode the pu nctu ality of the action as part of

its

  meaning. Thus, while

  it is

  important

  for

  theoretical

  an d

  empirical

reasons to disti ngu ish classifications of situations  from  classifications of

lexical aspect categories, in practice, they

 often

  coincide.

2 3 Lexical aspect

 and

 gramm atical aspect

As discussed above, it is clear that interpretation of the aspectual  proper-

ties

  of a

  sentence cannot

  be

  determined without considering

  the

  contri-

bution  of the lexical aspect of the verb. It is also clear that the aspectual

meaning

  of a sentence can, by and large, be predicted on the basis of

which  gram m atical aspect is com bined with wh ich lexical aspect cate-

gory.

  One

  explicit proposal

  in

  this regard

  is

  that

  of

  Smith

  (1991,

  1997).

In  this section, we present our view of how aspectual m eaning is dete r-

mined as a  function  of the combinatorial properties o f lexical aspect and

grammatical aspect

  on the

  basis

  of

  Smith's theory

  of

  aspect

  and the

grammaticization theory

  of Bybee,

  Perkins,

  an d

  Pagliuca  (1994).

Smith

  (1997)

  proposed a comprehensive theory of aspect, which she

labeled

  as a

  two-component theory .

  The two

  components

  are

  what

she calls situation  type

and viewpoint

  aspect ,  which roughly

correspond to what we have described so far as  lexical aspect and

  grammatical

  aspect ,

  respectively. Smith's theory attempts  to  account

for  diverse aspectual phenomena related to the interaction between

situation type (achievement, accomplishment, activity, state, and

  semel-

factive)  and viewpoint aspect (perfective, imperfective, and neutral)

12

.

W e have been using

  the

  term

  lexical

  aspect

  of

  verbs ,

  but

  strictly

speaking, the lexical aspect valu e is determ ined by both the verb and its

arguments, which Smith

  (1997)

  calls

  th e

  verb constellation. Ex am ples

include  [John love Mary] (state), [John run] (activity), [John run a m ile]

(accomplishment), [John reach the summit] (achievement), and [John

jump] (semelfactive). Note that what is inside [ ] is not a linguistic form

but  th e

  proposition underlying

 it,

  wi thout

 any

  verb morphology

  to

  signal

the viewpoint aspect. In such instances, we are talking about the semantic

structure of

  verb-plus-arguments without

  an y

  value imposed

  by

  gram-

matical aspect.

Unauthenticated

Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Page 9: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 9/18

Lexical and grammatical aspect  19

Another component of the two-component theory is viewpoint aspect.

Smith

  used the

  term  viewpoint

since it is essentially the  speaker 's

choice as to  which aspectual perspective should  be  used.  To  describe  the

fact

  that John ran a mile yesterday, the speaker can  choose  to present it

from  either

  a

  perfective viewpoint  John  ran a  mile)

  or an

  imperfective

viewpoint  John  w as  running  a mile).  If the  story  is about what happ ened

while

  John w as jogging y esterday, imperfective viewpoint wo uld be used.

If  th e  story  is  about what John  did

  after

  he ran a  mile, then perfective

viewpoint would be  used.

13

  The speaker chooses to use a certain combi-

nation of lexical items (with lexical aspect value) and m orph ology

  (with

gramm atical aspect v alue), wh ich results  in a  part icular intended mean-

ing,  such as action-in-progress, completion, stative, etc.

2 3 1 Interaction

  between  lexical aspect

  and grammatical

  aspect

How do particular comb inations of lexical aspect and gram ma tical aspect

result

  in

  particular interpretations

  of a

  sentence? What

  are the

  principles

behind

  it? In

  this subsection,

  we

 present

  an

  account

  of how the

  interac-

tion

  between  the two  levels  of  aspect works.  O ur  account  differs  f rom

Smith 's  in minor details, but the principles are essentially the same.

Imperfective

  and progressive aspect

As

  discussed in

  2.1,

  both general im perfective and progressive m arkers

have  the same viewpoint aspect value, except that the progressive view-

point is  [+dynamic]. Therefore, they show very similar distributions  in

their interaction with lexical aspect. Since imperfective viewpoint presents

a  situation  from

  within,

  disregarding  it s  beginning  or  endpoint ,  it re-

quires duration upon which the  internal view  can be  imposed.  If  there  is

no duration, it is impossible to treat the beginning point or endpoint

separately. This requirement predicts that some achievements are incom-

patible with imperfective aspect,

  as

  seen

  in the

  ungrammatical i ty

  of

  *H e

is   noticing  a  friend.

To see the properties of lexical aspect clearly, we repeat the

  fol lowing

diagram

  from

  section

  2.2

  here,

  with  th e

  addition

  of

  semelfactives.

  The

punctual  poin t of ac tion for seme lfactives is represented by ·, since it is

not

  telic,

  but atelic.

Unauthenticated

Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Page 10: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 10/18

20  Aspect

State  love contain know

Activi ty   run walk

swim

Accompl ishment   ~ — „ ~ ~   X

  paint

 a

 picture

build a house

Achievement  X   fall drop win the race

Semelfactive

  jump knock cough

Although  achievements do not normally allow for an internal view of

imperfective

  aspect, it is sometimes possible to impose an inte rna l view

on an achievement verb by focu sing on the preliminary stages of an

event (i.e.,  th e   process leading   up to the   endpoint).   To   elaborate,  John

was reaching the summit   means that John was at a stage jus t prior to

being

  at the

  summit,

  not

  that

  he was in the

  middle

  of

  reaching

  the

  sum-

mit. As another example,  Joh n is winning the race  is appropriate when

Jo hn s horse is ahead of all other horses in the race, even tho ug h he has

not a ctua lly reached the finishing l ine.

Semelfactive

  is

  another pu nctu al, instantaneou s category that imp lies

no

  durat ion.

 In

  this case,

  th e

  speaker

  can

  only impose

  an

  internal view

  on

the situatio n wh en the du ration is created by repetition. Thus,   John

  is

jumping  normally refers to

  John's

  repetitive acts of jum ping , that is,

multiple  jum ps in a single activity, which Smith  (1997)  called Multiple-

event Activity.

14

In

  contrast to achievements and semelfactives, categories that have

duration,  i .e., activities, accomplishments, and states, can readily accept an

internal  view. Both general imperfective a nd progressive ma rkers y ield

the

  action-in-progress

  meaning w hen attached to activity and accom-

plishment

  verbs, focusing

  on the

  dynamic durat ion

  of the

  si tuation

denoted

  by the

  verbs.

With respect to

  stative

  verbs, the pic ture is a little diffe rent. Stative

verbs do not  usual ly   combine with progressive aspect: a large set of

English stative verbs and their translation equivalents in other languages

do not  norm ally accept progressive marking, includin g psyc hologic al

and cognitive verbs such as   wan t need like love believe and   know

perception verbs such as  see hear and  feel and relational verbs or verbs

of existence such  as   resemble possess have and   be.  The   incompat ibi l i ty

seems

  to be

  that since progressive aspect presents

  a

  situation

  as

  ongoing,

it   requires that

  th e

  situation have successive  phases,  i.e., that

  i t be dy-

nam ic, whereas the cogn itive, perception, and relation or existence verbs

indicate   only   undifferentiated

  and

  hom ogeneou s situations.  Progress

presupposes

  th e

  dynamic development

  of a

  situation. Thus,  progressive

Unauthenticated

Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Page 11: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 11/18

  exical and grammatical  aspect  21

aspect comb ines natu rally with activity and ac com plishm ent verbs, but

not w ith stative verbs. How ever, unde r ce rtain circu ms tances some  Stative

verbs do appear in the progressive, w hich we will call stative  progres-

sive .  For  example, Smith (1983) showed that progressive aspect  can be

used with stative verbs to present a state as an event, such as in   John is

believing in ghosts these days.

  Another example is

  John is being stupid

in

  wh i ch

  th e

  stative progre ssive refers

  to a

  part icular behavior

  of

  J o h n '

 s

at

  th e

  time

  of

  speech,

  in

  contrast

  to

  John is stupid  which refers

  to a

general  characteristic  attributed  to  John.  John   is   being stupid  does  not

make any claim about John's intell igence before or after the moment of

speech,  but  rather that John  is behav ing s tupidly.

General imperfective aspect, in contrast to progressive, is na tur all y

com patible with stative verbs and does not yield special

  m e a n ings .

15

Note that general im perfective has the features of [+intemal view ] and

[ -dynamic] ,  and  therefore,  it can  easily  find  durat ion  in a  state,  and it

does  not trigger the action-in-progress m ean ing because stative verbs

also have the feature

  [-dynamic] .

  The obtained meaning of general

imperfective with stative verbs

  is

  thus  'state  co n t i n u i n g ' ,

  as in  La

  mer

etait  calme

  'the  sea was c a lm '  in

  Frenc h (Sm ith 1997: 197)

  or  Wuli-de

deng kai-zhe  'the  light in the room is on in Chinese.

Perfective aspect

Perfective aspect combines naturally  with  achievement verbs, because by

def ini t ion,  perfective aspect presents a si tuation as a single whole, and

achievement verbs provide ideal instantiation

  of

  such

  a

  v iewpoin t

  in

  t h a t

they  depict pu nctual s i tuat ions a s  single points wi thout internal s t ructure.

Because achievements involve  an  endpoint , thei r combinat ion  with

perfective aspect denotes

  the

  complet ion

  of a

  si tuat ion, al though

  in

  this

case

  th e

  beginning point

  of the

  situation coincides with

  it s

  poin t

  of

complet ion.

Perfective aspect also na tural ly combines wi th accom plishments  and is

norm ally interpreted as indica ting the com pletion of a si tuation, since by

definition  accomplishments incorporate  an  endpoin t ,  and  perfect ive

aspect views

 the

  situation

  as a

  single whole external ly ,  with  both  init ial

and

  terminal points.

  For

  example,

  John built a

  house

  not

  only presents

the building process as a single whole, but also indicates that the house

w as

  indeed finished.

The  not ion  of  complet ion  is not  appl icable  to  uses  of the  perfec t ive

with  activity verbs, since activit ies encode situatio ns with

  no

  i n h e ren t

Unauthenticated

Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Page 12: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 12/18

22

  Aspect

endpoints  (Comrie 1976; Lyons 1977). For example,   John  ran  cannot

indicate

  that John   completed

his

  running.

  To ask   Has John finished

runnin is not

  appropriate unless

  th e

  speaker

  has in

  mind

  in

  advance

some delimiter to the distance or time John ran.

  John ran,

  then, indicates

simply

  that John engaged

  in the

  activity

  of

  running

  for a

  while

  an d

  that

at

  some arbitrary (unsp ecified) point this activity terminated.

  In the

  same

vein, pe rfective aspect   with   semelfactive verbs conveys

  a

  termination

  and

not

  a

  completion

  of the

  situation (e.g.,

  John coughed).

Earlier

  we

 pointed

  out

  that

  the progressive  aspect  is not  compatible

with

  stative verbs because of the verbs lack of dyn amic me anin g.

Perfective

  aspect

  is not

  c ompatible with stative verbs, either. This

  is

because stative verbs

  do not

  include either

  a

  beginning point   or

  an

endpoint in

  their temporal structure. Therefore, perfective aspect, which

includes

  both

  the

  beginning

  and end

  points

  of a situation in its

  focus,

cannot  normally combine with stative verbs.

  In

  rare cases when

  th e

combination is

  possible,

  it

  indicates

  an

  entry into

  a

  state

  as in   Then

suddenly  I  knew  it

by

  focusing

  on the

  punctual point

  of

  entry into that

state. This

  in

  effect   translates

  a

  stative verb into

 an

  achievement verb.

The   interaction discussed   in   this subsection   is   summarized   in   Table

2.2, wh ich covers the most common grammatical aspect types, alth ou gh

it   is not

  meant

  to be

  exhaustive

 or

  without exception. Excluded from

  our

analyses here

  are the

  more marked cases

  of

  habitual reference  (e.g.,

  He is

walking to school these days)   an d

  futurate   (e.g.,

  W e are eating out

tonight),   which

 can be

  obtained with

 any of the

 dynamic verb  classes.

Table

  2.2. Intera ction

  of

  lexical

 aspect

 with grammatical

 aspect

State   Activi ty

  Accomplishment

  Achievement

Imperfective

Progressive

Perfective

state

??

stative prog

prog

prog

??  termination

inchoative

prog

prog

completion

prel im, stage

prelim, stage

99

completion

Semelfactive

iterative prog

iterative prog

termination

prog

 =

  progressive

prelim,

  stage

 =

 prel iminary

 stages of

  event

?? =

  questionable combination

Our   discussion   in   this subsection,   as   summarized   in   Table 2.2, shows

clearly   that there   are   combinatorial constraints   or   compatibility between

certain grammatical

  aspect and

  certain lexical  aspect. Comrie  (1976)

Unauthenticated

Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Page 13: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 13/18

Lexical  and   gramm atical aspect  23

discussed this kind of lexical-grammatical relationships with the  n a t u -

ralness

  of  com binat ion principle . According  to

  Comrie,

  perfect ive

aspect combines natu rally with pu nctu al verbs, for exam ple, because

perfective aspect presents  a  situation  as a  whole wi thout regard  to its

internal structure  and  punctual verbs

  encode

  a  s i tuat ion  as a  s ingle point

lacking internal structure. Conversely,

  imperfective

  aspect  is not  com-

patible with punctual verbs (except semelfactives, which

  can be

  const rued

iteratively),  because  imperfective aspect presents  a  situation  as  having  an

internal structure while

  a

  punctual verb

  encodes  it as a

  point lacking

internal structure.

  Activi ty

  verbs lend themselves naturally to the internal

perspective of impe rfective aspect because they encode the successive

phases of an event over t ime. Such interactions between lexical and

grammatical aspect presumably originate in certain natural relationships

between events in reali ty. For exam ple, w hen w e know that a si tuation

comes

  to its end

  with

 a

  clear result, this situation

  has

  probably al ready

become

  a  past event,  and we are  therefore l ikely  to  commen t  on its

completion  by  means  of  perfective aspect (Brown

  1973).

  Many s i tua-

t ions with

  an end

  result last

  for

  such

  a

  brief time that they

  are

  almost

certain to have ended before one can comment on them, such as si tua-

tions denoted

  by

  verbs like  drop fall

and

  crash. These  situations almost

exclude the internal perspective of imperfective aspect. In sum, there are

likely

  to be

  strong associations

  in the

  real world between resultat ivity

 or

telicity and the use of past and perfective verb forms, and between atelic

activities and the use of

  present

  and

  progressive verb forms.

  3 Variation  cross  l ngu ges

  in  gr mm ticiz tion

  o f

  aspect

As noted earl ier, there is variation across lan guag es in how aspec tual

notions

  are

  grammaticized. There

  are two

  types

  of

  variation

  to

  consider:

in  the pattern of grammaticization and in the degree of grammaticiza-

t ion.

First,

  different languages develop

  different

  types  of  grammat ica l  as-

pect,

  and

  some languages

  do not

  have grammatical aspect  (e.g.,  Hebrew,

Finnish).

  In

  terms

  of

  combinatorial properties

  of

  aspect

  and

  tense,

Ro m ance langua ges have a perfective-imperfective d ist inction only in the

past tense. Many Slavic languages , al though grammatical ly marking both

tense  and aspect,  do not  have perfective aspect  in the  present tense

  (e.g.,

Polish, Russian). In terms of combinatorial properties of grammatical

aspect and lexical aspect, the Japanese imperfective marker   -te i-  c a n n o t

Unauthenticated

Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Page 14: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 14/18

24

  Aspect

focus

  on the  prel iminary stage  of an  event  as in  English; instead  it

focuses   on the  duration  after  th e  terminal point  of a  situation,  and has the

perfect

  and

  resul tat ive me aning.

  The

  Chinese perfective  le  does

  no t

necessarily yield  th e  meaning o f  completion  wi th  telic verbs (see Chapter

5 for details). These variations are not rando m. Typological research

(Dahl

  1985;

  Bybee,

  Perkins, and Pagliu ca 1994) has sho w n that

  some

  of

these tendencies reflect common patterns of grammaticization. For

example, i t has been suggested that the reason ma ny languages do not

have perfective aspect

  fo r  present  is

  that

  th e

  most natural

  way of  talking

about situations existing

 at

  speech time

  is

  imperfective (state, progressive,

or

  habi tual)

  and not

  perfective, because perfective indicates something

has

  been completed or terminated. This pattern indicates the importance

of

  funct ional motivat ion

  in

  shaping systems

  o f

  grammatical aspect

  in

natural  language.

Second, grammatical aspect markers

  in

  different  languages  also  vary

in   their degree

  of

  grammaticization, which

  is in

  turn reflected

  in

  their

different  combinatorial patterns  with  lexical aspect.  As  noted earlier,  it

has

  been suggested  that  progressive markers grammaticize into general

imperfective markers (Bybee, Perkins, and Pagliuca 1994:  140-149 .  In

English,

  th e

  progressive aspect

  is

  highly grammaticized.

  In

  addition

  to its

unmarked progressive meaning (i.e., action in progress), it can also be

used for  stative progressive, prelimin ary stages, habitu al,  and  fu tura te .

Progressive  markers in other lan gu ages m ay be less gramm aticized, and

practically  limited

  to the

  typical action-in-progress meaning,

  for  exam-

ple, in Chinese, Malay, and Thai. Thus, in these languages,  progressive

mark ers cann ot be used for other occ asions (e.g., with states or achieve-

ments). Similarly, perfective aspect is also sensitive to the

  degree

  of

grammaticization in its

  interaction with lexical aspect.

  As

  discussed

above, perfective aspect

  is

 either incompatible

  wi th

  stative verbs,

  or has an

inchoat ive  meaning  with  some stative verbs.

  A s

  perfective aspect gram-

maticizes, however, it can be used as past tense marker, that is, to

  describe

a

  state that existed

  in the

  past (e.g.,

  The  book  was  there yesterday;  see

Bybee, Perkins,

  and

  Pagliuca 1994: 92).

  For

  example,

  th e

  English past

tense is aspec tually perfective (Smith 1997), signaling com pletion or

termination   with  dynamic verbs or  inception  with some  stative verbs (e.g.,

Then   I knew  // ).  But as a  highly grammaticized form  in the  func t ion  of

simple

  past tense,

  it can

  also

  be  freely

  applied

  to

  stative verbs

  to

  locate

states

  in the

  past time.

  The

  Japanese past tense marker  ta

  has a

  similar

Unauthenticated

Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Page 15: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 15/18

Lexical and grammatical aspect 25

property,  and is considered  to be in a  gramm aticization t ransi t ion fr om

perfect to perfective and furth er to simple past (Horie 1997).

Finally,

  a

  theoretically imp ortant variation concerns that

  of

  lexical

aspect. As we hav e discussed, some lang uage s (e.g., En glish) accept

imperfective/progressive with achievement verbs

  to

  focus

  on

  p re l i m i n a ry

stages of an event (e.g.,

  He is reaching the summ it

whereas others

  (e.g.,

Chinese and Japanese) do not. On the basis of this observation, Smith

(1997)  suggested that achieveme nts in Chinese do n ot inc lude prelim i-

nary

  stages

  of an

  event, whereas those

  in

  Engl ish

  do. An

  alternative

interpretation of this phen om enon is that the Chinese progressive m ar k-

ers   zai  or the

  Japanese marker

  -te i-

  simply cannot focus

  on

 p r e l im i n a r y

stages  of an  event .  In  other words,  it is not the  lexical aspect  of the  verb

that

  does

  not include preliminary stages, but the grammatical aspect

marker that cannot denote such stages (Shirai

  1998a).

  This interpretat ion

attributes

  the

  difference between Engl ish

  and

  Chinese

  to

  variat ion

  in

grammatical aspect,  not in  lexical aspect.  O f  course, this issue  -  wh e t h e r

th e  different ial behavior  of  progressives  in  different languages  is due to

grammatical aspect  or  lexical aspect  needs further empirical invest iga-

t ion.

2.4.  A time relational analysis  of

  aspect

Most l inguistic analyses, including ours, have adopted

  the

  def ini t ion

  of

grammatical aspect given in

  Comrie (1976).

  That is , gram ma tical aspect

involves di fferent ways

 of  v iewing th e

  temporal contour

  of a

  s i tua-

tion: perfective aspect presents

  an

  external view

  of the

  situation

  as a

single whole  in its  entirety without reference  to its  internal s t ructure,  and

imperfective

  aspect

  an

  internal view

  of the

  inner const i tuency

  of the

situation  without regard  for the  situation's init ial  or  final  b o u n d ar i e s .

Smith  (1983) treats this definit ion

  of

  aspect

  as

  v iewpoin t

  aspect ,  in

contrast

  to

  si tuat ion

  aspect

(see 2.3). Much

  th e

  same

  def ini t ion  of

aspect has been used in research by other l ingu ists  (e.g.,

  D ah l

  1985;

Bybee 1985; Bybee, Perkins,

  and

  Pagliuca 1994).

This approach to aspect has been challenged recently by Klein   (1994 ,

1995,

  in

  press). According

  to

  Klein,

  th e

  def in i t ion

  of

  aspect

  as

 d i f f e ren t

ways  of  v iewing a  situation is entirely metaphorical ,  and  thus vague: what

does  it

  mean exactly

  to

  view

  a

  s i tua t ion

in its

 entirety,

  or as a

  s ing le

whole,

  or

  with

  or

  witho ut reference

  to its

  in terna l cons t i tuency?

  For

example, the difference between

  John stood on h is toes

  and

  John was

Unauthenticated

Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Page 16: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 16/18

26  Aspect

standing

  on his

  toes  is

 characterized

  as

  that between

  th e

  perfective

  and

th e

  imperfective,

  yet it is difficult  to

  grasp

  th e

  metaphorical differen ces

of

  entirety, wholeness,

  or

  reference

  to

  internal structure

  in

  these

  in -

stances. In view of this problem , Kl ein proposed that aspect should be

examined  on a par  with tense, strictly in  terms  of  temporal relations, such

as  prior to

(>), contained  in (c),

  or

  posterior

  to

(<), between

  the

time intervals that

  are

  described

  by

  aspectual markers.

  In

  particular,

Klein  argued that three types

  of

  time intervals

  are

  involved

  in

  this time-

relational  theory  of  analysis and  aspect:

TU:

  t ime

 of

 utterance, i.e.,

  the

  time

  at

 which

 an

 utterance

  is

  produced;

T-SIT:

  time of situation, i.e., the time at w hich the situation described by

th e

  utterance obtains;

TT: topic time, i.e., the time span abou t wh ich something is said or for

which

  an

 assertion

  is

  made.

For exam ple, for the sentence

  John was

 writing

  a letter

TU is the time

at

  which

 th e

  speaker produces

  th e

  sentence,

 T-SIT  is the

  time

  at

  which

the

  situ ation of Joh n's writing a letter obtains, and TT is the time span

about  w h ich the  speaker makes  an  assertion.  In  this sentence,  all  that  is

claimed

  is

  that there

  was

  some time span within which John

  was

  per-

forming

  th e

  action

  of

  writing

 a

  letter; whether John completed

  th e

  letter-

writing  action

  is  left

  open. Thus,

  the

  tense function

  of the

  sentence

  is

represented by TT < TU (topic time preceding time of utterance), while

the aspect function  is represented  by TT c

  T-SIT

  (topic time contained

in  time

  of

  situation).

16

 In

 other w ords, tense

  is

  concerned with

 the

  tempo-

ra l

  relations between

  TT and TU,

  while aspect

  is

  concerned with

  the

temporal relations between TT and  T-SIT.  In this analysis, the same

kinds  of  temporal relations  (<, >, c)  operate  on  both tense  and  aspect.

This analysis thus eliminates

  th e

  vagu eness associated w ith traditio nal

metaphorical interpretations of aspect in terms of ways of viewing a

situation.  The

  follow ing diagram shows

  how the

  major tense

  and

  aspect

categories

  are

  represented

  in

  this fram ew ork (see Kle in 1994, 1995  fo r

  a

detailed analysis):

TENSE: Past  TT < TU

Present  TT D TU

Future  TT > TU

Unauthenticated

Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Page 17: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 17/18

Time relational analysis 21

ASPECT:

  Imperfective

  TT 

T-SIT

Perfective  TT

 

T-SIT

What does  this time-relational analysis have to say about the acquisi-

tion of

 aspect?

  Although currently no studies have used this framework

to

  directly examine

  th e

  acquisition

  of

  tense

  an d

  aspect,

  one can

  imagine

that

  a

  whole

  new set of

  interesting empirical questions would arise from

the standpoint of this perspective. For example, which temporal relations

are  entertained by children at which time? When and how do children

separate  TU  from

  T-SIT,

  TU

  from

  TT, and

  T-SIT

  from  TT?

  What

underlies children's development in their understanding of the various

relations between  th e  three temporal intervals,  fo r  tense  and

  aspect,

respectively? What language-universal  and  language-specific patterns  do

children display  in the  acquisition  of  these temporal relations?

  7

While there

  is a

  great deal

  of

  theoretical motivation

  an d

  empir ical

evidence  for  the time-relational analysis of tense and aspect, and answe rs

to the  above questions  are  important  in  addressing developmental  and

crosslinguistic issues, our analysis of aspect in this book rem ains t rad i-

tional, for several reasons. First, our original language acquisition studies

were conducted

  within

  the framework of a viewpoint  aspect type of

linguistic analysis.  Since  the kinds of research questions in the time-

relational framework

  are

  very

 different,

  it

  would

  be

  difficult

  to

  test these

question s with our existing data. Second, im portan t to our study is the

relationship between grammatical aspect and lexical aspect in adult as

well  as child language, as discussed throughout this chapter. It is not yet

entirely clear how the time-relational analysis of aspect captures the

intricate relationships between lexical and grammatical aspect (see Klein,

Li, and Hendriks,  in press,  for an  attempt  to

  study

  this issue  in  Chinese).

Finally, since almost all previous studies hav e exam ined the acq uisi tion

of

  aspect on the basis of the traditional definition of aspect, it would be

imp ortant as well as conv enient for us to evaluate and com pare our

studies against previous

  findings  on the

  same theoretical

  an d

  empir ica l

bases, using the same termino logies. Nevertheless, researchers in lan -

guage acquisition will  find  the time-relational analysis of aspect relevant

an d

  useful,

  as such new linguistic analyses could have considerable

significance

  fo r

  reconceptualizing issues

  of

 acq uisi tion.

Unauthenticated

Download Date | 4 2 16 6:26 PM

Page 18: Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

8/18/2019 Aspect: Problem of lexicon and morphology

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/aspect-problem-of-lexicon-and-morphology 18/18

28  spect

2.5.

  ummary

In th is

  chapter, we hav e provided an int roduct io n to theories of aspect by

discuss ing

  two kinds of aspect, grammatical and lexical,   with   respect

par t icular ly   to   l inguis t ic   an d

  typological considera t ions.

  W e

  examined

Comrie s

  c lassic definit ion of perfective and imperfective   aspect,

V e n d le r s

  four   c ategories of lexical aspect (activities, states, acc om plish -

ments,  an d   achievements) ,   an d   Sm ith s two-componen t theory   of   aspect

( the s i tuat ion type and the   v iewpoint   aspect). W e also presented K lein s

approach

  to

  aspect

  in

  terms

  of

  tempo ral relat ions, wh ich challeng es

classic

  defini t ions of aspect.

In

  this chapter, we have placed a strong emp hasis on the intera ction

between grammatical aspect and lexical aspect in the interpretat ion of the

sentence s aspectual meaning .

  W e

  looked

  at the

  combinatorial   propert ies

of  given aspectual markers with given types of verbs, and the differen t

implicat ions

  tha t a given grammatical aspect conveys when i t combines

with   verbs of different lexical aspect. As is the case with most studies in

language acquisi t ion, a c lear l inguist ic analysis of a part icular domain

serves as a

  good

  s tart ing

  poin t

  for understanding the acquisi t ion of tha t

domain. Thus, we have la id the basis here for the s tudy of the acquisi t ion

of   aspect.

Grammatical aspect, lexical aspect,

  and the

  interaction between

  the two

have important theoret ical implicat ions for language acquisi t ion. As we

will  see in later chapters, in both child language and adult

  second

  lan-

guage learning, there

  is a

  c lear interaction between gramm atical aspect

and   lexical aspect in the acqu isi t ion of tense-aspect m orphology, and this

interaction occurs across diverse languages.  Moreover,   a s we   have

stressed

  in

  this chapter, aspect stands

  at the

  interface between lexicon

  an d

mo rpholog y, and th us aspect not only emerges as a gramm atical task for

learners,

  but

  also serves

  as a

  ca ta lyst

  for

  the i r acquisi t ion

  of the

  lexicon.

For   example,   th e   morphological marking   of

  aspect

  is a   dist inct feature   of

verbs and not nouns or other grammatical categories of the sentence.

Chi ldren

  may use

  aspect markers

  as a

  means

  to

  he lp them di s t ingui sh

verbs

  from

  n o u n s ,

  in

  w hic h case aspect serves

  as a

  k ind

  of

  syntact ic

bootst rapping   device.

18

  To   ful ly   un dersta nd the relat ionships between

grammatical aspect and lexical aspect, not only descriptively for the

language, but   also   developmental ly for the language learner, we now turn

to the acquisi t ion of aspect.