5
634 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2001, 43: 634–638 Adult outcome of childhood learning disability Greg O’Brien FRCPsych FRCPCH MD Professor of Developmental Psychiatry, University of Northumbria. Correspondence to author at Northgate Hospital, Morpeth, Northumberland, NE61 3BP, UK. ‘But what are the prospects for the future, Doctor?’ This ques- tion is frequently asked by parents of children with learning disability.* But longitudinal studies of the outcome in adult- hood of childhood learning disability are lacking. 1 At the same time, extrapolating prognoses from the available evidence is unreliable. Increasingly, however, clinicians are faced with this basic question that is on the minds of parents who have gleaned ‘information’ from other sources, notably the inter- net. So, what can be usefully and reliably claimed regarding outcome in adulthood for children with learning disabilities? Outcome studies in learning disability Studies of the outcome of childhood learning disability have adopted a variety of strategies. Various outcome measures have been used including: (1) life expectancy, (2) general health, (3) adaptive functioning, (4) psychopathology, (5) service contact, and (6) integration into adult life. Early stud- ies focused mainly on morbidity and mortality (e.g. Decker and colleagues, 2 Richards and Sylvester 3 ), while more recent- ly the emphasis has shifted towards systematic consideration of quality of life . 15, 18 Life expectancy There is a long tradition of research on the mortality of people with learning disability. A variety of studies (e.g. Richards and Sylvester, 3 Balkrishnan and Wolff 4 ) have demonstrated that, in general, children with learning disability have a shorter life expectancy than other children. Previously, most of these stud- ies were carried out on long-stay hospital patients employing case note data, but the same outcome has been found in the recent community-based study by Hollins and colleagues, 5 that examined death certification data of individuals with learning disability. These studies have clarified that mortality increases with increasing severity of learning disability, most notably among those with IQ levels below 40. 5–7 Other high risk factors which predict or are associated with earlier death in people with learning disability include any additional physical disabili- ty or medical problem, notably cerebral palsy and poorly con- trolled epilepsy. Non-mobile children and those incapable of self-feeding are especially at risk of short life expectancy. 6 Another strategy has been to concentrate on the cause of death among people with learning disability. In a major 50-year study Carter and Jancar 8 found that before 1955, tuber- culosis was a common cause of death among people with learning disability while other respiratory infections were implicated more commonly in later deaths. Between 1930 and 1980, death rates by arterial disease increased while death from status epilepticus decreased. They also found that from 1959 to 1968, cancer became a more common cause of death among people with learning disability. These three latter trends mir- ror changes in lifestyle and improvements in medical treat- ment of people with learning disability over the last 50 years. Subsequently, Dupont and coworkers 9 applied the concept of ‘avoidable mortality’ to learning disability. A cause-specific mortality calculation was devised with the aim of demonstrat- ing the extent to which mortality among people with learning disability might be potentially avoidable. Accidents, including choking to death, figured prominently here. There was little tobacco and diet-related death among individuals with more severe disabilities (IQ under 50) when compared with the gen- eral population. Certain health problems which are more com- mon among people with learning disability were particularly associated with high mortality, namely major congenital mal- formations. Recent trends toward the widespread application of corrective surgery have contributed to reductions in this type of mortality. More recently, meta-analysis has been applied to the study of mortality among people with learning disability. In a major review, Harris and Baraclough 10 found that learning disability was associated with a death risk 2.8 times higher than expected, and the mortality risk from unnatural causes was 3.1 times higher. Seventy-eight percent of deaths among the people with learning disability were from unnatural causes, while only 22% were from natural causes, the latter still carrying a 2.1 times higher risk. General health There has been long-standing interest in general health out- comes for children with learning disability. Indeed, early con- cepts of ‘subnormality’ (such as that by Down 11 ) emphasized the extent to which eventual poor health was inextricably linked to the nature of learning disability (reviewed in O’Brien 12 ). Where researchers have focused on general health and morbidity, one widely adopted approach has been to begin by identifying the major common health problems in Annotation *UK usage. US usage: mental retardation.

Adult outcome of childhood learning disability

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

634 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2001, 43: 634–638

Adult outcome of childhood learningdisability

Greg O’Brien FRCPsych FRCPCH MD

Professor of Developmental Psychiatry, University of

Northumbria.

Correspondence to author at Northgate Hospital, Morpeth,

Northumberland, NE61 3BP, UK.

‘But what are the prospects for the future, Doctor?’ This ques-

tion is frequently asked by parents of children with learning

disability.* But longitudinal studies of the outcome in adult-

hood of childhood learning disability are lacking.1 At the same

time, extrapolating prognoses from the available evidence is

unreliable. Increasingly, however, clinicians are faced with this

basic question that is on the minds of parents who have

gleaned ‘information’ from other sources, notably the inter-

net. So, what can be usefully and reliably claimed regarding

outcome in adulthood for children with learning disabilities?

Outcome studies in learning disabilityStudies of the outcome of childhood learning disability have

adopted a variety of strategies. Various outcome measures

have been used including: (1) life expectancy, (2) general

health, (3) adaptive functioning, (4) psychopathology, (5)

service contact, and (6) integration into adult life. Early stud-

ies focused mainly on morbidity and mortality (e.g. Decker

and colleagues,2 Richards and Sylvester3), while more recent-

ly the emphasis has shifted towards systematic consideration

of quality of life .15, 18

Life expectancyThere is a long tradition of research on the mortality of people

with learning disability. A variety of studies (e.g. Richards and

Sylvester,3 Balkrishnan and Wolff 4) have demonstrated that, in

general, children with learning disability have a shorter life

expectancy than other children. Previously, most of these stud-

ies were carried out on long-stay hospital patients employing

case note data, but the same outcome has been found in the

recent community-based study by Hollins and colleagues,5 that

examined death certification data of individuals with learning

disability. These studies have clarified that mortality increases

with increasing severity of learning disability, most notably

among those with IQ levels below 40.5–7 Other high risk factors

which predict or are associated with earlier death in people

with learning disability include any additional physical disabili-

ty or medical problem, notably cerebral palsy and poorly con-

trolled epilepsy. Non-mobile children and those incapable of

self-feeding are especially at risk of short life expectancy.6

Another strategy has been to concentrate on the cause of

death among people with learning disability. In a major

50-year study Carter and Jancar8 found that before 1955, tuber-

culosis was a common cause of death among people with

learning disability while other respiratory infections were

implicated more commonly in later deaths. Between 1930 and

1980, death rates by arterial disease increased while death from

status epilepticus decreased. They also found that from 1959 to

1968, cancer became a more common cause of death among

people with learning disability. These three latter trends mir-

ror changes in lifestyle and improvements in medical treat-

ment of people with learning disability over the last 50 years.

Subsequently, Dupont and coworkers9 applied the concept of

‘avoidable mortality’ to learning disability. A cause-specific

mortality calculation was devised with the aim of demonstrat-

ing the extent to which mortality among people with learning

disability might be potentially avoidable. Accidents, including

choking to death, figured prominently here. There was little

tobacco and diet-related death among individuals with more

severe disabilities (IQ under 50) when compared with the gen-

eral population. Certain health problems which are more com-

mon among people with learning disability were particularly

associated with high mortality, namely major congenital mal-

formations. Recent trends toward the widespread application

of corrective surgery have contributed to reductions in this

type of mortality. More recently, meta-analysis has been

applied to the study of mortality among people with learning

disability. In a major review, Harris and Baraclough10 found

that learning disability was associated with a death risk 2.8

times higher than expected, and the mortality risk from

unnatural causes was 3.1 times higher. Seventy-eight percent

of deaths among the people with learning disability were

from unnatural causes, while only 22% were from natural

causes, the latter still carrying a 2.1 times higher risk.

General healthThere has been long-standing interest in general health out-

comes for children with learning disability. Indeed, early con-

cepts of ‘subnormality’ (such as that by Down11) emphasized

the extent to which eventual poor health was inextricably

linked to the nature of learning disability (reviewed in

O’Brien12). Where researchers have focused on general health

and morbidity, one widely adopted approach has been to

begin by identifying the major common health problems in

Annotation

*UK usage. US usage: mental retardation.

this population, and to proceed to closer consideration of

unmet health needs (e.g. the study by Decker and col-

leagues2). Studies of general health and morbidity in learn-

ing disability commonly employ disability registers, direct

assessment, or both. Disability registers offer the advantage of

a locally defined database including an account of the pres-

ence and severity of a selected set of common health vari-

ables.13 However, these registers are not necessarily regularly

updated and there is no common dataset between different

registers.14 The other general strategy, therefore, consists of

direct assessment of individuals according to a set of pre-

established criteria incorporating a standardized assessment

of specific health problems. One theme which has as yet

eluded researchers, however, is the development of a ‘posi-

tive health and well-being’15 measure for children with learn-

ing disability, because of the difficulties inherent in defining

this in the index population. Most initiatives, therefore,

remain problem based, concentrating on the measurement

of certain high profile and important general health outcome

variables for people with learning disability, for example,

those associated with epilepsy.15 The latter initiative now

offers the possibility of studying the quality of life of people

with learning disability who have major health problems,

where the opportunity for the individuals to make personal

choices and control their own circumstances is regarded as

central for general health outcome. Also, in consideration of

general health outcomes, a variety of important interactional

effects are apparent.16 For example, the child’s own percep-

tion of a physical illness, such as epilepsy, can play a crucial

role in outcome,17 as does family and parental adjustment to

the stress of disability.18–20

Adaptive functioningIn many instances, the first and the most fundamental question

is about which factors most powerfully influence subsequent

adaptive functioning in children with learning disability. This

issue can be further broken down into several subsidiary ones.

First, is there good evidence that the severity of learning dis-

ability in childhood (in terms of lower IQ) is of itself predictive

of subsequent social adaptive functioning? Reviewing this mat-

ter, Clarke and Clarke21 concluded that early severe learning

disability very powerfully predicts long-term dependency and

disability, much more than mild disability. In addition, the stud-

ies they reviewed indicated the importance of other child fac-

tors as predictors of subsequent adaptive functioning. Notably,

it was found that early conduct disorder was an important

adverse factor. A more recent review22 agreed that child vari-

ables are an important consideration of subsequent adaptive

functioning, but emphasized that a host of environmental vari-

ables are also crucially important. Studies of the social adapta-

tion of children with learning disability are potentially difficult

to design, given this combination of multiple intrinsic and envi-

ronmental influences. Many recent studies have therefore

focused on certain important high profile subpopulations of

children with learning disability, and on other informative sam-

ple groups. Galler and coworkers23 carried out a 15-year fol-

low-up of children who had experienced malnutrition in the

first year of life, to the extent that it resulted in measurable

intellectual disability in early life. Their follow-up study found

that early intellectual disability persisted into adolescence.

Similarly, Huttonlocher and Hapke24 followed up 145 patients

with intractable epilepsy, finding an association between early

learning disability and later adaptive functioning. In the same

vein, Steinhausen and colleagues25 performed a long-term

child-to-adult follow-up of 158 infants with foetal alcohol syn-

drome, finding that early intellectual disability had a powerful

effect on later intellectual outcome. The consensus which

emerges from these studies is that the severity of childhood

learning disability may be of itself a most powerful predictor of

later subsequent adaptive functioning in all subpopulations

with learning disability.

Faced with such a prospect, some reviewers have concen-

trated on how best to maximize social adaptive functioning of

children with learning disability. One long-standing debate

concerns patterns of parenting. Marfo26 noted that while it had

been widely believed that certain patterns of parenting such as

‘maternal directiveness’ (i.e. a proactive, directive style of par-

enting) were likely to have an adverse effect upon develop-

ment, this was in fact not based on empirical evidence. Another

possibility was that any such maternal directiveness is better

seen as an indicator of the severity of early intellectual and

behavioural challenges. In other words, a directive style of par-

enting may be a functional, adaptive response to behavioural

problems in a child, rather than the cause of the child’s difficul-

ties. However, it is generally recognized that maximal social

adaptive functional outcome for children affected by learning

disability is attained through careful, programmed interven-

tion. An illustrative example of this can be found in the work of

Hatton and colleagues27 which has demonstrated that contem-

porary specialized community-based care models do seem to

generate optimal global outcome.

PsychopathologyStudies of the outcome of learning disability in childhood have

been informative regarding the occurrence and nature of later

psychopathology. Bartak and Rutter28 in a comparative study of

children with autism, with or without learning disability, indi-

cated that severity of learning disability has a powerful effect on

both the nature and the eventual outcome of autism. Indeed,

reviewers of different outcome studies have long noted that

this is so, despite rigorous educational and therapeutic inter-

ventions (such as the work of Nakane29). Similarly, Havelkova

and coworkers30 performed a 15-year follow-up study of 20

‘preschool schizophrenic children’ (which in today’s language

corresponds to children with autism), demonstrating that chil-

dren’s preschool IQ was the most powerful predictor of later

psychiatric disturbance. Some caution is required in the inter-

pretation of such studies, as the IQ test result may well have

been affected by the presence of autism. Nevertheless, the pre-

dictive power of IQ in the context of autism is of interest itself.

This was corroborated by von-Knorring and Hagglof31 who

carried out a 9-year follow-up of individuals with autism,

including those with learning disabilities, concluding that the

symptoms of autism were remarkably constant over time.

One of the most convincing studies of the psychopatholog-

ical and behavioural outcome of child learning disability is

that of de Chateau.32 This was a 30-year follow-up of adminis-

trative records, which found that early (in 0- to 3-year-old

infants) difficulties in adaptive functioning predicted later

behavioural problems. Overall though, other factors were

identified as important determinants of outcome, particularly

sex of child and psychopathology in parents. These results are

in line with the major Cambridge Delinquency Study, which

found that low intelligence, poor academic attainment, and

Annotation 635

impulsive or antisocial behaviour in early childhood are

among the best predictors of subsequent delinquency, along

with non-child factors such as family criminality, poverty, and

adverse parenting.33

Service contactIt is to be expected that the nature and severity of learning dis-

ability will exert a substantial influence on service contact and

utility. Other factors, not the least of which is service availability,

636 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2001, 43: 634–638

Table I: Outcome studies in learning disability

Type of study Nr of IQ in Outcome measures including: work; Study Author(s)participants childhood marriage; offending; residence; location

mental health; physical morbidity and mortality; developmental attainments

25-year postal follow-up,a 646 60–85 More able participants were in work USA Fernald45

after hospital discharge 10% of women and few men were married

Recorded convictions in 42% women, 10% men

Rehospitalization in 35% women, 24% men

4-year case-note study.b 50 Average 20% were in work USA Bronner46

Follow-up of attendees 68 80% had reoffended

at one child guidance clinic 20% were living independently

for delinquents

17-year case recordc 122 60–85 78% were in ‘totally-self-supporting’ USA Fairbank47

adult, follow-up of child ‘More multiple marriages’

learning disability ‘Excess’ of offending

Prospective blindd 84 50–90 Half were successfully employed in dressmaking USA Abel and

follow-up of young adults Kinder48

with learning disability

selected and prepared for

work experience

30-year, official recorde 626 70–84 Low IQ group: typically in unskilled jobs Sweden Ramer49

follow-up of children with and Lower marriage rate; higher divorce rate

learning disability born 589 Higher rate of institutionalization

1905–1917, and normal No cross-group differences in offending

control children

6- and 9-year, communityf 432 70–85 Work record ‘good’ Scotland Ferguson and

case note and interview ‘High rate’ of offending Kerr50, 51

follow-up of special school (Those who had been hospitalized were

leavers excluded)

Semi-structured psychiatric g 520 40–50 27% in employment USA Saenger52

interview of adults who had None married; 4% had sexual experience

been classed ‘trainable retard’ Offending ‘extremely uncommon’

as children 39% whole life rate of hospitalization; 25% current

Psychiatric status: 20% ‘neurotic’; 6% ‘psychotic’

75% had physical disability or infirmity

Controlled prospectiveh (1) 256 45–75 Compared with 129 normal IQ-matched control USA Kennedy 53, 54

follow-up of adult social individuals for other indices, including social

functioning adversity; index group had a ‘less satisfactory’

work record and higher offending rate, but no

difference in marital history.

(2) 179 Mean Compared with control participants, no difference

18 y later 99.5 in work, less marked higher offending rate,

same marital history

Controlled, prospectivei, j, k 1933: <70 1936: 27% in work; low marriage rate; 3–7 times USA Baller55

30-year case note and 206 increased rate of offending Charles56

interview follow-up of 1953: 36% in work; high divorce rate; 40% Baller, Charles

social functioning in 1964: offender rate and Miller57

adulthood, after childhood 100 1964: 67% in work; high divorce rate; 1/3 dead

educational failure by age 56 y (age-expected mortality 1/6)

Adult follow-up of childrenl 221 Mostly 89% in full-time employment Scotland Richardson

born 1952–1954, 22 y <70; 2/3 not in special (learning disability) and Koller58

‘administratively classified’ some service contact

as having learning disability <80

a1919; b1933; c1933; d1942; e1946; f1955, 1958; g1957, 1960; h1948, 1966; i1936; j1953; k1966; l1996.

will also figure. One influential study34 emphasized that expec-

tations, and particularly unduly over optimistic expectations

concerning the potential outcome of severe disability in young

people on the part of service planners, often play a crucial role

in service outcome. Most authorities would agree that factors

of family functioning in the area of coping and problem solving

are also important in determining future and ongoing service

contact and utility for children with learning disability. This has

also been demonstrated in experimental studies (e.g. Black

and colleagues35). Similarly, Schalock’s36 5-year follow-up of a

cohort of individuals with varying degrees of intellectual dis-

ability (n=108), found that both disability-related and environ-

mental-associated factors determined outcome in terms of

service utility, emphasizing the importance of degree of motiva-

tion and participation on the part of individuals’ families.

Considering the nature of service provision and the manner

of its delivery, there have been numerous studies of how differ-

ent types of services, such as patterns of day care, result in dif-

ferent patterns of contact/utility on the part of individuals

with learning disability. One interesting example was that of

Jacobson and colleagues37 who included certain employment

characteristics and work satisfaction in their study. Their results

suggested that both ‘engagement’ in work and the nature of

the work are powerful factors in determining work satisfaction

in individuals with learning disability. Other studies have con-

sidered how aspects of service management and the organiza-

tion of individuals’ care plans can affect service utility and

contact. Here, Schalock38 described a resource allocation

model of care planning which aimed to maximize outcome in

terms of service contact. A subsequent survey by O’Neill39 indi-

cated that planned, coordinated care does indeed seem to lead

to better service utility, considering postinstitutional outcome

in terms of daily living and day care. This finding was corrobo-

rated by Criscione and coworkers,40 whose 1-year follow-up

study found that programmed case management affected out-

come in terms of readmission to hospital.

It has long been recognized that the service careers of chil-

dren affected by any major disability, such as learning disability,

will inevitably be determined by a combination of interactive

factors. Reviewing this issue, Eisen41 suggested that a vulnera-

bility/stressor/resilience model might be useful. Here, severity

of disability and concurrent health problems might be seen as

vulnerabilities, life events, and changing circumstances and

(lack of) services might function as stressors, while markers of

resilience are most likely to be discerned in certain styles and

patterns of parenting and family functioning. Another range of

influences on outcome are those in the area of expectations: a

recent study by Wehmeyer42 highlighted how, for example,

locus of control, notably excessive external locus of control

(and a parallel conviction of being powerless over their own sit-

uation), can have the most powerful, detrimental effect on out-

come for people with learning disability.

Adjustment to adult lifeJust as the most commonly asked question concerns the

prospects for later development of children with learning

disability, the most fervently held hope is that the child will

enjoy a satisfying and fulfilling adult life, and in whatever

ways possible, a successful one. On these themes, once

again, the available follow-up studies indicate that many indi-

viduals will indeed enjoy futures which live up to these

expectations. But the bad news, once again, is inescapable;

as the various follow-up studies of such key indicators of adult

adjustment as employment, relationships, criminality, and

adult mental health do not give cause for optimism. Some of

the main follow-up studies are summarized in Table I. It is

notable that: (1) in respect of most outcome indices of general

integration into adult life, the child with more severe disabili-

ties is at greater risk; (2) the findings of early studies have been

substantially corroborated by more recent work; and (3) the

outcomes for children with mild learning disability have

improved in more recent years. It is this latter observation

which has been pivotal. Many workers derive much optimism

from Tizard’s43 appraisal of the situation of people with learn-

ing disability over the closing decades of the 20th century. He

perceived that the outcome and situation of people with mild

learning disability had improved so much, that it must only be a

matter of time before the same improvements would apply to

the situation of children with more severe learning disability.

Sadly, there are no signs of the emergence of such a fundamen-

tal change. On the contrary, all available evidence points

towards corroboration of Penrose’s key observation44 that the

more severe the child’s learning disability, the less promising

are the prospects.

In summary, while seeking to predict definitive adult out-

comes for an individual child with learning disability may be

folly, certain key issues are helpful. First, the severity of child-

hood learning disability in terms of IQ exerts a major influence

on subsequent development and adjustment. Secondly, the

presence of any major physical disability has a substantial bear-

ing on outcome. Furthermore, self-perception, parental and

family factors, and wider societal influences are important

mediators of these key biological variables. In clinical practice,

therefore, the task is to identify opportunities for intervention

in these various domains in order to promote maximal out-

comes for children with learning disability.

Accepted for publication 20th December 2000.

References1. Aicardi J. (1998) Children’s Diseases of the Central Nervous

System. 2nd edn. London: Mac Keith Press.2. Decker HA, Herbug EN, Haythornthwaite MS, Rupke LK, Smith

DC. (1968) Provision of health care for institutionalised retardedchildren. American Journal of Mental Deficiency 73: 283–93.

3. Richards BW, Sylvester PE. (1969) Mortality trends in mentaldeficiency institutions. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research13: 276–92.

4. Balkrishnan TR, Wolff L. (1976) Life expectancy of mentallyretarded persons in Canadian institutions. American Journal ofMental Deficiency 80: 650–62.

5. Hollins S, Attard MT, von Fraunhofer N, McGuigan S, Sedgwick P.(1998) Mortality in people with mental retardation: risks, causes,and death certification findings in London. DevelopmentalMedicine & Child Neurology 40: 50–6.

6. Eyman RK, Grossman HJ, Tarjan G, Miller CR. (1987) Lifeexpectancy and mental retardation – a longitudinal study in astate residential facility. Monographs of the American Associationof Mental Deficiency 7: 1–73.

7. Molsa PK. (1994) Survival in mental retardation. MentalHandicap Research 7: 338–45.

8. Carter G, Jancar J. (1983) Mortality in the mentally handicapped –a 50-year survey at the Stoke Park group of hospitals(1930–1980). Journal of Mental Deficiency Research 27: 143–56.

9. Dupont A, Vaeth M, Videbeck P. (1987) Mortality, life expectancy,and causes of death of the mildly mentally retarded in Denmark.Journal of Medical Science. 44 (Suppl.): 76–82.

10. Harris EC, Barraclough B. (1998) Excess mortality of mentaldisorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 173: 11–53.

Annotation 637

11. Down JL. (1866) Observations on ethnic classification of idiots.Clinical Lectures and Reports, London Hospital 3: 259.

12. O’Brien G. (1992a) Behavioural phenotypy in developmentalpsychiatry – measuring behavioural phenotypes: a guide to theavailable schedules. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry1 (Suppl.): 1– 61.

13. Corbett J. (1977) Studies of Mental Retardation. In: Graham PJ,editor. Epidemiological Approaches in Child Psychiatry.London: Academic Press.

14. National Development Team. (1992) Back to the Future.Manchester, UK: National Development Team.

15. Espie CA, Kerr M, Paul A, O’Brien G, Betts T, Clark J, Jacoby A,Baker G. (1998) Mental retardation and epilepsy. II: A review ofavailable outcome measures and position statement ondevelopment priorities. Seizure 6: 337–50.

16. Jalava M, Sillampaa M, Camfield C, Camfield P. (1997) Socialadjustment and confidence thirty-five years after onset ofchildhood epilepsy: a prospective study. Epilepsia 38: 708–15.

17. Kemp S, Morley S, Anderson E. (1999) Coping with epilepsy: doillness representations play a role? British Journal of ClinicalPsychology 38: 43–58.

18. Medalie JH. (1997) Patient and family adjustment to conquerdisease at home. Disability and Rehabilitation 19: 163–70.

19. Stanton BR. (1999) Does family functioning affect outcome inchildren with neurological disorders? Paediatric Rehabilitation 3: 193–9.

20. Carlton-Ford S, Miller N, Nealeigh N, Sanchez N. (1997) Theeffects of perceived stigma and psychological over-control onthe behavioural problems of children with epilepsy. Seizure6: 383–91.

21. Clarke A, Clarke A. (1988) The adult outcome of earlybehavioural anomalies. International Journal of BehaviouralDevelopment 11: 3–19.

22. King EH, Logsdon DA, Shroeder SR. (1992) Risk factors fordevelopmental delay among infants and toddlers. Children’sHealth Care 21: 39–52.

23. Galler JR, Ramsey F, Forde V. (1986) A follow-up study of theinfluence of early malnutrition on subsequent development IV: Intellectual performance during adolescence. Nutrition andBehaviour 3: 211–22.

24. Huttonlocher PR, Hapke RJ. (1990) A follow-up study ofintractable seizures in childhood. Annals of Neurology 28: 99–705.

25. Steinhausen HC, Willms J, Spohr HL. (1993) Long termpsychopathological and cognitive outcome of children with fetalalcohol syndrome. Journal of the American Academy of Childand Adolescent Psychiatry 3: 990–4.

26. Marfo K. (1990) Maternal directiveness in interactions withmentally handicapped children: an analytical commentary.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 31: 531–49.

27. Hatton C, Emerson E, Robertson J, Henderson D, Cooper J.(1995) The quality and costs of residential services for adultswith multiple disabilities: a comparative evaluation. Research inDevelopmental Disabilities 16: 439–60.

28. Bartak L, Rutter M. (1976) Differences between mentallyretarded and normal intelligence autistic children. Journal ofAutism and Childhood Schizophrenia 6: 109–20.

29. Nakane A. (1983) Childhood autism and child and adolescentpsychiatric treatment. Japanese Journal of Child andAdolescent Psychiatry 24: 164–70.

30. Havelkova M, Rees S, Taylor A. (1976) Incidence of emotionaland thought disorders in parents of pre-school psychoticchildren. Acta Paedopsychiatrica 42: 109–20.

31. Von-Knorring AL, Hagglof B. (1993) Autism in NorthernSweden: a population-based follow-up study: psychopathology.European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2: 91–7.

32. de Chateau P. (1991) A 30-year prospective follow-up of 125infants in child guidance clinics in Stockholm. Infant MentalHealth Journal 12: 116–25.

33. Farrington D. (1994) The twelfth Jack Tizard Memorial Lecture.The development of offending and antisocial behaviour fromchildhood: key findings from the Cambridge Study inDelinquent Development. Journal of Child Psychology andPsychiatry 36: 929–64.

34. Hoddap RM, Zigler E. (1985) Placement decisions and theireffects on the development of individuals with severe mentalretardation. Mental Retardation 23: 125–30.

35. Black MH, Cohn JF, Smull MW, Crites LS. (1985) Individual andfamily factors associated with risk of institutionalisation ofmentally retarded adults. American Journal of Mental Deficiency90: 271–6.

36. Schalock RL. (1986a) Postsecondary community placement ofhandicapped residents: a five year follow-up. Mental RetardationQuarterly 9: 295–303.

37. Jacobson JW, Burchard SN, Ackerman LJ, Yoe JT. (1991) Assessingcommunity environmental effects upon people withdevelopmental disabilities: satisfaction as an outcome measure.Special Issue: environment and its significance to residentialservices. Adult Residential Care Journal 5: 147–64.

38. Schalock RL. (1986b) Resource allocation approach fordetermining clients’ need status. Mental Retardation 24: 27–35.

39. O’Neill J. (1990) Medicaid versus state funding of communityresidences: impact on daily life of people with mental retardation.Mental Retardation 28: 183–8.

40. Criscione T, Kastner TA, Walsh KK, Nathanson RJN. (1993)Managed health care services for people with mental retardation:impact on in-patient utilisation. Mental Retardation 31: 297–306.

41. Eisen P. (1979) Children under stress. Australian and NewZealand Journal of Psychiatry 13: 193–207.

42. Wehmeyer ML. (1994) Perceptions of self-determination andpsychological empowerment of adolescents with mentalretardation. Education and Training in Mental Retardation andDevelopmental Disabilities 29: 9–21.

43. Tizard J. (1974) Longitudinal studies: problems and findings. In:Clarke A, Clarke A, editors. Mental Deficiency: The ChangingOutlook. 3rd edn. London: Methuen. p 114–47.

44. Penrose LS. (1972) The Biology of Mental Defect. London:Sidgwick and Jackson.

45. Fernald WE. (1919) After-care study of the patients dischargedfrom Waverley for a period of twenty-five years. Ungraded 5: 25–31.

46. Bronner AF. (1933) Follow-up studies of mental defectives.Proceedings of the American Association of Mental Deficiency38: 258–67.

47. Fairbank RF. (1933) The subnormal child – 17 years after. MentalHygiene 17: 177–208.

48. Abel TM, Kinder EF. (1942) The Subnormal Adolescent Girl. NewYork: Columbia University Press.

49. Ramer T. (1946) The prognosis of mentally retarded children. ActaPsychiatrica, Neurology Suppl. 41: 1–142.

50. Ferguson T, Kerr AW. (1955) After-histories of girls educated inspecial schools for mentally handicapped children. GlasgowMedical Journal 36: 50–6.

51. Ferguson T, Kerr AW. (1958) After-histories of boys educated inspecial schools for mentally handicapped children. ScottishMedical Journal 3: 31–8.

52. Saenger G. (1957) The Adjustment of Severely Retarded Adults inthe Community. Albany, NY: New York State InterdepartmentalHealth Resources Board.

53. Kennedy RJR. (1948) The Social Adjustment of Morons in aConnecticut City. Hartford, CT: Mansfield-Southbury TrainingSchools, Social Service Department, State Office.

54. Kennedy RJR. (1966) A Connecticut Community Revisited: a studyof the social adjustment of a group of mentally deficient adults in1948 and 1960. Hartford, CT: Connecticut State Department ofHealth, Office of Mental Retardation.

55. Baller WR. (1936) A study of the present social status of a group ofadults who, when they were in elementary school, were classed asmentally deficient. Genetic Psychology Monographs 18: 165–244.

56. Charles DC. (1953) Ability and accomplishment of persons earlierjudged mentally deficient. Genetic Psychology Monographs47: 3–7.

57. Baller WR, Charles DC, Miller EL. (1966) Mid-life Attainment ofthe Mentally Retarded. A Longitudinal Study. Lincoln, NE:University of Nebraska.

58. Richardson SA, Koller H. (1996) Twenty-two years: causes andconsequences of mental handicaps. Cambridge, MA: HarvardUniversity Press.

638 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2001, 43: 634–638