45
A problem unstuck? Evaluating the effectiveness of sticker prompts for encouraging household food waste recycling behaviour. Linzi Shearer a b * , Birgitta Gatersleben c , Stephen Morse a , Matthew Smyth b , Sally Hunt b a Centre for Environmental Strategy, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey, GU2 7XH, United Kingdom b Waste Development, Surrey County Council, Kingston-Upon-Thames, KT1 2DW, United Kingdom c Department of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey, ,GU2 7XH, United Kingdom Abstract This Randomised Control Trial (RCT) investigated the effectiveness of using stickers as a visual prompt to encourage the separate collection of household food waste for recycling in two local authorities in South East England. During a baseline period of up to 15 weeks, separately collected food waste was weighed (in tonnes) and averaged across households in both treatment (N = 33,716 households within 29 defined areas) and control groups (N = 30,568 households within 26 areas). A sticker prompt was then affixed to the lids of refuse bins in the treatment group area only. Weights for both groups were subsequently measured across a 16-week experimental period. Results showed that, in the control group, there was no change in the average weight of food waste captured for recycling between the baseline and experimental period. However, there was a significant increase (20.74%) in the treatment group, and this change in behaviour persisted in the longer term. Sticker prompts therefore appear to have a significant and sustained impact on food waste 1

Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

A problem unstuck? Evaluating the effectiveness of sticker prompts for encouraging household food waste recycling behaviour.

Linzi Shearer a b *, Birgitta Gatersleben c, Stephen Morse a, Matthew Smyth b, Sally Hunt b

a Centre for Environmental Strategy, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey, GU2 7XH, United Kingdomb Waste Development, Surrey County Council, Kingston-Upon-Thames, KT1 2DW, United Kingdomc Department of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey, ,GU2 7XH, United Kingdom

Abstract

This Randomised Control Trial (RCT) investigated the effectiveness of using stickers as a visual prompt

to encourage the separate collection of household food waste for recycling in two local authorities in

South East England. During a baseline period of up to 15 weeks, separately collected food waste was

weighed (in tonnes) and averaged across households in both treatment (N = 33,716 households

within 29 defined areas) and control groups (N = 30,568 households within 26 areas). A sticker

prompt was then affixed to the lids of refuse bins in the treatment group area only. Weights for both

groups were subsequently measured across a 16-week experimental period. Results showed that, in

the control group, there was no change in the average weight of food waste captured for recycling

between the baseline and experimental period. However, there was a significant increase (20.74%) in

the treatment group, and this change in behaviour persisted in the longer term. Sticker prompts

therefore appear to have a significant and sustained impact on food waste recycling rates, while being

simple, practically feasible and inexpensive (£0.35 per household) for local authorities to implement

at scale.

Key words:

Visual prompts, household food waste, recycling behaviour, nudge, behaviour change.

* Corresponding author

Email addresses: [email protected], [email protected], [email protected],

[email protected], [email protected]

1

Page 2: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

1. Introduction

Changing patterns of human production and consumption in industrialised nations have resulted in

increased levels of household food waste (Parfitt et al., 2010). In the United Kingdom (UK),

households are responsible for generating around half of the 15 million tonnes of food and drink

waste that is produced each year (House of Commons, 2015; WRAP, 2011). The EU Landfill Directive

(1999/31/EC) specifies that Member States must reduce levels of biodegradable waste sent to landfill

to 35% of 1995 levels by 2020, but does not prescribe the treatment options, collection systems, or

other policies that should be introduced to meet these targets (Defra, 2011).

In the UK, local authorities are responsible for the collection and disposal of biodegradable waste,

which has traditionally been sent for disposal to landfill sites (House of Commons, 2015). Methane, a

greenhouse gas far more potent than carbon dioxide (CO2), is released when biodegradable waste

(which includes household food waste) decomposes anaerobically at landfill sites (Graham-Rowe et

al., 2015). The UK Waste and Resources Action Programme (WRAP) (WRAP, 2011) estimate the

annual environmental impact of manufacturing, distributing, storing, using and disposing of edible

food and drink in the UK to be around 17 million tonnes of CO2 equivalent.

If collected separately from residual waste (refuse), food waste can be used as a feedstock for

anaerobic digestion (AD), a ‘recycling’ process that produces methane-rich ‘biogas’ that can be used

to generate renewable energy, and ‘digestate’ which can be used to produce agricultural bio-fertiliser

(Zhang et al., 2007) and has a lower disposal cost (approximately 50%) than landfill (Nomura et al.,

2011). Households are issued with a small food waste bin (known as a ‘caddy’) for use inside the

house and a larger caddy that is stored outside. The purpose of introducing the service is to

encourage households to separate their food waste from their refuse and store it in their indoor

caddy before transferring it to the outdoor caddy in advance of their weekly collection day. By

diverting food waste in this way, local authorities can increase their overall recycling rate, while saving

money and improving their environmental performance.

2

Page 3: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

When the first local authorities in the UK introduced separate food waste collection services in 2006,

just 1% of household food waste was being collected separately (Defra, 2015). By 2013/14, more than

half of the local authorities responsible for waste collection in England had introduced some form of

food waste collection scheme (House of Commons, 2015). As a result, the total amount of separately

collected food waste in England increased from 118,000 to 290,000 tonnes between 2010 and 2014

(Waste Data Flow, 2016). Despite this dramatic improvement, almost a third of the refuse waste

stream in England is still composed of food waste (WRAP, 2011). This suggests that many households

are still not participating in the scheme, or those that are taking part are not using their caddies as

effectively as they potentially could be. Since the successful management of any household recycling

scheme is dependent on the effective participation of a sufficient number of households, local

authorities must introduce policy interventions designed to encourage public participation (Karim

Ghani et al., 2013).

To encourage food waste separation behaviour, local authorities have a number of policy

interventions at their disposal (Dahlén & Lagerkvist, 2010; Steg & Vlek, 2009). The decision about

which policy interventions should be introduced is not a simple one for decision-makers as they must

be clearly effective, in the sense of producing changes in behaviour that are sustained in the longer

term (Steg & Vlek, 2009). An intervention must also be practically feasible to introduce and there

must be a sufficient means of accurately monitoring the impact of the intervention to understand

whether it was successful or not (Steg & Vlek, 2009). They must also be cost efficient to implement as

local authorities in England are currently under increasing pressure to deliver ‘more with less’,

following a 40% reduction in funding from central government (LGA, 2014). Hence there is an obvious

attraction for the use of relatively simple and cost-effective approaches.

An emerging body of literature has advocated the use of ‘nudge interventions’ as alternatives to more

traditional forms of behavioural intervention (Dolan et al., 2010; Thaler & Sunstein, 2008). Nudge

approaches have been hailed to be a potentially powerful, low-cost set of tools for policy makers who

are faced with addressing the challenge of weighing environmental sustainability against fiscal

3

Page 4: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

responsibility when making decisions, particularly during times of austerity (Dolan et al., 2010; John,

2013; Kallbekken & Sælen, 2013).

Nudge is a valuable theoretical framework that summarises ideas drawn from the field of behavioural

economics, which itself draws insights from the field of psychology (Kirakozian, 2016). Proponents

argue that traditional policy tools have ignored the fact that much human behaviour is automatic in

nature (Michie, 2015), recognising that behaviours most often occur as a consequence of both

automatic and reflective processes running in parallel. Nudge interventions, therefore, mainly target

the automatic system and seek to change the ‘choice architecture’ of individuals to encourage

changes to attitudes and behaviour (Sunstein, 2015). The approach assumes that people will rely on

past ways of thinking and acting unless they are encouraged to act or think differently. The options for

changing behaviour centre on providing reminders and cues that both recognise where the individual

currently is while also placing them in a choice environment.

Good designers of nudge policy interventions can steer individuals down new decision pathways

without them necessarily noticing that it is happening. Behavioural change is achieved by altering how

individuals view the attractiveness of an alternative course of action by improving the messages they

receive or the opportunities they have. While no “precise, operational definition of nudging”

(Martaeu et al., 2011: 263) currently exists, a taxonomy of interventions published in a recent House

of Lords report (House of Lords, 2015) described nudges as being any one of the following: changes to

the physical environment; information provision; changes to the default policy and the use of social

norms and salience.

One form of nudge intervention, ‘visual prompts’ has had a wide applicability within a variety of

behavioural fields. Visual prompts are a form of informational intervention designed to stimulate

action or serve as a reminder to engage in a behaviour that might otherwise be forgotten (Chui,

2015). Visual prompts usually take the form of posters, signs, stickers or flyers (Bartram, 2009), and

display factual or persuasive information, or provide cues to aid behavioural decision-making (Geller,

1982; Sussman & Gifford, 2012). Their intensity can vary from simple notices that raise awareness or

4

Page 5: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

provide procedural information, to more comprehensive statements that provide context and

rationale (Tucker, 2001).

Several studies have demonstrated the effectiveness of visual prompts as a means for encouraging

transportation (e.g., Cope et al., 1991; Huybers et al., 2004) and health behaviours (e.g., Amass et al.,

1993; Andersen et al., 2012). They have also been used to encourage certain pro-environmental

behaviours, for example: litter reduction in public places (Baltes & Hayward, 1976; Geller et al., 1976);

increasing workplace recycling (Austin et al., 1993); and reducing household energy consumption

(Sussman & Gifford, 2012; Winett, 1978).

The effects of prompts on general household recycling behaviour specifically have also been widely

documented, but results are not consistent. Some research has shown that single prompts in isolation

can be an effective way of increasing recycling (e.g., Arbuthnot et al., 1976; Burn, 1991; Jacobs &

Bailey, 1982; Oskamp, 1995; Spaccarelli et al., 1990), while other studies suggest that prompts are

less effective than other types of intervention (Goldenhar & Connell, 1992; Schultz, 1999; Werner et

al., 1998; Witmer & Geller, 1976). A recent meta-analysis found that prompts were one of the most

effective intervention types for encouraging pro-environmental behaviour (Osbaldiston & Schott,

2011). However, as 78% of the studies included in the analysis tested interventions in combination, it

was not possible for the authors to make definitive conclusions about which interventions were most

effective in isolation. It is therefore possible that prompts are effective but only when delivered in

combination with other types of intervention.

Other research has explored the elements of design that can improve the effectiveness of visual

prompts. Several authors have indicated that ensuring the final product is noticeable, simple and clear

is important (Sussman et al., 2012). Adding pictures to written information may also improve

effectiveness (Roberts et al., 2009), provided the images used are congruent with the text (Jae et al.,

2008) and they do not ‘cloud’ the message (van Meurs & Aristoff, 2009). Some studies have shown

that certain attempts to persuade using visual prompts can cause individuals to protest and engage in

undesirable behaviours (Sussman & Gifford, 2012). This phenomenon, which threatens the perceived

5

Page 6: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

freedom of individuals, is known as psychological reactance (Brehm, 1966; Dillard & Shen, 2005) and

can be reduced by constructing messages using positive and polite language (Aronson & O’Leary,

1983; Reiter & Samuel, 1980). Finally, prompts work most effectively for those behaviours that are

‘simple, easy, effortless and repetitive’ to perform (Frederiks et al., 2015: 1391), and on those

individuals who already feel motivated to engage in the target behaviour (Schultz, 2013).

The persuasive impact of a visual prompt will not only depend on the message and its design, but also

on the recipient’s capacity to attend to and cognitively process the information (Borgstede &

Andersson, 2010). The same authors also suggest that the most important factor for any behavioural

intervention is attracting the attention of the target audience. Most previous studies on prompting

used leaflets or posters as the medium of delivery, yet their effectiveness has been called into

question (cf. Read, 1999). The more permanent nature of a bin sticker may allow for repeated

exposure to the prompt message, thus providing more opportunity than a leaflet for individuals to

notice and cognitively process the message.

Further, it is noteworthy that most experimental research on visual prompts and recycling behaviour

was conducted between 1980 and 2000, at a time when recycling had not yet been established as a

societal norm (Bedford et al., 2010; Thomas & Sharp, 2013). Since this period, there has been a

substantial societal shift in attitudes towards environmental issues in general (Thomas & Sharp, 2013)

and hence further research into the effects of prompting may be warranted. Little is also known about

the longer-term impact and cost effectiveness of behavioural interventions (Abrahamse & Steg, 2013).

With some exceptions (cf. Burn, 1991; Spaccarelli et al., 1990), most existing research on prompting

and recycling behaviour had short-term monitoring periods or did not include longer-term follow-ups

(Schultz et al., 1995). Finally, since most research on behaviour change interventions is academic in

nature, little is known about the actual costs of implementing such strategies in the real world

(Schultz, 2013). Academic studies tend to be small in scale and staff-intensive, often using research

assistants to carefully control implementation. However, when these techniques are implemented at

scale, costs usually increase. It is therefore important for researchers to provide detailed cost

information in addition to reporting results.

6

Page 7: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

Despite the mixed findings noted above, it seems logical to assume that visual prompts, if properly

designed and implemented, could provide local authorities with one practically feasible and

inexpensive strategy for targeting a large number of households. However, the evidence about their

effectiveness on general recycling behaviour is mixed and, to our knowledge, no academic studies

have investigated their effectiveness for promoting food waste recycling behaviour.

A recent non peer-reviewed research study (WRAP, 2016) sought to assess the effectiveness of

various combinations of the following interventions: a year’s free supply of caddy liners, a leaflet

providing information about how to recycle food waste and a ‘visual prompt’ in the form of a bin

sticker designed to be stuck on the lid of refuse wheelie bins (WRAP, 2016). Working in partnership

with 11 local authorities, a total of 19 pilot studies were carried out between 2013 and 2015, 11 of

which specifically tested the interventions outlined above. The weights of food waste (in tonnes, t)

collected from pre-defined groups of households were monitored before and after the delivery of

each intervention combination. Results were quite variable. The one study that tested the sticker and

liner combination achieved a 13% increase in weight of food waste collected. The mean increase in

weight achieved by the sticker and leaflet combination was slightly higher at 14% (two studies, range

= 4% to 24%). The full ‘package’ of interventions achieved a mean increase of 32% (six studies, range =

18% to 62%). WRAP concluded that the effect on food waste collection rates was amplified when

refuse bin stickers were included. They recommended that local authorities should introduce the full

‘package’ of interventions to have the greatest impact on behaviour, at a cost of £1.12 to £2.10 per

household (WRAP, 2016).

While the addition of the sticker prompt to the other interventions appeared to have an amplifying

effect, it was never tested in isolation. WRAP did not incorporate basic experimental design principles

(e.g., randomisation, replication and control groups) into their study and methodologies for analysing

results also varied between authorities. While sample sizes were often large (up to 15,000 households

in some study areas), the outcome measurement was the weight of food waste collected from

defined groups of households known as ‘collection rounds’. For each pilot study there was an

7

Page 8: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

insufficient number of collection round weights to statistically analyse results. The collection round

areas chosen to receive the interventions were also not selected at random and no control groups

were included for comparison against a ‘do nothing’ scenario on the part of the local authority.

Further, results from one authority could not always be directly compared with those from another

authority, as there were subtle, yet discernable, differences in the way in which the tonnage data

were recorded and analysed. As a result, WRAP’s findings cannot be generalised to other populations,

as these issues did not allow for a statistically robust set of conclusions to be produced about the

relative effectiveness of each intervention combination.

Separate food waste collection services were first trialled in England between 2006 and 2008

(Bridgwater & Parfitt, 2009). It is therefore surprising that so few academic studies have examined the

effectiveness of policy interventions for promoting this behaviour. A review of the literature found

just three studies that have examined the impact of informational strategies on food waste

separation behaviour. Bernstad et al. (2013) examined the effect of delivering oral information via a

door-stepping campaign. By weighing the separately collected food waste pre and post intervention,

they found that the difference in average weights collected by Swedish households in the control and

treatment groups was not statistically significant. Using the same outcome measures, Bernstad (2014)

assessed the effectiveness of written information (a leaflet about how to recycle and why it is

important) on a different group of Swedish households and found that this type of written

information also did not significantly increase the weight of food waste collected. The third study, a

Randomised Control Trial (RCT) conducted in Manchester, England (Nomura et al., 2011), showed that

feedback comparing a household’s food waste recycling behaviour with other households in the same

street resulted in a statistically significant increase in participation of 2.8% compared with a control

group.

In light of the above, the aim of the present study was to investigate the real world effectiveness,

practical feasibility and cost-efficiency of using sticker prompts to encourage households to recycle

their food waste. Since the true test of any behaviour change intervention lies in its ability to elicit a

change in behaviour, the first objective of the field experiment was to test the effectiveness of the

8

Page 9: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

visual prompt (a bin sticker) amongst households in two local authorities in Surrey, England. It was

hypothesised that, compared with the baseline period, significantly higher weights of food waste

would be collected in the treatment group during the experimental period than would be collected in

the control group (H1). A secondary objective was to assess the extent to which the impact of the

intervention persisted in the longer term. It was hypothesised that significantly higher weights of food

waste would be collected in the treatment group during each post-intervention period (short,

medium and long term) than in the control group (H2). The final objective of the study was to conduct

a cost-benefit analysis to estimate the payback period for this intervention.

2. Materials and methods

2.1 Participants & setting

Surrey is a county located in the South East of England that operates across 642 square miles and has

a population of over 1.1 million people (ONS, 2011). In Surrey there are a total of 11 district and

borough councils, each acting as Waste Collection Authorities (WCAs), responsible for the collection

of refuse and recycling. Two of these WCAs participated in this study. WCA1 and WCA2 are

responsible for the collection of waste and recycling from 33,538 households and 30,746 households

respectively (Ntotal = 64,284 households).

With regards to key demographics, these areas are broadly representative of England for the most

part. For gender, WCA2 exactly mirrors the national picture, while the split in WCA1 is broadly

equivalent to the national average (England) (ONS, 2011). A greater percentage of the population in

both WCAs are classed as ‘economically active’ (74-75% vs 70% in England) (ONS, 2011). However, of

the population of citizens who are classed as ‘economically inactive’, a higher than average proportion

of the population in both WCAs is retired than there is at the national level (between 6 – 9 % points

higher). This indicates that the older generation are marginally overrepresented in these regions

(ONS, 2011). Indeed, this is reflected in the census age spread data that shows that, compared with

England as a whole, a lower proportion of the population in both WCAs are aged 18-44, while a higher

proportion are aged 45 or over (ONS, 2011). The key area where the WCAs diverge from the national

picture is with respect to education levels. In both areas there is a lower than average (22% in

9

Page 10: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

England) proportion of residents (16% in WCA1, 20% in WCA2) that have no qualifications. However,

WCA1 has a much higher level of residents that have a qualification at Level 4 or above (36%) than in

WCA2 (26%) or nationally (27%) (ONS, 2011).

Households in WCA1 and WCA2 were issued with food waste recycling caddies in 2009 and 2011,

respectively. However, the recycling rate for food waste in these areas was just 29% (WCA1) and 23%

(WCA2) in 2013/14 (SCC, 2016). Households are grouped together into smaller geographic areas

known as collection rounds, which are residential areas that are serviced by the same collection

vehicle on the same day each week. WCA1 has a total of 20 collection round areas as it operates four

vehicles across five working days, while WCA2 collects from 35 collection rounds as it operates seven

vehicles each weekday. Following collection, the food waste is then taken to a waste transfer station

(WTF) for weighing, before being transported to the AD facility. As the unit of statistical analysis was

the waste collection round rather than the individual household, no background demographic data on

the sample of participants could be obtained. This is due to collection round areas not sharing the

same geographical boundaries that are used for the Census.

2.2 Design

The field study sought to establish whether a visual prompt (the independent variable) would

successfully increase the weight of food waste collected separately for recycling (the dependent

variable). A randomised pre-test/post-test control group design was employed, and the unit of

randomisation was the waste collection round. Table 1 outlines the total number of rounds and

households within each group. The 55 waste collection rounds were randomly assigned to either the

treatment (N=29) or control (N = 26) group using the random number function in Microsoft Excel.

Table 1.

Round information.Groupa Rounds HouseholdsControl 26 30,568Treatment 29 33,716

a Period length for Baseline period (up to 15 weeks) and for Experimental period (up to 16 weeks)

10

Page 11: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

The study ran for 30 consecutive weeks from the beginning of April 2015 to the end of October 2015.

The baseline period included the 13 weeks immediately prior to the delivery of the treatment and the

week(s) when the treatment (sticker prompt) was delivered (Nbaseline = 14 or 15 weeks, depending on

the authority). The baseline period was inclusive of the treatment delivery week(s) because the

impact of the treatment on resident behaviour could not be recorded until after residents had seen

the sticker and thus had the opportunity to amend their behaviour accordingly. The experimental

period therefore commenced the week following the delivery of the treatment (Nexperimental = 15 or 16

weeks, depending on the authority). Weights for the 26 control group rounds were monitored in the

same way as the 29 rounds in the treatment group.

2.3 Materials

The visual prompt used in this study was a sticker that was designed to encourage and remind

households to use the food waste recycling service, thereby discouraging food waste entry into refuse

bins. Previous research (cf. Aronson & O’Leary, 1983; Jae et al., 2008; Roberts et al., 2009; Sussman et

al., 2012; van Meurs & Aristoff, 2009) on design aspects known to improve the effectiveness of visual

prompts was taken into account during the design process. Stakeholder input was also sought during

a period of consultation with recycling officers and elected council officials from both local authorities.

The sticker design (Figure 1) was produced by professional designers and printed onto durable,

waterproof stickers. The stickers were A5 (148 x 210 mm) in size in order to fit neatly onto the raised

section of refuse wheelie bin lids.

11

Page 12: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

Figure 1: Sticker prompt design

The primary message, printed in large white bold capitals on a green background, read: “NO FOOD

WASTE PLEASE”. The secondary message, printed below in black lower case lettering on a white

background, prompted individuals to “Remember to use your food recycling caddy”. A picture of a

green outdoor food waste caddy was printed on the bottom right alongside a short web URL that

could be visited should households want further information or require a replacement caddy. Council

logos were printed in black at the very bottom of the sticker.

2.4 Procedure

For sticker distribution, an experienced waste consultancy was appointed to recruit and train

dedicated delivery staff. The consultancy was provided with collection round maps to plan routes in

advance and thus ensure that households could be stickered using the least amount of staff effort. All

collection rounds in the WCA1 treatment group were stickered during week 14 as this is when refuse

was collected. In WCA2, refuse is collected from half of the area one week and from the other half

during the following week. Therefore, some of the treatment group rounds for WCA2 were stickered

during week 14 and others during week 15.

If residents had queries or concerns about the stickers and approached sticker staff, they were

provided with an email address and contact number for the researcher. By deducting the number of

12

Page 13: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

stickers remaining at the end of each day from the total number of households in each round, it was

possible to estimate the total coverage rate. In total, 98% of treatment group households were

stickered during the exercise period.

2.5 Outcome measurement

To measure and compare changes in food waste recycling behaviour between the control and

treatment groups, weights (in tonnes, t) of separately collected food waste were monitored before

(baseline period) and after (experimental period) the delivery of the stickers. This information was

extracted from an established process that takes place before food waste gets transferred to the AD

facility. Each day, collection vehicles are weighed to determine the daily net weight of food collected

within each collection round. This information is then collated by a third party contractor and

provided to the local authority in spreadsheet format at the end of each month.

The researcher closely examined these data and any missing or unusually high/low weight values

were queried directly with the relevant WCA. If the WCA could not provide missing weight

information, or if their investigation could not provide a reasonable explanation for why a weight

value was unusually high or low, then the weight for that daily round was marked as ‘missing’.

The total weight of food waste collected from each of the 55 collection rounds was recorded for each

week within the baseline and experimental periods. Mean recycling weights for each collection round

were then calculated for each period, with missing weight values discounted from the calculation.

This provided a robust measurement of food waste recycling behaviour within each collection round

for each period. Food waste recycling behaviour for the control group was measured by calculating

the mean weight for the 26 randomly assigned collection rounds for both the baseline and

experimental period. Treatment group weights for both periods were calculated in the same way.

2.6 Data analysis

The assumption of multivariate normality (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007) was assessed graphically and by

examining skewness and kurtosis statistics. As these both showed the data were normally distributed,

13

Page 14: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

a parametric test was selected. A repeated measures ANOVA including the independent variable

experimental condition (2 levels: treatment vs. control) as the between subjects factor and the within-

subjects factor time (2 levels: baseline vs. experimental period) was initially conducted to determine

whether the sticker intervention had an effect by comparing the mean weight of food waste collected

during the experimental period with that captured during the baseline period (H1).

In order to test H2, another repeated measures ANOVA was conducted. This time, the mean weight

for each group at baseline was compared with the mean weight for each of the following post-

intervention time periods: short term (the 5 week period immediately following the intervention),

medium term (6 - 10 weeks post-intervention) and long term (11 - 16 weeks post-intervention).

3. Results

The first objective was to test whether providing households with a sticker prompt would significantly

increase the capture of food waste for recycling. It was hypothesised that, compared with the

baseline period, significantly higher weights of food waste would be captured in the treatment group

during the experimental period than in the control group (H1). Table 2 shows that there was no

change in weight of food waste captured for the control group between time periods. However, the

mean weight of food waste collected in the experimental group increased by 20% from 1.23 (SD =

0.35) to 1.49 (SD = 0.37) tonnes, and this increase was statistically significant (t (28) = -10.98, p =

0.00).

Table 2.

Mean weights (tonnes) for each condition and time period.Condition Baselinea Experimentala Difference (%)1. Control 1.24 (0.36)b 1.24 (0.36) -0.00912. Treatment 1.23 (0.35) 1.49 (0.37) 20.7426

a Weight values are weekly mean tonnages captured for all collection rounds in each condition over the baseline and experimental period respectively. b Figures in brackets are standard deviations.

A repeated measures ANOVA showed a significant main effect of time, F(1,53) = 74.15, p < 0.001, and

a significant interaction effect between condition and time, F(1,53) = 74.28, p < 0.001. Figure 2 shows

14

Page 15: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

that there was very little difference between experimental groups with respect to mean weights at

baseline, and highlights the significant difference between mean weights during the experimental

period. It can therefore be concluded that, compared with the baseline period, significantly higher

weights of food waste were collected in the treatment group during the experimental period than in

the control group.

Baseline Experimental1.22

1.27

1.32

1.37

1.42

1.47

1.52

ControlTreatment

Time

Mea

n fo

od w

ate

capt

ured

(t)

Figure 2: Mean weights of food waste for both groups pre and post intervention

The second objective was to determine whether this effect was sustained over time. It was

hypothesised that significantly higher mean weights would be collected in the treatment group during

each post-intervention period (short, medium and long term) than would be collected in the control

group (H2). Table 3 shows that the mean weight of food waste captured at baseline (M=1.24, SD =

0.36) in the control group fluctuated across each follow-up period: short term (M = 1.20, SD = 0.34),

medium term (M = 1.28, SD = 0.41), and long term (M = 1.25, SD = 0.38), whereas these weights

clearly increased across time in the treatment group: baseline (M = 1.23, SD = 0.35), short term (M =

1.42, SD = 0.34), medium term (M = 1.74, SD = 0.44) and long term (M = 1.59, SD = 0.49).

Mauchly’s test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated, X2 (5) = 49.99, p = 0.00,

therefore the degrees of freedom were corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser estimates of sphericity (Ɛ

= 0.701). The results show that there was a significant main effect of time, F(2.10, 111.47) = 11.49, p <

0.001, and a significant interaction effect between condition and time, F(2.10, 111.47) = 10.63, p <

0.001.

15

Page 16: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

Table 3.

Mean weights (tonnes) for each condition and time period.Condition Baselinea Short Medium Long1. Control 1.24 (0.36) b 1.20 (0.34) 1.28 (0.41) 1.25 (0.38)2. Treatment 1.23 (0.35) 1.42 (0.34) 1.47 (0.44) 1.59 (0.49)

a Weight values are weekly mean tonnages captured for all collection rounds in each condition over each of the time periods b Figures in brackets are standard deviations.

Figure 3 highlights the significant differences between mean weights in the short, medium and long

term. It can therefore be concluded that, compared with the baseline period, significantly higher

weights of food waste were captured in the treatment group during each phase of the experimental

period than in the control group. It is perhaps also interesting to note that not only did these

increases persist into the longer term, they also increased over time.

Baseline Short Medium Long1.2

1.25

1.3

1.35

1.4

1.45

1.5

1.55

1.6

ControlTreatment

Time

Mea

n fo

od w

aste

capt

ured

(t)

Figure 3: Mean weights of food waste for both groups by time period

3.1 Cost-benefit analysis

It was important that the policy intervention under evaluation was affordable as well as effective. The

total cost for printing and distributing (i.e., the cost of the delivery staff and their recruitment) the

sticker prompt was £11,702, or £0.35 per household. This cost does not include the cost of sticker

design or the cost of analysing results as these were both completed in-house by existing staff

members. Instead these were recorded as 'in-kind' contributions as such project work typically falls

16

Page 17: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

within the remit of local authority staff members and, as such, is rarely factored in as a financial

project cost.

The savings were calculated based on the difference in costs between general waste disposal at

landfill and separate food waste treatment. The cost of disposal varies between local authorities, as

these are dependent on the proportion of waste that gets sent to Energy from Waste facilities (EfW)

vs. landfill, and on the local gate fees that are charged. At the time of writing, the cost per tonne for

AD in the UK (£18 to £38) was much lower than the cost per tonne for general waste disposal (£70 to

£109) (Lets Recycle, 2016). So, for every tonne of food waste that gets diverted away from the general

waste stream, the local authority stands to make a substantial saving.

During the baseline period for this study, the mean weight of food waste collected from the 29

treatment group rounds each week was 1.23 tonnes per round. This increased to 1.49 tonnes during

the experimental period, a difference of 0.26 tonnes. By extrapolating this increase across all

treatment group collection rounds and assuming the increase was maintained over time, using actual

costs of disposal and AD for Surrey in 2014/15 (SCC, 2016) the payback period for this study was

calculated to be just 23 weeks. This, of course, would increase if a drop-off in recycling participation

did occur. Also, an experienced waste consultancy was recruited to ensure that the sticker

distribution was carried out to the highest possible standard during the experimental study. In

practice, stickers could be distributed by in-house staff members (e.g., existing collection crews,

recycling officers) or by agency staff, which could marginally reduce the cost of the intervention. It

may also be possible to achieve economies of scale for the printing and distribution of a larger scale

rollout of this intervention.

In addition to the financial benefits already outlined, there were a few unmeasured benefits

associated with this study. There was a marked increase in requests for new food waste recycling

caddies. The diversion of food waste to recycling would have reduced greenhouse gas emissions and

other associated environmental impacts (Bekker et al., 2010; Nomura et al., 2011).

17

Page 18: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

4. Discussion

The aim of the present study was to investigate the real world effectiveness, practical feasibility and

cost-efficiency of using stickers as a visual prompt to encourage food waste recycling behaviour. It

was predicted that significantly higher weights of food waste would be captured in the treatment

group than in the control group (H1), and that the impact of the intervention would be sustained over

time (H2).

This study adds to the existing body of literature by supporting the theory that visual prompts, in this

case a simple and practically feasible bin sticker, can be an effective and affordable method of

improving food waste recycling rates, producing effects that appear to persist for up to four months.

These results appear to conflict with previous research that has suggested that, in isolation, visual

prompts are only modestly effective for encouraging recycling behaviour (e.g., Spacarelli et al., 1990).

There are several potential explanations for the impact of this intervention. It is possible that the

success of the intervention could be attributed to the longer-term exposure proffered by the semi-

permanent sticker, compared with leaflets or posters that might only be read once before they are

discarded. Stickers were affixed to the top of refuse bin lids, therefore increasing the opportunities for

them to be noticed and the message to be internalised. As food waste recycling is a relatively new

service, many individuals may be aware of the food waste recycling bins but are still in the habit of

automatically putting food waste into the refuse bin together with other waste. For such individuals,

the prompt may serve as a persistent reminder that they should be separating out their food waste.

Stickers therefore appear to be an effective medium for capturing the attention of a target audience,

as they allow for information to be processed sufficiently (Bernstad, 2014).

It is also possible that the indiscriminate distribution of stickers to all households could be partially

responsible for the effects achieved. By visibly placing stickers on all refuse bins, the local authority

may have signalled to households that food waste recycling was the socially approved behaviour (the

injunctive norm) (cf. Cialdini et al., 1990). Further, as more households began to use their caddies in

response to the sticker, the behaviour may have become more visible to non-recycling households. It

18

Page 19: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

is also therefore possible that the descriptive (group) norm for food waste recycling changed in some

areas and motivated a further group of households to start using their own food waste caddies. In this

way, the prompt may have served as a guide to behaviour for some households (Schultz et al., 2008).

Future research could include a follow-up survey to investigate this further.

Several authors have noted that few pro-environmental behaviour change studies have successfully

examined the longer-term effects of behaviour change interventions (Abrahamse et al., 2005;

DeYoung, 1993; Sussman & Gifford, 2012). In this study, the impact of the sticker prompt was

monitored for up to 16 weeks following the distribution of the sticker. Results showed that

significantly higher mean weights of food waste were collected in the treatment group during each of

the short, medium and longer term post-intervention time periods than were collected in the control

group. Further, the weights continued to increase, rather than stay level, across time.

There could be several possible explanations for this finding. Firstly, the sticker distribution resulted in

additional requests for food waste recycling caddies. There was a lead time of several weeks between

caddy requests and delivery, therefore the effect on behaviour for these households would not have

materialised until the medium to long term monitoring periods. It is also possible that there was a

‘time lag’ between some individuals noticing the sticker on the refuse bin and this translating into a

change in behaviour. In some households a different individual could be responsible for disposing of

refuse into the outdoor bin (where the sticker was displayed) than the one who disposes of food

waste indoors. Therefore it could take time for the message to translate between members of a

household and for a collective decision to change behaviour to be made. Again, future research would

be required to investigate this further, perhaps by monitoring the behaviour of a smaller cohort of

households more closely and/or by conducting a follow up survey. Finally, in their study examining

the effect of signs on ‘lights off’ behaviour in public bathrooms, Sussman & Gifford (2012) found that

pro-environmental behaviour returned to near to baseline levels when the signs were removed, and

increased again when signs were re-introduced. It is therefore possible that the more permanent

nature of the bin sticker accounted for the persistency of results found in this study. More research

19

Page 20: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

would be required, however, to establish whether this effect would persist beyond 16 weeks as

residents became habituated to the prompt.

The strength of this field experiment lies both in its ecological validity and its practical implications

(e.g., financial savings and environmental impacts) for local authorities (Cialdini, 2009). As the key

goal of this research was to provide practical insights for local authorities to apply in the real world,

high external validity was a priority. A lab-based study could have been used to test the effectiveness

of the sticker prompt but the results would have had high-internal validity and low external validity

due to the artificial setting and probable use of students as participants (Keizer et al., 2013). By

incorporating both randomisation and a control group in this study, the disadvantages associated with

field experiments (cf. Cialdini, 2009; Keizer et al., 2013) were certainly limited. However, the

generalizability of these results could have been improved had there been the opportunity to

replicate the study elsewhere.

While results of this study were promising, there were also several limitations. This study was

designed to inform decision-making concerning the countywide rollout of a programme of policy

interventions designed to encourage food waste recycling behaviour. Since these decisions were time

sensitive to meet targets (Timlett & Williams, 2011), it was only possible to monitor behaviour for a

total of 16 weeks post intervention. While this monitoring period was similar, and in some cases

longer, than can be found in many similar studies (cf. Steg & Vlek, 2009 for further references), future

research would benefit from continuously monitoring behaviour over a longer time period (e.g., six to

twelve months) to examine if and when effects begin to ‘drop-off’ and to what extent. Without

monitoring the effects that occur during the full term of the payback period, the authors cannot

conclusively determine whether the intervention is in fact truly cost effective. Furthermore, although

it was not possible to access such data for the purposes of this study, future research should compare

the weight data for the study with data from the same time period during the previous year, to isolate

any seasonal effects.

20

Page 21: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

The design was limited by the spatial level at which the participating local authorities’ could provide

accurate weight data to measure outcomes. For financial and logistical reasons, it was not possible to

measure behaviour at the individual household level (e.g., weigh individual caddies). This meant that

it was not possible to apportion responsibility for the observed increases in weights to: (1) current

food waste recyclers who were reminded to use their caddy more often or more effectively, thereby

improving their performance; or (2) non-recyclers who were persuaded to start using their caddy for

the first time. Measuring the weight of individual food waste caddies requires substantial human and

financial resources, as a dedicated team of research assistants must manually weigh each caddy prior

to collection. A review of the literature found no previous experimental research studies that used

measurements of individual households food waste caddies as an outcome measure. Future research

should explore the logistical and financial viability of this monitoring technique, to be able to

determine the relative responses of each group of individuals to the prompt.

Finally, a manipulation check was also not included, meaning it was not possible to be certain about

how many of the residents who received a sticker actually noticed its presence. Future research could

include a short follow up survey or door-stepping exercise to evaluate residents’ perceptions of the

sticker and the extent to which it was actually viewed and processed.

5. Conclusions

This experimental study tested a simple behavioural intervention in a residential area in South East

England, which would be practically feasible for local authorities to implement at scale. The study

demonstrated that visual prompts, in the form of a green refuse bin sticker, significantly increased the

capture of food waste for recycling and the effect was sustained in the longer term. Additionally, at

£0.35 per household, this intervention appears to be a cost-efficient method for increasing recycling

rates.

While the environmental impact of recycling food waste might be low in comparison to some types of

pro-environmental behaviour, the payout could be high if many individuals are encouraged to engage

(Sussman & Gifford, 2012). Similarly, while visual prompts may not be as effective as other types of

21

Page 22: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

behaviour change intervention (Steg & Vlek, 2009), they are one of the simplest and most cost-

efficient to introduce and could represent a ‘quick win’ for local authority waste managers looking to

achieve change with limited budgets. Visual prompts will not change the minds and behaviour of all

individuals, and the individual impact of each person’s behavioural change may be small. However,

the aggregate impact of this intervention, if introduced at scale, could be large.

Acknowledgements

This study was funded by the Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council (EPSRC) (Student

grant number EP/G037612/1), and Surrey County Council.

6. References

Abrahamse, W. & Steg, L. (2013). Social influence approaches to encourage resource conservation: A

meta-analysis. Global Environmental Change, 23(6), pp.1773-1785.

Abrahamse, W., Steg, L., Vlek, C. & Rothengatter, T. (2005). A review of intervention studies aimed at

household energy conservation. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25(3), pp.273-291.

Amass, L., Bickel, W., Higgins, S., Budney, A. & Foerg, F. (1993). The taking of free condoms in a drug

abuse treatment clinic: the effects of location and posters. Am J Public Health, 83(10), pp.1466-1468.

Andersen, L., Bungum, T. & Moonie, S. (2012). The Effects of Environmental Prompts on Stair Usage.

Nevada Journal of Public Health, 9(1(2), pp. 6-12.

Arbuthnot, J., Tedeschi, R., Wayner, M., Turner, J., Kressel, S. & Rush, R. (1976). The Induction of

Sustained Recycling Behavior Through the Foot-in-the-Door Technique. Journal of Environmental

Systems, 6(4), pp.355-368.

Aronson, E. & O'Leary, M. (1983). The Relative Effectiveness of Models and Prompts on Energy

Conservation: A Field Experiment in a Shower Room. Journal of Environmental Systems, 12(3), pp.219-

224.

Austin, J., Hatfield, D., Grindle, A. & Bailey, J. (1993). Increasing recycling in office environments: The

22

Page 23: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

effects of specific, informative cues. J Appl Behav Anal, 26(2), pp.247-253.

Baltes, M. & Hayward, S. (1976). Application and evaluation of strategies to reduce pollution:

Behavioral control of littering in a football stadium. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61(4), pp.501-506.

Bartram, A. (2009). Behaviour Change Intervention Tools: A discussion paper. Government of South

Australia: Department for Transport Energy and Infrastructure.

Bedford, T., Collingwood, P., Darnton, A., Evans, D., Gatersleben, B. & Abrahamse, W. (2010).

Motivations for pro-environmental behaviour: a report to the Department for Environment Food and

Rural Affairs. London: Resolve.

Bekker, M., Cumming, T., Osborne, N., Bruining, A., McClean, J. & Leland, L. (2010). Encouraging

electricity savings in a university residential hall through a combination of feedback, visual prompts,

and incentives. J Appl Behav Anal, 43(2), pp.327-331.

Bernstad, A. (2014). Household food waste separation behavior and the importance of convenience.

Waste Management, 34(7), pp.1317-1323.

Bernstad, A., la Cour Jansen, J. & Aspegren, A. (2013). Door-stepping as a strategy for improved food

waste recycling behaviour: Evaluation of a full-scale experiment. Resources, Conservation and

Recycling, 73, pp.94-103.

Borgstede, C. & Andersson, K. (2010). Environmental Information: Explanatory Factors for Information

Behavior. Sustainability, 2(9), pp.2785-2798.

Brehm, J. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. New York: Academic Press.

Bridgwater, E & Parfitt, J (2009). Evaluation of the WRAP separate food waste collection trials.

Banbury, WRAP.

Burn, S. (1991). Social Psychology and the Stimulation of Recycling Behaviors: The Block Leader

Approach. J Appl Social Pyschol, 21(8), pp.611-629.

Chui, W.S., Wait, C.W., & Ahmad, R. (2015). Local and global Messages for Prompting Energy

23

Page 24: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

Conservation Behaviour: An Experimental Study in University. Jurnal Teknologi (Sciences &

Engineering), 74(2), pp. 87-92.

Cialdini, R. (2009). We Have to Break Up. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(1), pp.5-6.

Cialdini, R., Reno, R. & Kallgren, C. (1990). A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept

of norms to reduce littering in public places. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(6),

pp.1015-1026.

Cope, J., Allred, L. & Morsell, J. (1991). Signs as deterrents of illegal parking in spaces designated for

individuals with physical disabilities. J Appl Behav Anal, 24(1), pp.59-63.

Dahlén, L. & Lagerkvist, A. (2009). Evaluation of recycling programmes in household waste collection

systems. Waste Management & Research, 28(7), pp.577-586.

De Young, R. (1993). Changing Behavior and Making it Stick: The Conceptualization and Management

of Conservation Behavior. Environment and Behavior, 25(3), pp.485-505.

Defra, (2011). Government Review of Waste Policy in England. London: Department for Environment,

Food and Rural Affairs.

Defra, (2013). Waste Management Plan for England. London: Department for Environment Food and

Rural Affairs.

Defra, (2015). Food Statistics Pocketbook. In year update.. London: Department for Environment Food

and Rural Affairs.

Dillard, J. & Shen, L. (2005). On the Nature of Reactance and its Role in Persuasive Health

Communication. Communication Monographs, 72(2), pp.144-168.

Dolan, P., Hallsworth, M., Halpern, D., King, D., & Vlaev., I. (2010). MINDSPACE: Influencing behaviour

through public policy. Institute for Government Report for the Cabinet Office, UK. Available at:

www.instituteforgovernment.org.uk/our-work/better-policy-making/mindspace-

behavioural-economics. [Accessed 14 Sept. 2016].

24

Page 25: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

Durdan, C., Reeder, G. & Hecht, P. (1985). Litter in a University Cafeteria: Demographic Data and the

Use of Prompts as an Intervention Strategy. Environment and Behavior, 17(3), pp.387-404.

Emmerink, R., Nijkamp, P. & Rietveld, P. (1995). Is congestion pricing a first-best strategy in transport

policy? A critical review of arguments. Environment and Planning B: Planning and Design, 22(5),

pp.581-602.

Frederiks, E., Stenner, K. & Hobman, E. (2015). Household energy use: Applying behavioural

economics to understand consumer decision-making and behaviour. Renewable and Sustainable

Energy Reviews, 41, pp.1385-1394.

Geller, E. (1989). Applied Behavior Analysis and Social Marketing: An Integration for Environmental

Preservation. Journal of Social Issues, 45(1), pp.17-36.

Geller, E., Winett, R. & Everett, P. (1982). Preserving the environment. New York: Pergamon Press.

Geller, E., Wittmier, J. & Orebaugh, A. (1976). Instructions as a Determinant of Paper-Disposal

Behaviors. Environment and Behavior, 8(3), pp.417-439.

Goldenhar, L. & Connell, C. (1992). Effects of Educational and Feedback Interventions on Recycling

Knowledge, Attitudes, Beliefs, and Behaviors. Journal of Environmental Systems, 21(4), pp.321-333.

Graham-Rowe, E., Jessop, D. C., & Sparks, P. (2015). Predicting household food waste reduction using

an extended theory of planned behaviour. Resources Conservation and Recycling, 101. pp. 194-202.

House of Commons (2015). Food Waste. Briefing Paper Number CBP07045. London: House of

Commons Library.

House of Lords (Science and Technology Select Committee). (2011). Behaviour Change Report.

Stationery Office. Available:

publications.parliament.uk/pa/ld201012/ldselect/ldsctech/179/179.pdf.

[Accessed 7 Apr. 2016].

Huffman, K., Grossnickle, W., Cope, J. & Huffman, K. (1995). Litter Reduction: A Review and

25

Page 26: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

Integration of the Literature. Environment and Behavior, 27(2), pp.153-183.

Huybers, S., Van Houten, R. & Malenfant, J. (2004). Reducing conflicts between motor vehicles and

pedestrians: the separate and combined effects of pavement markings and a sign prompt. J Appl

Behav Anal, 37(4), pp.445-456.

IPCC (2013) Climate Change 2013: The Physical Science Basis. Contribution of Working Group I to the

Fifth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change [Stocker, T.F., D. Qin, G.-

K. Plattner, M. Tignor, S.K. Allen, J. Boschung, A. Nauels, Y. Xia, V. Bex and P.M. Midgley (eds.)].

Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, United Kingdom and New York, NY, USA.

Jacobs, H. and Bailey, J. (1982). Evaluating Participation in a Residential Recycling Program. Journal of

Environmental Systems, 12(2), pp.141-152.

Jae, H., Delvecchio, D., & Cowles, D. (2008). Picture-Text Incongruency in Print Advertisements among

Low- and High-Literacy Consumers. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 42(3), pp.439-451.

John, P. (2013). All tools are informational now: how information and persuasion define the tools of

government. Policy & Politics, 41(4), pp605-620.

Kallbekken, S & Sælen, H. (2013). ‘Nudging’ hotel guests to reduce food waste as a win-win

environmental measure. Economics Letters, 119(3), pp.325-327.

Karim Ghani, W., Rusli, I., Biak, D. & Idris, A. (2013). An application of the theory of planned behaviour

to study the influencing factors of participation in source separation of food waste. Waste

Management, 33(5), pp.1276-1281.

Keizer, K., Lindenberg, S. & Steg, L. (2013). Doing field studies. What is it all about?. Group Processes

& Intergroup Relations, 17(3), pp.404-410.

Kirakozian, A. (2016). One without the other? Behavioural and incentive policies for household waste

management. Journal of Economic Surveys, 30(3),pp.526-551.

Lets Recycle (2016). Lets Recycle prices. [online] Available at: http://www.letsrecycle.com/prices/.

26

Page 27: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

[Accessed 7 Apr. 2016].

Local Government Association (2014). Under Pressure. How councils are planning future cuts..

London: Local Government Association.

Nigbur, D., Lyons, E. & Uzzell, D. (2010). Attitudes, norms, identity and environmental behaviour:

Using an expanded theory of planned behaviour to predict participation in a kerbside recycling

programme. British Journal of Social Psychology, 49(2), pp.259-284.

Nimon, K. F. (2012). Statistical Assumptions of Substantive Analyses Across the General Liner Model: A

Mini-Review. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, pp.322.

Nomura, H., John, P. & Cotterill, S. (2011). The use of feedback to enhance environmental outcomes: a

randomised controlled trial of a food waste scheme. Local Environment, 16(7), pp.637-653.

Marteau, T. M., Ogilvie, D., Roland, M., Suhrcke, M., & Kelly, M. P. (2011). Judging nudging: can

nudging improve population health?. British Medical Journal, 342(jan25 3), pp228-228.

Michie, S. (2015). Behaviour change beyond nudge. Available at:http://instead.group.shef.ac.uk/wp-

content/uploads/2015/02/Susan-Michie-Behaviour-Change-beyond-Nudge.pdf. [Accessed 15

September 2016].

Office of National Statistics (2011). Census: Aggregate data (England and Wales). UK Data Service

Census Support.

Osbaldiston, R. & Schott, J. (2011). Environmental Sustainability and Behavioral Science: Meta-

Analysis of Proenvironmental Behavior Experiments. Environment and Behavior, 44(2), pp.257-299.

Oskamp, S. (1995). Resource Conservation and Recycling: Behavior and Policy. Journal of Social Issues,

51(4), pp.157-177.

Parfitt, J., Barthel, M. & Macnaughton, S. (2010). Food waste within food supply chains: quantification

and potential for change to 2050. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological

Sciences, 365(1554), pp.3065-3081.

27

Page 28: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

Read, A.D. (1999) “A weekly doorstep recycling collection, I had no idea we could!” Resources,

Conservation and Recycling, 26(3-4), pp.217-249.

Reiter, S. & Samuel, W. (1980). Littering as a Function of Prior Litter and The Presence or Absence of

Prohibitive Signs1. J Appl Social Pyschol, 10(1), pp.45-55.

Roberts, N., Mohamed, Z., Wong, P., Johnson, M., Loh, L. & Partridge, M. (2009). The development

and comprehensibility of a pictorial asthma action plan. Patient Education and Counseling, 74(1),

pp.12-18.

Schultz, P. (1999). Changing Behavior With Normative Feedback Interventions: A Field Experiment on

Curbside Recycling. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21(1), pp.25-36.

Schultz, P. (2013). Strategies for Promoting Proenvironmental Behavior: Lots of tools but few

instructions. European Psychologist, 19(2), pp.107-117.

Schultz, P., Oskamp, S. & Mainieri, T. (1995). Who recycles and when? A review of personal and

situational factors. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15(2), pp.105-121.

Schultz, P., Khazian, A.M., Zaleski, A.C. (2008). Using normative social influence to promote

conservation among hotel guests. Social Influence, 3 (1), pp. 4-32.

Siaw Chui, W., Weng Wai, C. & Ahmad, R. (2015). Local and Global Messages for Prompting Energy

Conservation Behaviour: An Experimental Study in University. Jurnal Teknologi, 74(2).

Spaccarelli, S., Zolik, E. & Jason, L. (1990). Effects of Verbal Prompting and Block Characteristics on

Participation in Curbside Newspaper Recycling. Journal of Environmental Systems, 19(1), pp.45-57.

Steg, L. & Vlek, C. (2009). Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: An integrative review and

research agenda. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 29(3), pp.309-317.

Sunstein, C.R. (2015, in press). Do people like nudges? Administrative Law Review. Abstract available

at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2604084. [Accessed 14 Sept. 2016].

28

Page 29: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

Sussman, R. & Gifford, R. (2012). Please turn off the lights: The effectiveness of visual prompts.

Applied Ergonomics, 43(3), pp.596-603.

Sussman, R., Greeno, M., Gifford, R. & Scannell, L. (2012). The Effectiveness of Models and Prompts

on Waste Diversion: A Field Experiment on Composting by Cafeteria Patrons. J Appl Soc Psychol, 43(1),

pp.24-34.

Surrey County Council (SCC) (2016). Results from 2013/14 composition analysis. SCC, Surrey.

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics. Boston: Pearson/Allyn & Bacon.

Thaler, R., & Sunstein, C. (2008). Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth and Happiness.

Boston Yale University Press, USA.

Thomas, C. & Sharp, V. (2013). Understanding the normalisation of recycling behaviour and its

implications for other pro-environmental behaviours: A review of social norms and recycling.

Resources, Conservation and Recycling, 79, pp.11-20.

Timlett, R. & Williams, I. (2011). The ISB model (infrastructure, service, behaviour): A tool for waste

practitioners. Waste Management, 31(6), pp.1381-1392.

Tucker, P. (2001). Understanding recycling behaviour. Glasgow: University of Paisley.

Van Houten, R. & Malenfant, J. (2004). Effects of a driver enforcement program on yielding to

pedestrians. J Appl Behav Anal, 37(3), pp.351-363.

van Meurs, L. & Aristoff, M. (2009). Split-Second Recognition: What Makes Outdoor Advertising

Work? Journal of Advertising Research, 49(1), pp.82-92.

Werner, C., Rhodes, M. & Partain, K. (1998). Designing Effective Instructional Signs With Schema

Theory: Case Studies of Polystyrene Recycling. Environment and Behavior, 30(5), pp.709-735.

Winett, R. (1978). Prompting Turning-Out Lights in Unoccupied Rooms. Journal of Environmental

Systems, 7(3), pp.237-241.

Witmer, J. & Geller, E. (1976). Facilitating paper recycling: effects of prompts, raffles, and contests1. J

29

Page 30: Abstract - epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/812309/14/Surrey Uni Manuscript.docx  · Web viewThe aim of the present study was to investigate the real world ... economics to understand

Appl Behav Anal, 9(3), pp.315-322.

Wastedataflow.org. (2016). WasteDataFlow Waste Management. [online] Available at:

http://www.wastedataflow.org. [Accessed 7 Apr. 2016].

WRAP, (2011). The water and carbon footprint of household food and drink waste in the UK. Banbury:

WRAP and WWF.

WRAP (2013). Household Food and Drink Waste in the United Kingdom 2012. Banbury: WRAP.

WRAP (2016). Household food waste collections guide. [online] Available at:

http://www.wrap.org.uk/content/food-waste-collections-guide-section-11-increasing-food-waste-

capture-existing-separate-week [Accessed 19 Apr. 2016].

Zhang, R., Elmashad, H., Hartman, K., Wang, F., Liu, G., Choate, C. & Gamble, P. (2007).

Characterization of food waste as feedstock for anaerobic digestion. Bioresource Technology, 98(4),

pp.929-935.

30