American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    1/18

    http://abs.sagepub.com/American Behavioral Scientist

    http://abs.sagepub.com/content/56/12/1718The online version of this article can be found at:

    DOI: 10.1177/0002764212463357

    20122012 56: 1718 originally published online 19 OctoberAmerican Behavioral Scientist

    Karl Kaltenthaler and William J. MillerHyper-Partisanship

    The Polarized American : Views on Humanity and the Sources of

    Published by:

    http://www.sagepublications.com

    can be found at:American Behavioral ScientistAdditional services and information for

    http://abs.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

    http://abs.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:

    http://abs.sagepub.com/content/56/12/1718.refs.htmlCitations:

    What is This?

    - Oct 19, 2012OnlineFirst Version of Record

    - Nov 5, 2012Version of Record>>

    by guest on December 3, 2012abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/content/56/12/1718http://abs.sagepub.com/content/56/12/1718http://www.sagepublications.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://abs.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://abs.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://abs.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://abs.sagepub.com/content/56/12/1718.refs.htmlhttp://abs.sagepub.com/content/56/12/1718.refs.htmlhttp://abs.sagepub.com/content/56/12/1718.refs.htmlhttp://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://abs.sagepub.com/content/early/2012/10/17/0002764212463357.full.pdfhttp://abs.sagepub.com/content/early/2012/10/17/0002764212463357.full.pdfhttp://abs.sagepub.com/content/56/12/1718.full.pdfhttp://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://abs.sagepub.com/content/early/2012/10/17/0002764212463357.full.pdfhttp://abs.sagepub.com/content/56/12/1718.full.pdfhttp://abs.sagepub.com/content/56/12/1718.refs.htmlhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://abs.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://abs.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://www.sagepublications.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/content/56/12/1718http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    2/18

    American Behavioral Scientist

    56(12) 17181734

    2012 SAGE PublicationsReprints and permission: http://www.

    sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

    DOI: 10.1177/0002764212463357

    http://abs.sagepub.com

    ABS463357ABS561210.1177/0002764212463357American Behavioral ScientistKaltenthaler and Miller 2012SAGE Publications

    Reprintsand permission: http://www.agepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

    1The University of Akron and Case Western Reserve University, Akron, OH, USA2Flagler College, St. Augustine, FL, USA

    Corresponding Author:

    Karl Kaltenthaler, Department of Political Science, The University of Akron, Akron, OH 44325-1904, USA

    Email: [email protected]

    The Polarized

    American: Views on

    Humanity and the Sourcesof Hyper-Partisanship

    Karl Kaltenthaler1 and William J. Miller2

    Abstract

    In recent years, many possible explanations have been offered for why America hasbecome a more polarized country. In this article, we introduce a new variable to thediscussion. Rather than focusing on specific policy beliefs or political attitudes, weinstead suggest that social trust can be an important explanatory factor in predictingwhich Americans are most likely to be polarized. Through the analysis of ANES data,we find that individuals who are strong trusters of others are more likely to identifyas Strong Democrats while those less likely to do so are more likely to self-identify as

    Strong Republicans. The article concludes by looking at how considering social trustas a predictor of polarization impacts our understanding of American politics.

    Keywords

    social trust, polarization, public opinion, citizen attitudes

    One of the most debated issues in the study of American politics centers on the alleged

    polarization of the electorate. Some scholars argue that the American electorate isbecoming increasingly polarized (e.g., Koch, 1994; McDonald & Popkin, 2001;

    Olson, 2008). Others contend that American voters are not becoming more polarized

    (Hetherington, 2009). Still others have maintained that it is not the electorate that has

    become increasingly polarized but rather the political elite in the United States, par-

    ticularly in the U.S. Congress (Bond & Fleisher, 2000; Carmines & Stimson, 1989;

    Article

    by guest on December 3, 2012abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    3/18

    Kaltenthaler and Miller 1719

    Dixit & Weibull, 2007; Jones, 2001; Layman, 2001; Layman & Carsey, 2002a; Mann,

    2006; McCarty, Poole, & Rosenthal, 2009; Poole & Rosenthal, 1984; Rohde, 1991).

    This article does not seek to weigh in on the debate about whether the electorate or

    the Congress has become more polarized over time; instead, it explores those indi-viduals who think of themselves at the polar extremes of the mainstream American

    party spectrum. In other words, this article develops an argument about why some

    Americans view themselves as strong partisans, either of Republican or Democratic

    leanings. Thus, we hope to explain what individual-level factors make one think of

    oneself as a strong Republican or a strong Democrat.

    Our argument is that a crucial factor in determining whether one is a hyper-

    partisan is how an individual views humanity. We contend that the more negative a

    view of humanity a person has, the more likely she is to self-identify as a strong

    Republican. Conversely, the more positive someone is toward the nature of humanity,the more likely one is to self-identify as a strong Democrat. When an individual

    thinks of herself as a strong Democrat or Republican, she is thinking of herself as

    being either a strong liberal Democrat or a strong conservative Republican. A strong

    conservative Republican is likely to view the social world from a pessimistic basis,

    believing that people are, generally, not trustworthy. A strong liberal Democrat is

    likely to have a more positive view of humanity. Thus, it is the social psychology of

    the individual that shapes a large part of the ideological predisposition that culminates

    in a particular partisan identity.

    This study will first explore what others have argued about the sources of polariza-tion among the American electorate. The section to follow develops an argument about

    how views of humanity shape partisan worldviews. From this, a set of hypotheses are

    derived that are then tested in a regression analysis employing data from the 2008

    American National Election Study (ANES). The article concludes with what the find-

    ings tell us about the nature of polarized partisanship in the United States.

    Definitions of Political Polarization

    What do we mean when we refer to political polarization within the electorate?Esteban and Ray (1991, 1994) have argued that polarization is the process whereby

    individuals feel alienated from each other. They identify with a particular group, say

    a political party or an ideological movement, and they believe themselves to be at

    odds with the out-group. They call this an identification-alienation framework. In

    a polarized situation, by identifying with one group, an individual feels alienated from

    another group.

    DiMaggio, Evans, and Bryson (1996) have created a definition of polarization that

    is often used in the literature. They posit that polarization in the electorate is the

    movement toward the poles of the Left-Right distribution of voters. Thus, if the elec-torate is polarized, voters are clustered on the ideological Left and Right extremes,

    with the middle being relatively empty. What is important for purposes of this study

    is who the people at the polar ends of the distribution of the electorate are and why

    they are there.

    by guest on December 3, 2012abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    4/18

    1720 American Behavioral Scientist56(12)

    Common Explanations of Elite and Voter Political Polarization

    There has been a large amount of literature generated on the issue of political polariza-

    tion in the United States in recent decades. The literature tends to focus on politicalelite polarization, voter polarization, or the nexus between the two.

    Some have argued that the institutional changes of the past several decades in the

    United States should have reduced political elite polarization because measures to

    promote openness in government should logically have reduced distrust among elites

    and thus reduced a sense of us versus them (see Stasavage, 2007). But Stasavage

    (2007) has argued that the opposite has happened. He and others have pointed to the

    political parties convention delegates, candidates, and members of Congress as all

    having grown increasingly polarized on social welfare, racial, and cultural issues

    (Bond & Fleisher, 2000; Carmines & Stimson, 1989; Layman, 2001; Layman &Carsey, 2002a; Poole & Rosenthal, 1984; Rohde, 1991; Stasavage, 2007; Stone,

    Rapoport, & Abramowitz, 1990). Some have argued that when the leadership of the

    parties polarizes, it can pull the whole party system in a centripetal pattern. (Layman

    & Carsey, 2002b, p. 200). Some of the explanations offered for this elite polarization

    are gerrymandering (Wolfson, 2006) and ones location in the electoral calendar

    (Nicholson, 2003). This elite polarization may seem to make voters more polarized

    because the parties have offered them polarized candidates. The voterspositions on

    the issues may be moderate, even if theirchoices in the voting booth are not (McGhee

    & Krimm, 2009, p. 348).Layman and Carsey (2002b, p. 227) argue that elite polarization is indeed having

    an impact on voter polarization in a concrete way:

    Our evidence confirms that this is part of the partisan change process. However,

    we also clearly show that some individuals respond to increases in elite-level

    polarization not by changing their party ties but by changing their issue attitudes

    in response to those ties. When Democratic and Republican elites are clearly

    differentiated on an issue and citizens recognize that difference, then a signifi-

    cant number of party identifiers will bring their attitudes on the issue closer intoline with the stance of their partys leaders. When party elites are polarized on

    more than one issue agenda, the process of party identifiers converting their

    positions on issues toward the dominant stand of their parties means that issue

    dimensions, such as social welfare and culture, that were once orthogonal to

    each other will not remain so over time.

    Layman and Carsey (2002b, p. 228) also argue that party politics exerts a strong

    influence on political values and issue attitudes among the electorate. Those individu-

    als who are most attached to a particular political party are likely to be most influencedby the cues that they get from the party leadership on issues (Carmines & Stimson,

    1989; Claassen & Highton, 2009, p. 546; Kam, 2005; Nie, 1976; Zaller, 1992). But it

    is not just partisan attachment; it is also awareness of politics that will play a role in

    by guest on December 3, 2012abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    5/18

    Kaltenthaler and Miller 1721

    shaping how elite behavior and cues affect the electorate. Thus, the most politically

    sophisticated and aware people are most likely to be affected by relative positions of

    political elites (see Sniderman, Brody, & Tetlock, 1991; Zaller, 1992).

    The above arguments about the process of elite and voter polarization, by nomeans, represent a consensus view. Several scholars have argued that the data do not

    bear out the argument that the American electorate in becoming more polarized.

    Hetherington (2001, 2009) has shown that if one uses the definition of polarization

    as a growing bi-modality of American voters at the extremes, then there is little evi-

    dence of polarization. The distribution of American voters is not clearing out the

    middle as the polarization hypothesis would suggest.

    McGhee and Krimm (2009) are also prominent critics of the polarization thesis.

    They point to the growth in independent registration among voters. This would indi-

    cate less partisan attachment and not more. It might make the electorate appear morepolarized, however, because the voters who are left with partisan registrationswho

    then vote in the primaries (particularly closed primaries)are the most ardent parti-

    sans and most ideological voters, thus creating the appearance of a more polarized

    electorate.

    The Focus of This Study

    This study does not seek to answer the question of whether there is indeed a growing

    process of polarization in the American electorate or what has or has not driven thatprocess. This study is not interested in change in partisanship over time but rather

    explores the extremes of the partisan distribution in the United States in a cross-

    sectional snap-shot. This study focuses on explaining what individual-level factors

    put voters at the extreme ends of the Democrat-Republican distribution of voters.

    The studys dependent variable is self-identification as a strong Democrat or a

    strong Republican. Thus, we seek to identify the individual-level factors that would

    account for why an individual would think of himself or herself as a strong Democrat

    or strong Republican.

    It is common for models meant to predict partisanship to include ideology as anexplanatory variable (Abramowitz & Saunders, 1998, 2006; Alvarez & Garcia Bedolla,

    2003; Carmines & Berkman, 1994; Carmines & Stanley, 1992; Franklin & Jackson,

    1983). We have chosen to do the same despite acknowledging that they are so closely

    linked in a persons mind that there may be a high degree of endogeneity in a model with

    both variables included. This is because a person who views herself as a strong partisan

    is also very likely to view herself as a strong ideologue, leaning toward one or the other

    end of the ideological distribution. To control for this possibility, we will present results

    demonstrating the inclusion and exclusion of ideology as an explanatory variable.

    Views on Humanity and Ideological Self-Identification

    We contend that a persons view on the basic nature of humanity will play a large role

    in shaping her general ideological worldview. A persons basic view of human nature

    by guest on December 3, 2012abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    6/18

    1722 American Behavioral Scientist56(12)

    refers to whether a person believes that other people can be thought of as being inher-

    ently good or inherently bad. If an individual thinks of human nature as inherently

    bad, that person will be less willing to trust other people in general. If, on the other

    hand, a person views human nature as being inherently good, this person is morelikely to trust others in general than the person who has a negative view of basic

    human nature.

    This propensity to trust or not trust others has profound implications toward how

    one views politics (Binning, 2007; Brewer & Steenbergen, 2002; Brewer, Gross, Aday,

    & Wilnat, 2004; Citrin & Sides, 2008; Herreros & Criado, 2009; Inglehart, 1997;

    Rothstein & Uslaner, 2005; Torgler, 2007; Uslaner, 2000-2001). While much research

    has focused on how social trust affects a whole range of attitudes toward political

    phenomena, there has been very little research on how social trust affects basic parti-

    san self-identification. We contend that this is an important oversight as social trustforms a crucial part of the psychological basis for an individuals general ideological

    worldview.

    Let us first address what we mean by ideology. Ideology is a coherent set of ideas

    meant to generally explain political phenomena that a person confronts as well as offer

    a general set of remedies to political issues that arise. Scholars have looked at political

    ideology as being a set of ideas that sit along two basic dimensions. The classical ideo-

    logical dimension that has been referred to for a very long time is the traditional Left-

    Right dimension (Lipset & Rokkan, 1967). This dimension was characterized by how

    an individual viewed the proper role of the state in managing the economy and distrib-uting wealth in a society. People who were on the Left side of the ideological spectrum

    were likely more in favor of the state playing an activist role in the economy and

    redistributing wealth from the more fortunate (wealthy) to the less fortunate (the poor).

    This view was premised on the argument that the wealthy achieved their wealth in a

    system that was largely stacked in their favor. Thus, it was only fair that the state play

    a redistributive role and manage the market in order to aid those who were losers

    from the outcomes of the economic and political system.

    The traditional Right, on the other hand, largely opposed a large redistributive and

    economic management role for the state, as the wealthy were assumed to have achievedtheir economic gains through hard work and perseverance and therefore a large, redis-

    tributive, and economic activist state was not fair to the wealthy. Thus, the Left and the

    Right both couched their general arguments about the state in terms of fairness.

    Political science viewed the political bases of support for these Left and Right posi-

    tions as a function of a persons class background (Lipset & Rokkan, 1967). Thus, a

    person who was born into the working class was likely to be more supportive of the

    Left, because of her identification with those who looked to the state for redress of

    social and economic inequality. Conversely, those who were born into the middle and

    upper classes would tend to support the Right because they would want the state toplay a minimal role in redistributing wealth to those who had lower incomes as funds

    for that redistribution would come from the wealthy through taxation and other means.

    Therefore, a persons placement on the Left-Right ideological space was viewed as a

    function of economic self-interest.

    by guest on December 3, 2012abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    7/18

    Kaltenthaler and Miller 1723

    But there has been another ideological dimension that has been identified by politicalscientists more recently that is meaningful: the Right authoritarian-Left libertarian dimen-

    sion (Kitschelt, 1995). This ideological dimension does not center on economic issues as

    the traditional Left-Right dimension does but rather is concerned with the amount of

    freedom or constraint the state should create in society for its citizens. A person who is

    viewed as being more on the Right authoritarian end of the dimension would believe that

    the state should play a larger law and order role in society as well as play some role in

    preserving traditional moral values. An individual who is seen as more towards the Left

    libertarian end of the dimension would believe that the state should try to maximize indi-

    vidual freedom of expression and democratic participation and place less emphasis onlaw and order and the state playing a role in defending traditional moral values. The Right

    authoritarian has a view of society as needing a Leviathan, and the Left libertarian view

    is that man is best served by allowing people the maximum input into governance without

    being threatened by a state that infringes on their personal rights.

    Inglehart (1977, 1997) has argued that the Right authoritarianLeft libertarian

    dimension of politics1

    has grown more important in many advanced industrial

    democracies as basic needs have been met by economic growth and the creation of

    welfare states.2 Thus, political parties in such states have tended to add authoritarian-

    libertarian issues to their platforms as these issues have resonated more with theirpublics. Right parties have tended to take authoritarian stances, arguing for the need

    for law and order measures in society, whereas Left parties have tended to take more

    libertarian stances in this New Politics.3

    When the two dimensions of ideology are combined, we are presented with a

    schema that looks like the following:

    When we apply this schema to American politics, we find that because of the nature

    of the electoral system and its tendency to punish small parties, the two main parties,

    the Republicans and the Democrats, have absorbed New Politics issues into their

    platforms alongside the traditional stances on economic distributional issues. TheRepublican Party has maintained its traditional stance as being the party less inclined

    to support economic redistribution in the American party system. The Democratic

    Party has maintained its traditional platform of being the major American political

    party more inclined to support economic redistribution and an economically activist

    state to aid the economically less advantaged.

    by guest on December 3, 2012abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    8/18

    1724 American Behavioral Scientist56(12)

    In terms of the New Politics issues, the Republican Party has also taken on the

    mantle of being the party home for Right authoritarian votersthat is, those who are

    more in favor of law and order politics and the state playing a role in preserving tradi-

    tional moral values. The Democratic Party, on the other hand, has become the majorparty home for Left libertarian voters who want the polity to increase inclusion in the

    political process, protect minority rights, and keep the state from trying to protect con-

    servative, traditional morality. Thus, most Republican and Democratic voters can be

    thought of as situated along the diagonal axis that bisects traditional Left-Right politics

    as well as the New Politics. Thus, voters for the two major parties are a mix of mate-

    rialists and those who vote along New Politicsthat is, authoritarian-libertarian lines.

    We posit that the degree to which one trusts others will play a very important role

    in determining the leanings of the highly partisan American. We hypothesize that the

    more social trust an individual says she has, the more likely she is to self-identify as astrong Democrat. Conversely, the less social trust an individual says he has, the more

    likely that person is to self-identify as a strong Republican.

    Social trust will condition attitudes toward both traditional Left-Right politics, as well

    as New Politics. A person with a higher relative degree of social trust will have a more

    positive attitude toward redistributive policies because that person will be more positive

    toward other people, in general, and believe that the recipients of aid from the government

    are deserving of such help. Thus, the social truster will be more likely to identify with the

    Left than the Right. Such a person would more likely be a Democrat than a Republican.

    Conversely, a person who is less trusting of other people, relative to others, will beless willing to have the state redistribute wealth to others because that person would

    not be positively inclined toward others, in general, and would thus not favor such

    policies. This person would identify more with the Right and thus would more likely

    think of herself as a Republican.

    When it comes to New Politics, a persons relative propensity to trust others will

    also influence her identification with the Right and the Left. A social truster will likely

    identify with the Left on New Politics issues because such a person would not look to

    the state to constrain others through law and order policies because such a person would

    feel less threatened by others. This identification with the Left on New Politics issueswould more likely make the person think of herself as a Democrat than as a Republican.

    Those who are relatively less trusting of others would be more likely to identify

    with the Right on New Politics issues. The person who is relatively less trusting of

    others would be more likely to want the state to play a more constrained role in soci-

    ety in terms of law and order because of the more generalized lack of trust in others.

    Because of this identification with the Right on New Politics issues, the less trust-

    ing individual is more likely to be a Republican in the American context.

    Data, Measures, and Methods

    We employ ordered logistic regression analyses given the dependent variable is bino-

    mial and ordinal in nature. As mentioned earlier, we employ data from the 2008

    American National Election Survey. The number of respondents in this sample was

    2,203. Our final models include 518 respondents who responded to all questions

    by guest on December 3, 2012abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    9/18

    Kaltenthaler and Miller 1725

    employed as variables in our study. Most respondents are excluded for not responding

    to questions regarding their ideology or income. Based on diagnostics, there are no

    clear patterns in the excluded cases.

    The Dependent Variable

    The dependent variable in this model is strong partisan beliefs. The ANES asks respon-

    dents to identify the political party they most closely identify with and are able to break

    down respondents into a 7-point scale ranging from strong Democrat to strong

    Republican. In order to examine our question of what individual-level factors deter-

    mine whether someone is a strong Democrat or strong Republican, we create a binary

    variable for each. Respondents who identify as strong Democrats are coded 1, with all

    other respondents as 0. Then, all respondents who identify as strong Republicans (7 onthe 7-point scale) are coded 1, with all other respondents as 0. By doing so, we end up

    with two dependent variables.

    A Picture of Polarization

    The breakdown of the dependent variables find that 25.0% of respondents label them-

    selves as strong Democrats, while 9.9% declare to be strong Republicans. While it is

    unclear we there are so many fewer strong Republicans compared to strong

    Democrats, it could be that many of the strong conservatives have come to label them-selves Independents rather than strong Republicans.

    We can go further in a descriptive analysis by looking at demographic differences

    between strong Democrats, strong Republicans, nonpolarized individuals, and all respon-

    dents. By doing so, we can get a picture of the typical polarized Democrats and Republicans.

    When we look at the demographics (as seen in Table 1), we find that there are many

    splits that follow widely known partisan trends. Strong Democrats, for example,

    appear to have relatively lower incomes, while strong Republicans report relatively

    higher incomes. Likewise, strong Republicans are more likely to be married than the

    average respondent to the ANES, while the typical strong Democrat is less likely to bemarried. The characteristic racial divide emergeswith strong Republicans more

    likely to be White than the average respondent and strong Democrats more likely to be

    members of a minority group. Lastly, we see a gender gap in terms of polarization with

    women more likely to be strong Democrats and men strong Republicans than their

    corresponding average respondents. None of these findings are particularly unex-

    pected and confirm previous findings and beliefs.

    Where we find more unique results are when looking at region and age. First, we

    can see that the Northeast and Southern Census regions have clear polarization pat-

    terns. Individuals living in the Northeast are considerably less likely to be polarized,while those in the South are more likely to be politically polarized than nonpolarized.

    It is, by far, the most polarized region in the country. The South is a region where

    strong Republicans outnumber strong Democrats, but not by a large margin. The North

    Central region has less polarization and there is a slightly greater likelihood of strong

    by guest on December 3, 2012abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    10/18

    1726 American Behavioral Scientist56(12)

    Democrats than Republicans and the West shows the same for strong Republicans

    outnumbering strong Democrats. This suggests that there are some marked regional

    considerations related to polarization in the United States. A pattern emerges with age

    as well, with older respondents being more likely to be polarized (regardless of party)than their younger counterparts. As individuals age, they appear to move toward the

    poles when considering their levels of partisanship.

    The Independent Variables

    Below are the questions that we use to test the predictive power of our argument about

    the role of social trust contributing to polarization in the United States. We also test a

    set of alternative hypotheses to determine relative predictive power of the most com-

    mon arguments about what drives said polarization.We include a very common measure of relative social trust in the model to test our

    primary hypothesis. The question asks, Can people be trusted? The respondent has the

    choice of answering that eitheryou cant be too carefulormost people can be trusted.

    We argue that those individuals who believe people can be trusted will be more likely

    to be strong Democrats and that those who do not believe people can be trusted will be

    more likely to be strong Republican identifiers.

    Given the links often discussed between religious beliefs and religiosity and parti-

    sanship, we include two religious measures (Abramowitz & Saunders, 2006; Carmines

    & Stanley, 1992; Franklin & Jackson, 1983). First, the variable examining religiosityasks, Would you say you religion provides someguidance in your day-to-day living,

    quite a bit of guidance, or a great deal of guidance in your day-to-day life? Respondents

    are coded for not believing religion is important, religion provides some guidance, reli-

    gion provides quite a bit of guidance, and religion provides a great deal of guidance.

    The religious beliefs measure asks, Which of the these statements comes closest to

    describing your feelings about the Bible? Respondents are asked to choose from the

    following: 1 = The Bible is the actual word of God and is to be taken literally, word for

    word; 2 = The Bible is the word of God but not everything in it should be taken literally,

    word for word; and 3 = The Bible is a book written by men and is not the word of God.We use a 7-point scale of ideology to measure where respondents place them-

    selves in terms of political beliefs (Abramowitz & Saunders, 1998, 2006; Alvarez

    & Bedolla, 2003; Carmines & Berkman, 1994; Carmines & Stanley, 1992; Franklin

    & Jackson, 1983). The scale runs from 1, indicating strong liberal, to 7, indicating

    strong conservative. Strong liberals are predicted to be strong Democrats, and

    strong conservatives are most likely to be strong Republicans. As touched on above,

    there are concerns regarding endogeneity when utilizing ideology to predict parti-

    sanship. As a result, we will present our models showing results both with and

    without ideology included. We also include a measure of race. Our variable isdummy coded, with all non-White respondents coded as 1 and White respondents

    coded 0. Previous literature has demonstrated that race needs to be considered

    when examining partisanship and polarization (Avery, 2007; Hutchings & Valentino,

    2004; McDaniel & Ellison, 2008). As with typical party identification, White

    by guest on December 3, 2012abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    11/18

    Kaltenthaler and Miller 1727

    respondents are predicted to more likely be strong Republicans, and non-White

    respondents are predicted to be more likely strong Democrats.

    We also include a number of control variables for gender, age, education, and

    income as these are very common controls in studies of public attitudes toward polar-

    ization in the United States. We also include a measure of job security as a way to

    control for the economic insecurity of the respondent. We include this measure because

    it is possible that the respondents economic insecurity can affect the responses givenin the survey.

    Results of the Analyses

    The ordered logistic regression analysis results for our model are presented in Table 2.

    Models 1 and 2 examine predictors of strong Democrats. Model 1 includes ide-

    ology as an explanatory variable, while Model 2 removes ideology given possible

    concerns of multicollinearity from having it explain party identification (a variable

    it is closely linked with). We find relative consistency between both models interms of predictions. However, our variable of most interest theoreticallysocial

    trustis not significant when ideology is included. When ideology is excluded,

    however, we find that the more individuals trust others, the more likely they are to

    Table 1. Demographic Breakdown

    AllRespondents

    StrongDemocrat

    StrongRepublican Nonpolarized

    Lowest 25% income 22.8 27.1 10.2 23.4

    25%-50% income 26 30.1 19.7 25.6

    50%-75% income 29.5 24.7 32.5 30.7

    Highest 25% income 21.7 18.1 37.6 20.3

    Married 42.2 36.7 61.4 41.5

    Not married 57.8 63.3 38.6 58.5

    White 51.4 27.4 82.1 55.9

    Not White 48.6 72.6 17.9 44.1

    Northeast 10.9 10.2 5.7 12North Central 17.1 17.4 13.5 17.5

    South 47.3 50.3 55.7 44.9

    West 24.7 22.1 25.2 25.6

    Male 43 39 46.7 43.9

    Female 57 61 53.3 56.1

    Lowest 25% age 25.6 18.4 13.3 30.3

    25%-50% age 24.2 22.4 24 24.9

    50%-75% age 25.3 29.5 28 23.2

    Highest 25% age 24.9 29.7 34.7 21.6

    by guest on December 3, 2012abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    12/18

    1728 American Behavioral Scientist56(12)

    identify as strong Democrats (significant at the .05 level). Ideology, as would beexpected, is significant at the .01 level when included in the model, with strong

    liberals showing as being more disposed to being strong Democrats. Being non-

    White proves to be a strong predictor (.01 level) in both models in the assumed

    direction. When ideology is excluded, age becomes the only other significant pre-

    dictor (at the .05 level). Being older is linked with individuals being more likely to

    be strong Democrats. No other variables produce significant relationships, includ-

    ing religion or other demographics.

    For the strong Republican modelsthree and fourwe find similar results.

    When we account for ideology, it is significant at the .01 level and demonstrates thatstrong conservatives are more likely to be strong Republicans. In the same model,

    social trust is significant at the .05 level. Unlike in the Democrat model, however,

    the relationship is negative (as hypothesized) and suggests that social distrusters are

    more likely to be strong Republicans. When ideology is removed from the model,

    we again see congruity with the Democratic model. Social trust remains significant

    at the .05 level in the negative direction. Non-White racial statusas it does in the

    Democratic model without ideologyremains as a significant predictor (at the .01

    level) and demonstrates that White respondents are more likely to be strong

    Republicans. Unlike in the Democratic models, however, in our Republican modelthat excludes ideology, we find both religious variables to be significant predictors.

    At the .05 level, individuals who believe the Bible to be the literal word of God and

    who claim to use religion to guide their life are shown to be more likely to be strong

    Table 2. Logistic Regression Model Results: Dependent VariableStrong Partisanship

    Model 1: Strong

    Democrat

    Model 2: Strong

    Democrat

    Model 3: Strong

    Republican

    Model 3: Strong

    Republican

    Independent

    variables Coef. SEFirst

    Diff. Coef. SEFirst

    Diff. Coef. SEFirst

    Diff. Coef. SEFirst

    Diff.

    Social trust .289 .244 .505** .197 .085 -.721** .308 .053 -.669** .269 .051

    Religion guide life -.206 .155 -.090 .123 .358 .196 .442** .170 .062

    Biblical literalism -.047 .206 -.147 .164 .281 .272 .556** .236 .070

    Ideology -.389*** .085 .375 .852*** .131 .454

    Non-White 1.327*** .267 .202 1.534*** .220 .251 -1.109*** .335 .083 -1.596*** .296 .134

    Age .019* .009 .184 .020** .008 .225 -.007 .012 .001 .010

    Education .055 .051 .083 .045 -.064 .072 -.028 .065

    Household income -.037 .124 -.111 .100 .167 .166 .261 .142

    Gender .239 .184 .141 .148 -.137 .219 -.200 .170Worry lose job .114 .148 .118 .114 -.167 .212 -.272 .185

    Pseudo R2 .1357 .1042 .2626 .1509

    Log likelihood -235.52 -357.93 -169.05 -227.89

    Log likelihood 2 73.94 83.26 120.38 81.01

    N 516 713 516 713

    Note: Figures are unstandardized coefficients shown alongside standard errors.

    *p < .1. **p < .05. ***p < .01.

    by guest on December 3, 2012abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    13/18

    Kaltenthaler and Miller 1729

    Republicans. This finding aligns with the belief that conservative Christians are

    more likely to be strong Republicans.

    When we consider the impact played by ideology in both models, we find that the

    inclusion of ideology does far more in explaining strong Republican identificationcompared to strong Democrats. If we look at the pseudo-R

    2values for the models, we

    find that the inclusion of ideology leads to the strong Democrat model explaining

    approximately 3.5% more of the variance in our dependent variable, whereas its inclu-

    sion leads to an increase in model explanatory power of just over 10% when looking

    at the strong Republican model.

    Given that we use logit, the coefficients reported in Table 2 do not represent the

    marginal effects of the independent variables on the dependent variables. As a result,

    we report the first differences of our significant independent variables in Table 2 as

    well. The first difference reported measures the probability of the dependent variable,signifying identification as a strong partisan, when the independent variable of interest

    is moving from its minimum to its maximum value, while all remaining independent

    variables are held at their appropriate measures of central tendency. This allows us to

    consider the substantive significance of each independent variable to explaining varia-

    tion in the dependent variable.

    When looking at the strong Democrat models, we find that ideology has the largest

    first difference value.375. This value tells us that taking an individual from strong

    liberal to strong conservative makes them 37.5% more likely to be a strong Democrat

    when all other variables are held constant at their central tendency. Non-White racialstatus is the second most substantive predictor (.202) followed by age (.184). With

    ideology removed, however, we see social trust having a significant impact in the

    model with a value of .085, yet it still has less substantive impact than non-White

    racial status (.251) and age (.225).

    Both Republican models present similar results. In Model 3, ideology has a first

    difference of .454, while social trust is only at .053. Non-White racial status splits the

    two with a first difference value of .083. When ideology is removed in Model 4, being

    non-White has the largest first difference value (.134) while biblical literalism, reli-

    gion guide life, and social trust and all increase the likelihood of someone being astrong Republican by between 5.1% and 7.0% when moved from their minimum to

    maximum values.

    In short, our results demonstrate that while social trust is an important predictor that

    must be considered when discussing the increased polarization of the American elec-

    torate, previous research on partisan identity has ignored this important social psycho-

    logical factor.

    Conclusion

    This article has examined what factors lead Americans to identify as polarized parti-

    sans. Moreover, we have attempted to test congruency in explanatory factors between

    strong Democrats and strong Republicans. Most importantly, we introduced the

    by guest on December 3, 2012abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    14/18

    1730 American Behavioral Scientist56(12)

    concept of social trust into Americas understanding of political polarization. We

    argued that Americans general predispositions toward others will impact the likeli-

    hood of their declaring to be strong partisans. For Democrats, we posited that being

    social trusters would increase the chances of an individual identifying as a strongpartisan, while for Republicans we hypothesized that being a distruster would increase

    the odds of identifying as a strong partisan.

    Much of the current polarization research focuses on the role of varying ideological

    and demographic characteristics, including ideology, age, income, and religious

    beliefs and behavior. In this article, however, we demonstrate how social trust condi-

    tions the psychological basis for an individuals general ideological worldview

    including their propensity to be hyper-partisans. By including social trust, the

    discussion about polarization begins to see a shift in interpretation. First, it moves the

    decision away from any easily traced demographic basis. While variables such as race,income, or education can be gleaned from national census data, there is no similar

    measure of individual-level social trust available. Without being able to fully account

    for what has been shown to be a meaningful predictor, it makes the process of predict-

    ing the future of polarization more difficult.

    It is especially important to note this when we remember that social trust still proves

    to be significant even when ideology is included within the model. It comes as little

    surprise that strong liberals would be strong Democrats or strong conservatives would

    be strong Republicans given the long observed relationship between ideology and

    partisanship in the United States. This only goes further to explain why it is importantto consider the potential role played by social trust in shaping political attitudes.

    If hyper-partisanship is impacted by individual social trust, it makes it significantly

    more difficult to be optimistic regarding hope for more bipartisan political beliefs,

    attitudes, and policies in the United States. When polarization is based on politics or

    demographic characteristics, we can change policies or alter the discourse to bring

    people back to the middle. If it is caused by social psychologythat is, general predis-

    positions towards othershowever, it will require a more fundamental shift in citizen

    attitudes, which would be far more difficult to achieve.

    One of the key questions that arise from this research is, What does the relation-ship between social trust and polarized partisanship indicate for the future of partisan

    relations in the United States? There is reason to expect that polarized partisanship is

    likely to increase in the near to medium term. The reason for this is that the economic

    crisis that befell the United States in 2008 has left in its wake a weakened economy

    and bleak prospects for many younger Americans. As we know that an individuals

    pessimism about his or her future tends to make a person less trusting of others, this

    means that the proportion of people who have a dim view of humanity will grow in

    this country. This will increase the ranks of the hyper-partisan Right, as will also

    likely increase the ranks of the partisan Left who look to the government to protectand aid them from economic circumstances. Thus, this research does not offer much

    room for optimism that the highly partisan nature of American politics will diminish

    in the near future.

    by guest on December 3, 2012abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    15/18

    Kaltenthaler and Miller 1731

    Declaration of Conflicting Interests

    The authors declared no conflicts of interest with respect to the authorship and/or publication

    of this article.

    Funding

    The authors received no financial support for the research and/or authorship of this article.

    Notes

    1. These issues are often referred to as New Politics issues.

    2. Ingelhart referred to postmaterialist values as values that stressed helping others achieve a

    voice in politics and society and placed emphasis on things like environmental problems.

    He viewed this as a new dimension of the Left. They developed in societies where basicneeds were met and the more economically secure could focus on matters beyond their

    own pocketbooks. In multiparty systems, postmaterialists would often join Green parties.

    In party systems given to two-party dominance, the postmaterialists would likely join the

    left-leaning party in the system. This would be the case in the United States.

    3. This libertarianism should not be confused with the specific American brand of liber-

    tarianism, which is more focused on economic freedom from state action, although it does

    include an element of social freedom as well. American libertarianism is a more conser-

    vative form of libertarianism, which focuses more on reducing the states role in society

    overall, whereas the brand of libertarian politics referred to by scholars such as Inglehart(1977, 1997) envisions a positive and even activist role for the state to play in society by

    bringing people into the political process and intervening in societal processes to ensure

    that individuals are free from oppression from others.

    References

    Abramowitz, A., & Saunders, K. (1998). Ideological realignment and the U.S. electorate. The

    Journal of Politics, 60, 634-652.

    Abramowitz, A., & Saunders, K. (2006). Partisanship in the American electorate: Social identity

    vs. ideology.Political Research Quarterly, 59, 175-187.Alvarez, M., & Bedolla, L. G. (2003). The foundations of Latino voter partisanship: Evidence

    from the 2000 election.Journal of Politics, 65, 31-49.

    Avery, J. M. (2007). Race, partisanship, and political trust followingBush versus Gore (2000).

    Political Behavior, 29, 327-342.

    Binning, K. (2007). Its us against the world: How distrust in Americans versus people-in-

    general shapes competitive foreign policy preferences.Political Psychology, 28, 777-799.

    Bond, J. R., & Fleisher, R. (2000).Polarized politics: Congress and the President in a partisan

    era. Washington, DC: CQ Press.

    Brewer, P., Gross, K., Aday, S., & Willnat, L. (2004). International trust and public opinionabout world affairs.American Journal of Political Science, 46, 93-109.

    Brewer, P., & Steenbergen, M. (2002). All against all: How beliefs about human nature shape

    foreign policy options.Political Psychology, 23, 39-58.

    by guest on December 3, 2012abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    16/18

    1732 American Behavioral Scientist56(12)

    Citrin, J., & Sides, J. (2008). Immigration and the imagined community in Europe and the

    United States.Political Studies, 56, 33-56.

    Carmines, E., & Berkman, M. (1994). Ethos, ideology, and partisanship: Exploring the paradox

    of conservative Democrats.Political Behavior, 16, 203-218.Carmines, E., & Stanley, H. (1992). The transformation of the New Deal party system: Social

    groups, political ideology, and changing partisanship among Northern whites, 1972-1988.

    Political Behavior, 14, 213-237.

    Carmines, E., & Stimson, J. (1989).Issue evolution: Race and the transformation of American

    politics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Claassen, R. L., & Highton, B. (2009). Policy polarization among party elites and the signifi-

    cance of political awareness in the mass public.Political Research Quarterly, 62, 538-551.

    DiMaggio, P., Evans, J., & Bryson, B. (1996). Have Americans social attitudes become more

    polarized?American Journal of Sociology, 102(3), 690-755.Dixit, A. K., & Weibull, J. W. (2007). Political polarization.Proceedings of the National Acad-

    emy of Sciences, 104, 7351.

    Esteban, J., & Ray, D. (1991). On the measurement of polarization (Institute for Economic

    Development Discussion Paper 18). Boston: Institute for Economic Development.

    Esteban, J., & Ray, D. (1994). On the measurement of polarization.Econometrica, 62, 819-851.

    Franklin, C., & Jackson, J. (1983). The dynamics of party identification. The American Political

    Science Review, 77, 957-973.

    Herreros, F., & Criado, H. (2009). Social trust, social capital, and perceptions of immigration.

    Political Studies, 57, 337-355.Hetherington, M. J. (2001). Resurgent mass partisanship: The role of elite polarization.Ameri-

    can Political Science Review, 95, 619-632.

    Hetherington, M. J. (2009). Review article: Putting polarization in perspective.British Journal

    of Political Science, 39, 413-448.

    Hutchings, V. L., & Valentino, N. A. (2004). The centrality of race in American politics.Annual

    Review of Political Science, 7, 383-408.

    Inglehart, R. (1977). The silent revolution: Changing values and political styles. Princeton, NJ:

    Princeton University Press.

    Inglehart, R. (1997).Modernization and postmodernization: Cultural, economic, and politicalchange in 43 Societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Jones, D. R. (2001). Party polarization and legislative gridlock. Political Research Quarterly,

    54, 125-141.

    Kam, C. D. (2005). Who toes the party line? Cues, values, and individual differences.Political

    Behavior, 27, 163-182.

    Kitschelt, H. (1995). The Radical right in Western Europe. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan

    Press.

    Koch, J. W. (1994). Group identification in political context.Political Psychology, 15, 687-698.

    Layman, G. (2001). The great divide: Religious and cultural conflict in American party politics.New York: Columbia University Press.

    Layman, G. C., & Carsey, T. M. (2002a). Party polarization and conflict extension in the

    American electorate.American Journal of Political Science, 46, 786-802.

    by guest on December 3, 2012abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    17/18

    Kaltenthaler and Miller 1733

    Layman, G. C., & Carsey, T. M. (2002b). Party polarization and party structuring of policy atti-

    tudes: A comparison of three NES panel studies.Political Behavior, 24, 199-236.

    Lipset, S. M., & Rokkan, S. (1967). Party systems and voter alignments: Cross-national per-

    spectives (International Yearbook of Political Behavior Research, Volume 7). New York:The Free Press.

    Mann, T. E. (2006). Polarizing the House of Representatives: How much does gerrymandering

    matter?Red and Blue Nation, 263-283.

    McCarty, N., Poole, K. T., & Rosenthal, H. (2009). Does gerrymandering cause polarization?

    American Journal of Political Science, 53, 666-680.

    McDaniel, E. L., & Ellison, C. G. (2008). Gods party? Race, religion, and partisanship over

    time.Political Research Quarterly, 61, 180-191.

    McDonald, M. P., & Popkin, S. L. (2001). The myth of the vanishing voter.American Political

    Science Review, 95, 963-974.McGhee, E., & Krimm, D. (2009). Party registration and the geography of party polarization.

    Polity, 41, 345-367.

    Nicholson, S. P. (2003). The political environment and ballot proposition awareness.American

    Journal of Political Science, 47, 403-410.

    Nie, N. (1976). The changing American voter. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Olson, J. (2008). Whiteness and the polarization of American politics.Political Research Quar-

    terly, 61, 704-718.

    Poole, K. T., & Rosenthal, H. (1984). The polarization of American politics. The Journal of

    Politics, 46, 1061-1079.Rohde, D. (1991).Parties and leaders in the postreform House. Chicago: Chicago University

    Press.

    Rothstein, B., & Uslaner, E. (2005). All for all: Equality, corruption, and social trust. World

    Politics, 58, 41-72.

    Sniderman, P. M., Brody, R. A., & Tetlock, P. E. (1991).Reasoning and choice: Explorations in

    political psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Stasavage, D. (2007). Polarization and publicity: Rethinking the benefits of deliberative democ-

    racy.Journal of Politics, 69, 59-72.

    Stone, W., Rapoport, R., & Abramowitz, A. (1990). Sex and the caucus participant: The gendergap in 1984 and 1988.American Journal of Political Science, 34, 725-740.

    Torgler, B. (2007). Trust in international organizations: An empirical investigation focusing on

    the United Nations (Working Paper 2006-20). Basel, Switzerland: Center for Research in

    Economics, Management, and the Arts.

    Uslaner, E. (2000-2001). Producing and consuming trust. Political Science Quarterly, 115,

    569-590.

    Wolfson, A. (2006). Red and blue nation? Causes, consequences, and correction of Americas

    polarized politics. Issues in Governance Studies, Number 1. Washington, DC: The Brook-

    ings Institution.Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univer-

    sity Press.

    by guest on December 3, 2012abs.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/http://abs.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 American Behavioral Scientist 2012 Kaltenthaler 1718 34

    18/18

    1734 American Behavioral Scientist56(12)

    Bios

    Karl Kaltenthaler is the director of research at the Ray C. Bliss Institute of Applied Politics

    and a professor in the Department of Political Science at the University of Akron. He is also onthe faculty in the Department of Political Science at Case Western Reserve University. He is the

    author of several articles and books on public opinion, political economy, and political extrem-

    ism. His current research centers on the role that social psychology plays in shaping attitudes

    toward politics.

    William J. Miller is an assistant professor of public administration at Flagler College. He is

    the author of several articles and books on the Tea Party movement, campaign strategy, public

    opinion, political extremism, and the pedagogy of political science and public administration.

    His current research examines how social psychology (particularly social trust) impacts citizenattitudes toward various policies and functions of government.