20
184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological Society The British Psychological Society www.wileyonlinelibrary.com ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember’: Exploring the contributions of episodic and semantic self-knowledge to strength of identity Catherine Haslam 1, Jolanda Jetten 12 , S. Alexander Haslam 1 , Cara Pugliese 1 and James Tonks 1 1 School of Psychology, University of Exeter, UK 2 School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia The present research explores the relationship between the two components of auto- biographical memory – episodic and semantic self-knowledge – and identity strength in older adults living in the community and residential care. Participants (N = 32) completed the autobiographical memory interview and measures of personal identity strength and multiple group memberships. Contrary to previous research, autobiographical memory for all time periods (childhood, early adulthood, and recent life) in the semantic domain was associated with greater strength in personal identity. Further, we obtained support for the hypothesis that the relationship between episodic self-knowledge and identity strength would be mediated by knowledge of personal semantic facts. However, there was also support for a reverse mediation model indicating that a strong sense of identity is associated with semantic self-knowledge and through this may enhance self-relevant recollection. The discussion elaborates on these findings and we propose a self-knowledge and identity model (SKIM) whereby semantic self-knowledge mediates a bidirectional relationship between episodic self-knowledge and identity. The relationship between autobiographical memory and identity has been the source of considerable interest and debate among philosophers and researchers in cognitive and social psychology. Traditionally, researchers have emphasized the importance of episodic self-knowledge (i.e., one’s experiences over time) for the maintenance of self (e.g., Schacter, 1996; Tulving, 2002). However, as Klein (2001) argues, this may simply reflect a failure to investigate the contribution of personal semantic memory (i.e., factual knowledge about the self). In fact, there is evidence that extensive loss of episodic self- knowledge, as occurs in amnesia, need not result in identity loss (e.g., Klein, Rozendal, & Cosmides, 2002; Rosenbaum et al., 2005). This has led researchers to suggest that Correspondence should be addressed to Dr Catherine Haslam, School of Psychology, University of Exeter, Exeter EX4 4QG, UK (e-mail: [email protected]). DOI:10.1348/000712610X508091

‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

184

British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203C© 2010 The British Psychological Society

TheBritishPsychologicalSociety

www.wileyonlinelibrary.com

‘I remember therefore I am, and I am thereforeI remember’: Exploring the contributionsof episodic and semantic self-knowledgeto strength of identity

Catherine Haslam1∗, Jolanda Jetten12, S. Alexander Haslam1,Cara Pugliese1 and James Tonks1

1School of Psychology, University of Exeter, UK2School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia

The present research explores the relationship between the two components of auto-biographical memory – episodic and semantic self-knowledge – and identity strength inolder adults living in the community and residential care. Participants (N = 32) completedthe autobiographical memory interview and measures of personal identity strength andmultiple group memberships. Contrary to previous research, autobiographical memoryfor all time periods (childhood, early adulthood, and recent life) in the semantic domainwas associated with greater strength in personal identity. Further, we obtained supportfor the hypothesis that the relationship between episodic self-knowledge and identitystrength would be mediated by knowledge of personal semantic facts. However, therewas also support for a reverse mediation model indicating that a strong sense of identityis associated with semantic self-knowledge and through this may enhance self-relevantrecollection. The discussion elaborates on these findings and we propose a self-knowledgeand identity model (SKIM) whereby semantic self-knowledge mediates a bidirectionalrelationship between episodic self-knowledge and identity.

The relationship between autobiographical memory and identity has been the sourceof considerable interest and debate among philosophers and researchers in cognitiveand social psychology. Traditionally, researchers have emphasized the importance ofepisodic self-knowledge (i.e., one’s experiences over time) for the maintenance of self(e.g., Schacter, 1996; Tulving, 2002). However, as Klein (2001) argues, this may simplyreflect a failure to investigate the contribution of personal semantic memory (i.e., factualknowledge about the self). In fact, there is evidence that extensive loss of episodic self-knowledge, as occurs in amnesia, need not result in identity loss (e.g., Klein, Rozendal,& Cosmides, 2002; Rosenbaum et al., 2005). This has led researchers to suggest that

∗Correspondence should be addressed to Dr Catherine Haslam, School of Psychology, University of Exeter, Exeter EX4 4QG,UK (e-mail: [email protected]).

DOI:10.1348/000712610X508091

Page 2: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

Self-knowledge and identity 185

semantic self-knowledge may also have an important role to play in maintaining theintegrity of self (see Klein, 2001; Klein Cosmides, Tooby, & Chance, 2002; Klein &Loftus, 1993). In this paper, we extend this argument by examining the relationshipsbetween these two forms of self-knowledge and identity strength. Specifically, based onprevious findings, our analysis starts from the proposition that knowledge of personalsemantic facts mediates the relationship between episodic self-knowledge and identityand we test this empirically in a sample of older adults living in the community andresidential care.

Memory and identityMost researchers agree that autobiographical memory has two components – episodicand semantic self-knowledge (e.g., Addis & Tippett, 2004; Baddeley, 1992; Kopelman,Wilson, & Baddeley, 1989, 1990). The former comprises knowledge of personallyexperienced events contextualized in time and place (Tulving, 1972). It supports ourcapacity to re-experience past events (i.e., to mentally time travel; Corballis, 2008;Suddendorf & Corballis, 2007; Tulving, 2002) and is a system that researchers believe isunique to humans, emerging comparatively recently in evolutionary terms from semanticmemory. Semantic self-knowledge comprises facts and details about oneself (e.g., whereyou attended school, your first job) in the absence of the personal experiences fromwhich they were abstracted and there is evidence that it is distinct from more genericsemantic knowledge (i.e., general knowledge about the world, objects, words, etc;Klein, Cosmides, Costabile, & Mei, 2002; Klein, Rozendal, et al., 2002; Levine et al.,2004). Although all semantic self-knowledge is episodic in origin (e.g., remembering thetime you fell from your bike during a race), over time personal facts from these episodesmay be recalled in the absence of the original memory experience (e.g., I am a cyclist;see Squire & Zola, 1998).1

There is perhaps greater debate regarding definitions of self or identity, as researcherstend to differ in their conceptualization of this construct as a function of their(meta)theoretical orientation. Some researchers focus on the importance of personal

representations of self, focusing on aspects that are unique to a person (e.g., emphasizingnotions of individuality, personality traits; e.g., Allport, 1924; Eysenck, 1953) to indexpersonal identity strength (or knowing ‘who I am’). Such approaches, however, oftenfail to explain how knowledge about the self is derived. The approach advocated in thepresent paper emphasizes the social dimensions of identity, whereby personal identitystrength and personally relevant knowledge is derived from the relationships we havewith important social groups (e.g., family, community groups). This perspective assertsthat we come to understand ourselves (e.g., to know that we are kind, friendly, but alsoirritable at times) primarily through our relationships with others. This reasoning is drawnfrom the social identity perspective (comprised of social identity and self-categorizationtheories; Tajfel & Turner, 1979; Turner, 1982; Turner, Oakes, Haslam, & McGarty, 1994),which emphasizes the importance of social groups in providing content and meaningto the self. According to this view, membership in social groups informs individual

1It is important to highlight the fact that this process – via which semantic memories are abstracted and emerge fromepisodic memory – is different to that proposed in episodic memory theory in which it is claimed that episodic memoryevolved from semantic memory at some point in human evolution (Tulving, 1985, 2001, 2002). The latter theory addressesthe development of memory systems in an evolutionary context, whereas our focus in the present paper is on the interactionbetween these systems as they are played out within the minds of humans on a day-to-day basis.

Page 3: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

186 Catherine Haslam et al.

cognition, emotion, and behaviour and group life is a primary source of self-knowledge(Jetten, Haslam, Pugliese, Tonks, & Haslam, 2010).

Interestingly, the former view, with its focus on personal trait knowledge, has tendedto dominate research investigating relationships between autobiographical memory andidentity in clinical populations (see Table 1). Aside from the fact that generating traits canbe problematic in populations whose fluency is compromised (e.g., dementia sufferers),we argue that it is important to examine the multi-level nature of identity and, in that way,encourage a more comprehensive conceptualization of identity.2 Accordingly, we extendupon previous research by incorporating measures that tap a broader representation, ortheory, of self, assessing both the strength of personal identity (see Iyer, Jetten, Tsivrikos,Postmes, & Haslam, 2009; Jetten et al., 2010) and the extent to which this is informed bysocial identity, or the affiliation that a person has with multiple social groups (see Brook,Garcia, & Fleming, 2008; Haslam et al., 2008; Iyer et al., 2009; Jetten et al., 2010).

Episodic self-knowledge, semantic self-knowledge, and identity: Implicationsof their lossPhilosophers (e.g., Grice, 1942; Locke, 1970) and psychologists (e.g., Addis & Tippett,2004, 2008; Bluck & Alea, 2008; Klein, 2001) have argued that having an understanding ofwho one is requires access to memories and, in particular, those relevant to our personalexperiences (i.e., episodic self-knowledge). Based on this view, one would predict thatloss of episodic self-knowledge should result in loss of self. Yet clinical evidence doesnot support this prediction (also see Klein, 2001). As summarized in Table 1, there arenumerous reports of individual patients who present with extensive loss of episodicself-knowledge, but still retain knowledge of who they are.

The findings from amnesic patients suggest that identity can be maintained providedthere is some preservation of semantic self-knowledge. An example is provided by K.C. who was unable to recall a single episode from his past, but was able to recall somepersonal facts from earlier lifetime periods which may have supported his capacity toaccurately rate past and present personality traits (Rosenbaum et al., 2005). Similarly,the data from Alzheimer patients suggests that identity loss occurs in the later stages ofthe disease when knowledge of personally relevant facts is compromised in addition tothat for personally relevant experiences. This is illustrated by K. R. (Klein, Cosmides, &Costabile, 2003) and D. B. (Hehman, German, & Klein, 2005), both of whom showedpreservation of trait knowledge but only for the period prior to disease onset.

Together, the body of patient data suggests that access to episodic knowledge maynot be critical for the integrity of identity and that some semblance of self remains tothe extent that semantic self-knowledge is preserved. Importantly, these findings do notalter the contribution of episodic self-knowledge, but merely highlight a role for personalsemantics in identity maintenance.

2While it is the case that some tests of personality traits, involving responses to ‘I am . . . ’ statements (e.g., the TwentyStatements Test; Kuhn & McPartland, 1954), have been used to access the multi-level nature of identity, responses aredifficult to code as it is not always clear whether the self provided is personal or social. More problematic for memory-impaired populations is the possibility that the measure taps aspects of the distant, and not current, self. Accordingly, for thesepopulations, the measure is confounded by general cognitive ability and, more specifically, memory ability.

Page 4: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

Self-knowledge and identity 187

Tabl

e1.

Sum

mar

yof

rese

arch

inve

stig

atin

gre

latio

nshi

psbe

twee

nep

isod

icse

lf-kn

owle

dge,

sem

antic

self-

know

ledg

e,an

did

entit

yin

patie

nts

with

amne

sia

and

dem

entia

Page 5: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

188 Catherine Haslam et al.

Tabl

e1.

(Con

tinue

d)

Page 6: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

Self-knowledge and identity 189

Models of memory and identitySo how are we to understand the interrelationships between identity and the two aspectsof self-knowledge? Klein and colleagues (Klein, Cosmides, Tooby, et al., 2002; Klein &Loftus, 1993) offer one framework in their scope hypothesis. There are two criticalaspects to their model – that knowledge about the self is represented in abstract formand may be functionally independent of the experiences that helped to embed them.In support of this, the authors have shown that inconsistent trait behaviours are morecritical when it comes to understanding the scope of generalizations from which we baseour trait summaries (Klein, Cosmides, Costabile, et al., 2002; Klein, Cosmides, Tooby,et al., 2002). Specifically, when asked to judge whether you are friendly, you draw on atrait summary and the extent to which this is truly representative will be determined byexperiences that are inconsistent with this trait. There are also various sources of supportfor the functional independence of trait knowledge from episodic self-knowledge, mostnotably from KC, who was able to acquire trait summaries post-injury despite an inabilityto access episodic memories (Tulving, 1993).

In this way, the scope hypothesis essentially defines identity in terms of personalitytraits. However, there are two limitations with such an approach. First, there areother components of identity (see Klein, 2004; Klein, Rozendal, et al., 2002), includingsemantic self-knowledge, that require investigation. Second, in the course of collapsingthese constructs it is difficult to investigate the interrelationships between them. Toaddress this issue, Conway and colleagues (Conway, 2005; Conway & Pleydell-Pearce,2000) offer another framework, the self-memory system (SMS), that separates episodicand semantic self-knowledge. The SMS has two components. The first is the workingself, responsible for tracking and maintaining coherence between, short- and long-termgoals. This is achieved through reference to the second component, the autobiographicalmemory knowledge base, which is a hierarchy of conceptual knowledge (from broadthemes to lifetime periods to general events) that terminate in episodic memories.Conway (2005) argues these two components of the SMS interact in the process ofremembering, but that they can also operate independently. The main aim of the SMSis to specify the processes involved in constructing and retrieving autobiographicalmemories regulated by the goal-driven working self.

Importantly, the interaction between the working self and autobiographical memoryis predicted to be reciprocal, in line with the schematic model presented in a recentpaper by Rathbone, Moulin, and Conway (2009). This model posits a bidirectionalrelationship between episodic autobiographical memory and the working self andbetween conceptual autobiographical knowledge and the working self. Unfortunately,though, the data on which the model is based do not speak to the issue of directionalityand the model provides no explanation for the interrelationships between the constructslinking the two pathways. Nevertheless, the general view that self-influences memory andmemory shapes our understanding of self is consistent with the wider cognitive literature.In particular, we know that reference to a person’s self-concept can influence, or cue,the personal experiences recalled (e.g., Humphreys & Kashima, 2002; Ross & Buehler,1994; Sanitioso, Kunda, & Fong, 1990) but at the same time that personal experiences canalter a person’s self-concept (e.g., Wilson, 2000). Indeed, this interdependence betweenself and memory has been acknowledged for some time (e.g., Fivush, 1988; Schank &Abelson, 1994; Wilson & Ross, 2003; but also see the review by Libby & Eibach, 2007)and the dynamics of this relationship are coherently summarized by Tessler and Nelson(1994) who argue that ‘self and memory organize, construct and give meaning to each

Page 7: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

190 Catherine Haslam et al.

other in a way so intimate that we can truly say that we are what we remember and thatour memories are ourselves’ (p. 321). A more complete model therefore needs to accountfor the interrelationships between episodic self-knowledge, semantic self-knowledge andidentity to establish how best to represent the bidirectionality.

The self-knowledge and identity modelAs we have seen, existing models of the relationship between self and memoryare either based on a limited conceptualization of identity (i.e., knowledge of traitslargely to the exclusion of other components of self) or fail to adequately explain theinterrelationships between variables and therefore do not help to articulate a testablemodel of bidirectionality. We address these limitations in the present research and offera model that incorporates what we know about the basic relationships between episodicand semantic self-knowledge and identity. First, we hypothesize that the accumulationof personal experience (or episodic self-knowledge) supports integrity of self (H1).Second, as knowledge of personal semantics is abstracted from these experiences,we hypothesize that episodic self-knowledge supports semantic self-knowledge (H2).Third, we hypothesize that personal semantic memories will also enhance a person’ssense of identity (H3). The resulting model is represented schematically in Figure 1and incorporates the additional hypothesis that the relationship between episodic self-knowledge and identity is mediated by semantic self-knowledge (H4). Put another way,this model suggests (a) that episodic self-knowledge supports a strong sense of selfbecause it is the basis for the acquisition of personal semantic knowledge and (b) that it isdevelopment of this latter knowledge base that supports a sense of identity. Accordingly,it may not be knowledge of events from their past, but the factual knowledge that theseevents give rise to that allows people to know themselves.

Yet on its own, this mediation model fails to capture the previously reportedbidirectional relationship between episodic self-knowledge and identity (e.g., Barclay& DeCooke, 1988; Conway, 2005; Rathbone et al., 2009; Robinson & Taylor, 1998).That is, while it is generally agreed that our experiences influence what we understandabout the self, it is also the case that knowledge about the self can shape memory retrieval(e.g., Ross, 1989). Relating this to the above model, it could be argued that integrity ofidentity is associated with semantic self-knowledge (see Klein, 2001; Klein, Cosmides,

Episodicself-knowledge

Semanticself-knowledge

H2 H3

Identity strengthH4H1

Figure 1. The self-knowledge and identity model. Note. This figure presents a schematic representationof hypothesized relationships between episodic self-knowledge, semantic self-knowledge, and identitystrength. In the primary model, episodic self-knowledge is a basis for semantic self-knowledge whichsupports identity strength. Lines in grey relate to a secondary mediational model in which theseassociations are reversed.

Page 8: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

Self-knowledge and identity 191

Tooby, et al., 2002), and through the latter mechanism, aids recollection of experiential(i.e., episodic) memories. Based on this reasoning, a person’s understanding of self (ortheir identity strength) might facilitate memories for particular events. This sequenceis the reverse of the primary mediational model presented in Figure 1 and is indicatedby the light grey arrows (where identity strength is the predictor variable and episodicself-knowledge is the outcome variable). These two paths are brought together in theself-knowledge and identity model (SKIM). This suggests a bidirectional relationshipbetween identity and episodic self-knowledge within which semantic self-knowledgeplays a key mediating role.

The present researchTo examine this model, we conducted a study to test both the primary mediationalmodel and the secondary reverse mediational model (together encapsulated in the SKIM)in a clinical sample. To this end, we assessed episodic self-knowledge, semantic self-knowledge, and identity strength in a sample of older adults living in the communityand in residential care. Our measures of episodic and semantic self-knowledge werecomprised, respectively, of the personal semantic and autobiographical incident com-ponents of the autobiographical memory interview (AMI; Kopelman et al., 1990). Thisallowed comparison with previous research that has explored the three constructs inwhich we were interested (see Table 1). However, our measures of identity differed inseeking (a) to capture a broader conceptualization of personal identity that goes beyondknowledge of personality traits, (b) to supplement measures of personal identity strengthwith measures that explored the social dimension of identity by assessing a person’saffiliation with multiple social groups, and (c) to avoid reliance on self-generation ofresponses which has been noted as a potential limitation in studies involving olderadults with dementia (Addis & Tippett, 2004).

Our rationale for testing the mediational model with older adults was driven bytwo factors. First, memories from different lifetime periods (i.e., childhood, earlyadulthood, and recent life) are more extensive in older adults. Second, inclusion ofolder adults from the community and from residential care allows consideration ofthe model’s generalizability to participants with a range of cognitive abilities. Basedon previous findings, we predict that greater integrity in episodic and semantic self-knowledge will be associated with greater identity strength. In testing the relationshipsbetween these three constructs, we predict that semantic self-knowledge will mediateboth the relationship between episodic self-knowledge and identity strength and therelationship between identity strength and episodic self-knowledge. We also examinedthe correlates of memory and personal identity strength and of memory and multiplegroup membership. Here, we predict positive associations between variables wherebyboth aspects of identity (i.e., personal and social) would allow for, and be strengthenedby, episodic and semantic self-knowledge.

MethodParticipantsA total of 32 older adults residing in the south-west of England were recruited for thisstudy. Seventeen were living in the community (9 male and 8 females, mean age = 81.18years, range = 65–97 years) and 15 were living in residential care (6 male and 9 female,

Page 9: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

192 Catherine Haslam et al.

mean age = 85.39 years, range = 74–99 years). These participants were recruited as partof a larger study investigating the effects of autobiographical memory and identity losson well-being (see Jetten et al., 2010). Exclusion criteria included prior history of anypsychiatric illness, significant language impairment, and poor comprehension.

ProcedureEthical approval for the study was provided by the Psychology Ethics Committee at theresearchers’ university. All participants were interviewed individually. During interviewtheir general cognitive ability and memory was tested and additional informationconcerning identity was collected. Those residing in the community were recruited viathe School of Psychology participant panel and interviewed in their home. Participantsin residential care were recruited during coffee mornings with an advocate present (ifrequired) and were interviewed in their own rooms. Allowing for regular breaks, thetotal duration of interviews was between 1 and 3 h, though only a portion of the datacollected are reported in the present paper.

MaterialsThe AMI (Kopelman et al., 1990) was used to assess retrieval of semantic and episodicself-knowledge. Questions relevant to personal semantic memories involved retrieval ofpersonal facts from a person’s past life (e.g., names of teachers, friends, school location).Those relevant to episodic memory (i.e., referred to as ‘autobiographical incidents’ inthe test) involved retrieval of events or incidents from the past (e.g., recalling an incidentfrom college or from a first job, with prompts such as ‘your first day at work’). In eachdomain, participants were required to retrieve memories from three lifetime periods –childhood, early adulthood, and recent life – with questions and prompts used to tap arange of memories across each period (e.g., for childhood memories, respondents wereasked to recall memories from before school, their first school, and their main/secondaryschool). Responses were scored by two raters using the standard criteria and examplesoffered in the test, and this was also used to resolve any discrepancy between raters.This was particularly important for autobiographical incidents which were scored on afour-point scale. A score of 3 was assigned to memories that were specific in time andplace; a score of 2 to less specific events in which either time or place was not recalled;a score of 1 for vague personal memories; and a score of 0 for either non-responses or aresponse based on general semantic knowledge. Data obtained from the overall measureincluded individual scores for each time period in each domain (score range = 0–21 forpersonal semantic memories and 0–9 for autobiographical incidents) and a total scorefor personal semantic memories (max = 63) and autobiographical incidents (max = 27).In both domains, a higher score indicates superior memory retrieval.

A questionnaire was administered in order to measure personal identity and socialidentity – the latter indexed through membership of multiple social groups. Items wereread aloud by the interviewer and participants were asked to respond using a five-pointscale ranging from 1 ‘do not agree at all’, to 5 ‘agree completely’. The five-point responsescale was also presented visually on an A4 sheet to facilitate responding. Five items (� =.65) were used to assess the extent to which participants had a clear understandingof who they were, providing a index of personal identity strength (from Jetten et al.,2010). These items were adapted from the self-clarity scale developed by Campbell et al.

(1996) and the personal identity strength scale employed by Baray, Postmes, and Jetten

Page 10: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

Self-knowledge and identity 193

(2009). These items were: ‘I know what I like and what I don’t like’, ‘I know whatmy morals are’, ‘I have strong beliefs’, ‘I know what I want from life’, and ‘I am awareof the roles and responsibilities I have in my life’. Three items assessed the extent towhich participants belonged to multiple social groups, providing an index into theirsocial networks. For each scale, responses were totalled and the average score recorded.These scales have been used widely in previous research (e.g., Jetten et al., 2010) andwere found to have good reliability in a sample of stroke sufferers (Haslam et al., 2008)and among students entering university (Iyer et al., 2009). These items were: ‘I am amember of lots of different groups’, ‘I am active in lots of different social groups’, and ‘Ihave friends who are in lots of different groups’ (� = .84).

The Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination – Revised (ACE-R; Mioshi, Dawson,Mitchell, Arnold, & Hodges, 2006) was administered to provide a brief measure of generalcognitive ability. It has previously been used to detect early stage dementia and examinesfive cognitive domains: attention/orientation, memory, verbal fluency, language, andvisuospatial ability. The maximum score is 100 with higher scores indicating superiorcognitive function. A cut-off score of 82/100 has been shown to have 84% sensitivity and100% specificity in diagnosing dementia (Mioshi et al., 2006).

ResultsPreliminary analysis revealed no differences between the community and residential caresamples in age (t(30) = 1.3, p = .21) or gender (� 2(1) = 0.54, p = .46). However, aswould be expected, there were differences in ACE-R scores (t(30) = −3.27, p = .003),such that those residing in care (M = 60.33, SD = 20.49) scored lower than those livingin the community (M = 79.59, SD = 12.25).

Mean data for the autobiographical memory and identity measures are presentedin Table 2. Analysis indicated that people residing in care retrieved fewer personalsemantic and episodic (i.e., autobiographical incident) memories than those living inthe community (semantic self-knowledge: F(1,30) = 35.91, p < .001; episodic self-knowledge: F(1,30) = 13.97, p = .001). There were no effects for time period, with

Table 2. Mean scores and standard deviations for autobiographical memory and identity measures forparticipants (N = 32) residing in the community and in care

Page 11: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

194 Catherine Haslam et al.

recall being similar across childhood, early adulthood, and recent life periods (semanticself-knowledge: F(2,29) = 2.4, p = .11; episodic self-knowledge: F(2,29) = 0.16, p =.86). There were also no interactions between care level and time period (semanticself-knowledge: F(2,29) = 0.70, p = .51; episodic self-knowledge: F(2,29) = 2.25, p =.12). In the case of identity measures, we found differences in personal identity strength,with those in the community reporting a stronger self of self than those in care (t(30) =2.93, p = .006), but no differences in social identity (t(30) = 1.83, p = .08).

CorrelationsPartial correlations, controlling for general cognitive performance (i.e., ACE-R), werecomputed in order to explore relationships between memory and identity. First, weexamined relationships between the personal semantic memories and autobiographicalincident total scores and the two measures of identity, controlling for general cognitiveability (i.e., ACE-R scores). All variables were positively correlated, and the strength ofall correlations was either moderate or strong as indicated in Table 3. This is particularly

Table 3. Partial correlations between autobiographical memory total scores and identity measures,controlling for general cognitive performance (ACE-R)

noteworthy in light of the different measures used to index each construct. Importantly,the relationship we predicted between personal identity and multiple (or social) groupmembership was significant and of moderate strength.

We were most interested in relationships between the particular lifetime periodsand identity measures and for this reason we also report the correlations betweenmemory and identity measures broken down as a function of childhood, early adulthood,and recent life (see Table 4). Of the two identity measures, personal identity strengthwas more strongly associated with autobiographical memory retrieval (.31 < r < .70).Importantly, these associations emerged for knowledge of personal facts as well as forpast events and involved all time periods, with the exception of recent episodic (i.e.,autobiographical incident) memories. Previous research has only found an associationbetween particular time periods (i.e., childhood and early adulthood; Addis & Tippett,2004). In contrast, our findings indicate that the integrity of personal identity is related tothe ability to retrieve semantic self-knowledge irrespective of the time period in whichthey were originally experienced. The relationships between social identity measuresand autobiographical memory were weaker in comparison (−.003 < r < .43), but againit was in the personal semantic domain that all time periods contributed to integrity ofself as defined by multiple group membership.

As the correlational data showed that knowledge of personal semantics was morestrongly associated with identity measures, we extended our analysis and conductedhierarchical regression to determine whether particular time periods, namely childhood

Page 12: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

Self-knowledge and identity 195

Table 4. Partial correlations between autobiographical memory lifetime periods and identity measures,controlling for general cognitive performance (ACE-R)

and early adulthood – as suggested by previous research – were more critical to identity(Addis & Tippett, 2004). Using the procedure reported in Addis and Tippett (2004),ACE-R scores were entered into the first block to control for cognitive decline. This wasfollowed by childhood and early adulthood components of personal semantic memoriesfrom the AMI in the second block. In the third block, the recent life component ofpersonal semantic memories from the AMI was entered. These analyses were conductedseparately for personal identity strength and multiple group membership. In the case ofpersonal identity strength, we found that inclusion of the ACE-R produced a significantchangeover a model containing the constant (R2 change = .32, F(1,30) = 13.83, p =.001). The addition of childhood and early adulthood components resulted in a significantimprovement in the model (R2 change = .27, F(3,28) = 13.17, p < .001), as did inclusionof the recent life component (R2 change = .07, F(4,27) = 12.64, p = .03). Hence, alltime periods made a significant contribution to prediction of personal identity strength.In the case of multiple group membership, however, no time period made a significantcontribution over and above the initial constant model. The model containing the ACE-Rdid not produce a significant change over that based on a constant (R2 change = .04,F(1,30) = 1.3, p = .256), nor was there a significant change with inclusion of childhoodand early adulthood components (R2 change = .18, F(3,28) = 2.63, p = .06) or therecent life component (R2 change = .006, F(4,27) = 1.97, p = .13).

Mediational analysesTo test the SKIM model proposed in Figure 1, in which the relationship between episodicself-knowledge (i.e., recall of autobiographical incidents) and identity is mediated byknowledge of personal facts (i.e., personal semantics), a series of regression analyseswere conducted along lines recommended by Baron and Kenny (1986). Given its strongerassociation with the different components of the AMI, all analyses were undertakenincluding only the personal identity strength measure.

First, we found that episodic self-knowledge predicted personal identity strength, � =0.62, t = 4.35, p < .001, suggesting that retrieval of more autobiographical incidents wasassociated with greater personal identity strength. Second, we examined the relationshipbetween the predictor variable (episodic self-knowledge) and the mediator (semantic

Page 13: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

196 Catherine Haslam et al.

self-knowledge). Again there was a significant positive relationship between thesevariables, � = 0.78, t = 6.9, p < .001; participants who retrieved more autobiographicalincidents also recalled more personally relevant facts. A final step involved regressingidentity strength on both the predictor variable and mediator. Results are presented inFigure 2a. In line with the requirements for mediation, we found that the previouslysignificant relationship between episodic knowledge and identity strength (a) wasrendered non-significant (� = −0.013, t = −0.07, p = .94) and (b) was significantlyreduced (Sobel’s test z = 3.12, p = .002), while (c) the influence of personal semanticson identity strength was significant (� = 0.81, t = 4.56, p < .001). This finding isconsistent with the suggestion that episodic knowledge contributes to identity strengthbecause it provides the basis for the development of knowledge of personal facts fromwhich a theory of self is formed.

We then tested the reverse mediation model in which episodic self-knowledgewas regressed on both identity strength (the predictor) and semantic self-knowledge(the mediator; see Figure 2b). The regression revealed that the previously significantrelationship between personal identity strength and autobiographical incidents was nolonger significant (� = −0.014, t = −0.07, p = .94) and was also significantly reduced(Sobel’s test z = 2.67, p = .007). Thus, consistent with the second path outlined inthe proposed bidirectional SKIM, semantic self-knowledge was found to mediate therelationship between personal identity strength and knowledge of past events. Thisfinding is consistent with the suggestion that a strong sense of personal identity predictssuperior recollection of episodic self-knowledge because it is associated with superiorrecall of personal facts.3

In summary, our analysis suggests that knowledge of personal episodes leads to astrong sense of personal identity because that knowledge is a basis for awareness ofpersonal facts (i.e., I have a strong sense of self because I remember what I did andtherefore know facts about myself). Intriguingly, though, there is equally compellingevidence that a strong sense of personal identity leads to recall of personal episodesbecause it is associated with awareness of personal facts (i.e., I remember what I didbecause I have a strong sense of self and therefore know facts about myself).

DiscussionThis study explored the relationship between episodic self-knowledge, semantic self-knowledge, and identity in older adults residing in the community and in care. Extendingprevious research, we incorporated measures that tapped both personal and social

3Theoretically, there is no reason to expect that the predicted relationships between episodic self-knowledge, semantic self-knowledge, and identity strength would differ for the two populations. However, to explore this issue, we repeated the primaryand reverse mediation analyses for each group separately. While some of the relationships were only marginally significant(due to low power) the same pattern of relationships emerged. Essentially, when personal identity strength was regressedon both episodic and semantic self-knowledge the previous relationship between episodic memory and identity was reduced(standard care: β = 0.19, t = 99, p = .34; community: β = 0.37, t = 1.54, p = .14) and the influence of semanticself-knowledge on identity strength was significant (standard care: β = 0.74, t = 3.97, p = .002; community: β = 0.81,t = 7.88, p = .001). Similar results emerged when examining evidence for reversed mediation. When episodic self-knowledgewas regressed on both personal identity strength and semantic self-knowledge the relationship between identity strengthand episodic self-knowledge was reduced in strength (standard care: β = 0.44, t = 1.31, p = .18; community: β = 0.08,t = −0.302, p = .78) and the influence of identity strength on episodic self-knowledge remained strong (standard care:β = 0.38, t = 1.6, p = .06; community: β = 0.47, t = 2.06, p = .055). For this reason, we report the collapsed data in themain text.

Page 14: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

Self-knowledge and identity 197

.62**/–.01Δ

.72** .81**

.62**/–.01Δ

.80** .71**

Episodicself-knowledge

Semanticself-knowledge

(a)

(b)

Identity strength

Identity strength

Semanticself-knowledge

Episodicself-knowledge

Figure 2. Mediation analysis for (a) relationship between autobiographical incidents and identitystrength and (b) reverse model. Note. Correlations: ∗∗p ≤ .01; change: �p ≤ .05; N = 32, dottedlines indicate where a previously significant relationship is rendered non-significant.

dimensions of identity. Results of correlational analysis revealed highly significantpositive associations between semantic measures of autobiographical memory andpersonal identity strength. More interestingly, though, we found that semantic self-knowledge mediated the relationship between episodic self-knowledge and personalidentity strength. This pattern is consistent with the suggestion that episodic self-knowledge contributes to personal identity strength because it provides the basis forsemantic self-knowledge. At the same time, though, there was also support for a secondmediational model in which these associations were reversed. This suggests that a strongsense of personal identity predicts enhanced episodic self-knowledge because it is thebasis for superior semantic self-knowledge.

Autobiographical memory and identityAs reported in previous research, we found that both autobiographical memorycomponents were associated with integrity of self (as indexed by personal and socialidentity measures), but mainly for personal semantic memories. Thus, poorer retrievalof semantic self-knowledge was associated with reduced identity strength. Extendingprevious findings, we found that this relationship emerged consistently across all timeperiods – childhood, early adulthood, and recent life – though results of hierarchicalregression only confirmed this finding in the case of our personal identity strengthmeasure. This finding is consistent with evidence that people experience a rangeof important identity-related transitions throughout the course of their lives, each ofwhich has a significant bearing upon self-definition (e.g., Iyer, Jetten, & Tsivrikos,2008; Sani, 2008). Nevertheless, it is at odds with previous claims that memoriesfrom childhood and early adulthood periods are distinctly self-defining (e.g., Addis &

Page 15: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

198 Catherine Haslam et al.

Tippett, 2004; Fitzgerald, 1988, 1996, 1999; Holmes & Conway, 1999). Researchershave previously suggested that this time period (often referred to as the ‘reminiscencebump’) is when more stable identities are developed (Fitzgerald, 1988, 1996, 1999)and the interdependence between memory and identity seems to support the increasedaccessibility of autobiographical memories associated with these times periods.

It may be the case, of course, that the inconsistency between our findings, and thoseof previous studies reflects differences in samples and measures. In particular, some ofthe previous studies that investigated the reminiscence bump account were conductedwith younger adults for whom recent memories were more temporally proximal tothose from early adulthood. Such proximity also makes it more difficult to distinguishthe contribution of memories from different time periods. In the particular case ofAddis and Tippett’s (2004) sample (which had a similar age profile to ours), it is possiblethat the challenge of generating self-statements was responsible for the failure to observestronger relationships between memory and identity across the life-span. This suggestionis lent some credibility by the results of a recent study reported by Rathbone et al. (2009).As with the present study, this indicated that self-defining memories were not isolated tothe reminiscence bump period, but occurred across the life-span. These data, togetherwith our own, suggest that identity processes (and not just temporal context) are criticalto the organization of autobiographical memories.

Having found that all time periods in the personal semantic domain were associatedwith integrity of self, it is important to note that these patterns emerged on measuresof both personal identity strength and multiple group membership. This confirms thevalue of exploring different levels of the self by examining not only personal identity,but also the social identities that inform and strengthen it. Accordingly, and in linewith social identity reasoning, it would appear that memory is related to both personaland social aspects of the self. However, we need to acknowledge that while results ofhierarchical regression supported the contribution of all time periods when predictingpersonal identity strength, lifetime period did not contribute to predicting social identity(or multiple group membership). It is likely that this reflects the different aspects ofidentity that these measures tapped and, even though multiple group membershipseemed to inform personal identity, its association with the semantic components ofautobiographical memory was clearly weaker.

Semantic and episodic self-knowledge and identityMediation analysis focused on the relationships between the components of autobio-graphical memory and personal identity strength. Results of this analysis showed thatknowledge of personal semantics mediated the relationship between episodic self-knowledge and identity. Theoretically, this pattern is consistent with the suggestion(a) that episodic memories contribute to identity strength because they provide thefoundation from which knowledge of personal facts are abstracted and (b) that it is thisfactual base from which a theory of self is supported. Importantly, this finding doesnot diminish the contribution of episodic self-knowledge to a person’s sense of self, assemantic self-knowledge is based on this. Thus, the analysis highlights the nature of thepersonal semantic contribution to identity in the context of accounting for knowledgeof past events and episodes.

However, in addition to evidence that supported our primary mediation model, wealso found that personal semantic memory mediated the reverse relationship betweenidentity strength and episodic self-knowledge. This reverse model supports claims that

Page 16: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

Self-knowledge and identity 199

identity is a basis for memory as much as memory is a basis for identity (see Klein, 2001).Thus, results of the primary mediational analysis are consistent with the suggestion thatone’s conscious memories of the past (i.e., autobiographical incidents) support one’ssense (or theory) of self (i.e., I know who I am because of what I remember); yet resultsof the second reversed mediational analysis suggest that one’s theory of self may alsodetermine what one remembers (i.e., knowing who I am determines what I remember).

The fact that there was evidence for both primary and secondary mediational modelstherefore supports claims that there is a bidirectional relationship between episodic self-knowledge and identity. Along the lines of the SKIM presented in Figure 1, this suggestthat these processes are cyclical in nature or involve feedback loops such that memoryboth shapes, and is shaped by, identity. This possibility has been alluded to in previousresearch (e.g., Barclay & DeCooke, 1988; Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Robinson &Taylor, 1998) but has typically only been captured in simple bivariate correlations, notin mediation analysis of the form undertaken here. Having said this, there are limitationswith an analysis of this form. While the analysis allowed us to test mechanisms throughwhich episodic self-knowledge influenced identity, the data are correlational and cross-sectional and thus cannot inform us about causality. This should be addressed throughexperimental and longitudinal investigation of autobiographical memory and identity indementia populations, and this sets an important agenda for future work in this area.

ConclusionThe present paper makes several important contributions to our understanding of therelationship between autobiographical memory and identity. First, it highlights theimportant contribution that all time periods (childhood, early adulthood, and recentlife) make to integrity of self, and thereby challenges the suggestion that only particulardevelopmental periods are self-defining. Second, it offers a novel perspective from whichto understand the contribution that the two components of autobiographical memorymake to identity – suggesting that semantic self-knowledge might play a mediatingrole in the relationship between memory for autobiographical incidents and a strongsense of self. At the same time, though, there was also evidence of a bidirectionalrelationship between episodic self-knowledge and identity. This suggests not only thatmemory influences what one understands about the self and hence the strength of one’ssense of identity, but also that a strong sense of identity is associated with enhancedunderstanding of the self and thereby shapes memory retrieval. To be is to know andtherefore to remember; to remember is to know and therefore to be.

AcknowledgementsWe are grateful to the staff and residents of Somerset Care Limited and to Age Concern fortheir help with this study. This research was supported by a grant from the Economic andSocial Research Council (RES-062-23-0135).

ReferencesAddis, D. R., & Tippett, L. J. (2004). Memory of myself: Autobiographical memory and identity in

Alzheimer’s disease. Memory, 12, 56–74. doi:10.1080/09658210244000423

Page 17: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

200 Catherine Haslam et al.

Addis, D. R., & Tippett, L. J. (2008). The contributions of autobiographical memory to the contentand continuity of identity: A social-cognitive neuroscience approach. In F. Sani (Ed.), Self

continuity: Individual and collective perspectives (pp. 71–84). New York: Psychology Press.Allport, F. H. (1924). Social psychology. New York: Houghton Mifflin.Baddeley, A. D. (1992). What is autobiographical memory? In M. A. Conway, D. C. Rubin, H.

Spinnler & W. A. Wagenaar (Eds.), Theoretical perspectives on autobiographical memory

(Vol. 65, pp. 13–29). Dordrecht: Kluwer.Barclay, C. R., & DeCooke, P. A. (1988). Ordinary everyday memories: Some of the things of which

selves are made. In U. Neisser & E. Winograd (Eds.), Remembering reconsidered: Ecological

and traditional approaches to the study of memory (pp. 91–125). New York: CambridgeUniversity Press.

Baray, G., Postmes, T., & Jetten, J. (2009). When I equals We: Exploring the relation betweensocial and personal identity of extreme right-wing political party members. British Journal of

Social Psychology, 48, 625–647. doi:10.1348/014466608X389582Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psycho-

logical research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 51, 602–619. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173Bluck, S., & Alea, N. (2008). Remembering being me: The self continuity function of autobiograph-

ical memory in younger and older adults. In F. Sani (Ed.), Self continuity: Individual and

collective perspectives (pp. 55–90). New York: Psychology Press.Brook, A. T., Garcia, J., & Fleming, M. (2008). The effects of multiple identities on

psychological well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1588–1600.doi:10.1177/0146167208324629

Campbell, J. D., Trapnell, P. D., Heine, S. J., Katz, I. M., Lavallee, L. F., & Lehman, D. R. (1996).Self-concept clarity: Measurement, personality correlates, and cultural boundaries. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 141–156. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.1.141Conway, M. A. (2005). Memory and the self. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 594–628.

doi:10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.005Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical mem-

ories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261–288. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.2.261

Corballis, M. (2008). Mental time travel and the shaping of language. Experimental Brain Research.Retrieved from http://www.springerlink.com/content/5489515n53w13758/

Eysenck, H. (1953). The structure of human personality. London: Methuen.Fitzgerald, J. M. (1988). Vivid memories and the reminiscence phenomenon: The role of a self-

narrative. Human Development, 31, 261–273. doi:10.1159/000275814Fitzgerald, J. M. (1996). The distribution of self-narrative memories in younger and older adults:

Elaborating the self-narrative hypothesis. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 3, 229–236.doi:10.1080/13825589608256626

Fitzgerald, J. M. (1999). Autobiographical memory and social cognition. In T. M. Hess & F. Blanchart-Fields (Eds.), Social cognition and aging (143–171). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Fivush, R. (1988). The functions of event memory: Some comments on Nelson and Barsalou.In U. Neisser & E. Winograd (Eds.), Remembering reconsidered: Ecological and traditional

approaches to the study of memory (pp. 277–282). New York: Cambridge University Press.Grice, H. P. (1942). Personal identity. Mind, 50, 330–350.Haslam, C., Holme, A., Haslam, S. A., Iyer, A., Jetten, J., & Williams, W. H. (2008). Maintaining

group memberships: Social identity continuity predicts well-being after stroke. Journal of

Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 18, 671–691. doi:10.1080/09602010701643449Hehman, J. A., German, T. P., & Klein, S. B. (2005). Impaired self-recognition from recent

photographs in a case of late-stage Alzheimer’s disease. Social Cognition, 23, 118–123.doi:10.1521/soco.23.1.118.59197

Holmes, A., & Conway, M. A. (1999). Generation identity and the reminiscence bump: Memoryfor public and private events. Journal of Development, 6 , 21–34.

Page 18: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

Self-knowledge and identity 201

Humphreys, M. S., & Kashima, Y. (2002). Connectionism and self: Distributed representationalsystem and their implications for self and identity. In M. Foddy, Y. Kashima & M. J. Platow(Eds.), Self and identity: Personal, social and symbolic (pp. 27–54). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Iyer, A., Jetten, J., & Tsivrikos, D. (2008). Torn between identities: Predictors of adjustmentto identity change. In F. Sani (Ed.), Self-continuity: Individual and collective perspectives

(pp. 187–197). New York: Psychology Press.Iyer, A., Jetten, J., Tsivrikos, D., Postmes, T., & Haslam, S. A. (2009). The more (and the

more compatible) the merrier: Multiple group memberships and identity compatibility aspredictors of adjustment after life transitions. British Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 707–733. doi:10.1348/014466608X397628

Jetten, J., Haslam, C., Pugliese, C., Tonks, J., & Haslam, S. A. (2010). Declining autobiographicalmemory and the loss of identity: Effects on well-being. Journal of Clinical and Experimental

Neuropsychology, 32, 408–416.Klein, S. B. (2001). A self to remember: A cognitive neuropsychological perspective on how self

creates memory and memory creates self. In C. Sedikides & M. B. Brewer (Eds.), Individual

self, relational self, collective self (pp. 25–46). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.Klein, S. B. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of knowing one’s self. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.),

The Cognitive Neurosciences III (pp. 1077–1089). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Klein, S. B., Cosmides, L., & Costabile, K. A. (2003). Preserved knowledge of self in Azheimer’s

dementia. Social Cognition, 21, 157–165. doi:10.1521/soco.21.2.157.21317Klein, S. B., Cosmides, L., Costabile, K. A., & Mei, L. (2002). Is there something special about

the self? A neuropsychological case study. Journal of research in Personality, 36 , 490–506.doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00001-6

Klein, S. B., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., & Chance, S. (2002). Decisions and the evolution of memory:Multiple systems, multiple functions. Psychological Review, 109, 306–329. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.2.306

Klein, S. B., & Loftus, J. (1993). The mental representation of trait and autobiographical knowledgeabout the self. In T. S. Srull & R. S. Wyer (Eds.), The mental representation of trait and

autobiographical knowledge about the self: Advances in social cognition Volume V (pp. 1–49). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1996). Self-knowledge of an amnesic patient: Toward aneuropsychology of personality and social psychology. Journal of Experimental Psychology,125, 250–260.

Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (2002). Memory and temporal experience: The effects ofepisodic memory loss on an amnesic patient’s ability to remember the past and imagine thefuture. Social Cognition, 5, 353–357.

Klein, S. B., Rozendal, K., & Cosmides, L. (2002). A social-cognitive neuroscience analysis of theself. Social Cognition, 20, 105–135. doi:10.1521/soco.20.2.105.20991

Kopelman, M. D., Wilson, B. A., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). The autobiographical memoryinterview: A new assessment of autobiographical and personal semantic memory in am-nesic patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 724–744.doi:10.1080/01688638908400928

Kopelman, M. D., Wilson, B. A., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). The autobiographical memory

interview (manual). Windsor: Thames Valley Test Company.Kuhn, M. H., & McPartland, T. S. (1954). An empirical investigation of self-attributes. American

Sociological Review, 19, 68–76. doi:10.2307/2088175Levine, B., Turner, G. R., Tisserarnd, D., Hevener, S. J., Graham, S. J., & McIntosh, A. R. (2004).

The functional neuroanatomy and episodic and semantic autobiographical remembering:A prospective functional MRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16 , 1633–1646.doi:10.1162/0898929042568587

Libby, L. K., & Eibach, R. P. (2007). How the self affects and reflects the content and subjectiveexperience of autobiographical memory. In C. Sedikides & S. J. Spencer (Eds.), The self (pp. 75–91). New York: Psychology Press.

Page 19: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

202 Catherine Haslam et al.

Locke, J. (1970). An essay concerning human understanding (2nd ed.). Yorkshire: The ScholarPress (Original work published in 1694).

McCarthy, R. A., & Hodges, J. R. (1995). Loss of remote memory: A cognitive neuropsy-chological perspective. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5, 178–183. doi:10.1016/0959-4388(95)80024-7

Mioshi, E., Dawson, K., Mitchell, J., Arnold, R., & Hodges, J. R. (2006). The Addenbrooke’sCognitive Examination Revised (ACE-R): A brief cognitive test battery for dementia screening.International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 21, 1078–1085. doi:10.1002/gps.1610

Rathbone, C. J., Moulin, C. J. A., & Conway, M. A. (2009). Autobiographical memoryand amnesia: Using conceptual knowledge to ground the self. Neurocase, 15, 405–418.doi:10.1080/13554790902849164

Robinson, J. A., & Taylor, L. R. (1998). Autobiographical memory and self-narratives: A tale oftwo stories. In C. P. Thompson, D. J. Herrmann, D. Bruce, J. D. Read, D. G. Payne, & M. P.Toglia (Eds.), Autobiographical memory: Theoretical and applied perspectives (pp. 125–143).Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Rosenbaum, R. S., Kohler, S., Schacter, D. L., Moscovitch, M., Westmacott, R., Black, S. E.,. . . Tulving, E. (2005). The case of KC: Contributions of a memory-impaired person to memorytheory. Neuropsychologia, 43, 989–1021. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.10.007

Ross, M. (1989). Relation of implicit theories to the construction of personal histories. Psycholog-

ical Review, 96 , 341–357. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.96.2.341Ross, M., & Buehler, R. (1994). Creative remembering. In U. Neisser & R. Fivush (Eds.), The

remembering self (pp. 205–235). New York: Cambridge University Press.Sani, F. (2008). Self continuity: Individual and collective perspectives. New York: Psychology

Press.Sanitioso, R., Kunda, Z., & Fong, G. T. (1990). Motivated recruitment of autobiographical memories.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 229–241. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.2.229Schacter, D. L. (1996). Searching for memory: The brain, the mind and the past. New York:

Basic Books.Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1994). Knowledge and memory: The real story. In R. S. Wyer (Ed.),

Advances in social cognition (Vol. 8, pp. 1–85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Squire, L. R., & Zola, S. M. (1998). Episodic memory, semantic memory and amnesia. Hippocampus,

8, 205–211. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:3<205::AID-HIPO3>3.0.CO;2-ISuddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel,

and is it unique to humans? Behavioural Brain Sciences, 30, 299–351.Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin &

S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–48). Monterey, CA:Brooks/Cole.

Tessler, M., & Nelson, K. (1994). Making memories: The influence of joint encoding on later recallby young children. Consciousness and Cognition, 3, 307–326. doi:10.1006/ccog.1994.1018

Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.),Organization of memory (pp. 381–403). New York: Academic Press.

Tulving, E. (1985). How many memory systems are there? American Psychologist, 40, 385–398.doi:10.1037/0003-066X.40.4.385

Tulving, E. (1993). Self-knowledge of an amnesic individual is represented abstractly. In T. K. Srull& R. S. Wyer (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 5, pp. 147–156). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Tulving, E. (2001). The origin of autonoesis in episodic memory. In H. L. Roediger, J. S. Nairne, I.Neath, & A. M. Suprenant (Eds.), The nature of remembering: Essays in Honour of Robert G.

Crowder (pp. 17–34). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53,

1–25. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114Turner, J. C. (1982). Towards a cognitive redefinition of the social group. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Social

identity and intergroup relations (pp. 15–40). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Page 20: ‘I remember therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember ... BJP Memory and identity.pdf184 British Journal of Psychology (2011), 102, 184–203 C 2010 The British Psychological

Self-knowledge and identity 203

Turner, J. C., Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & McGarty, C. A. (1994). Self and collective:Cognition and social context. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 454–463.doi:10.1177/0146167294205002

Westmacott, R., Leach, L., Freedman, M., & Moscovitch, M. (2001). Different patterns ofautobiographical memory loss in semantic dementia and medial temporal lobe amnesia: Achallenge to consolidation theory. Neurocase, 7, 37–55. doi:10.1093/neucas/7.1.37

Wilson, A. E. (2000). The frequency of temporal-self and social comparisons in people’s personalappraisals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 928–942. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.928

Wilson, A. E., & Ross, M. (2003). The identity function of autobiographical memory: Time is onour side. Memory, 11, 137–149. doi:10.1080/741938210

Wilson, B. A., Baddeley, A. D., & Kapur, N. (1995). Dense amnesia in a professional musicianfollowing herpes simplex virus encephalitis. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsy-

chology, 17, 668–681. doi:10.1080/01688639508405157Wilson, B. A., & Wearing, D. (1995). Prisoner of consciousness: A state of just awakening following

Herpes Simplex Encephalitis. In R. Campbell & M. Conway (Eds.), Broken memories (pp. 14–30). Oxford: Blackwell.

Received 27 October 2009; revised version received 24 March 2010