32
DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche – Paper presented at the OMEP World Conference in Gothenburg August 11th - 13th 2010 By Ole Henrik Hansen PhD fellow Danish School of Education Aarhus University Department of Curriculum Research Research Unit of Childhood, Learning and Curriculum Theory Office B234 Tuborgvej 164, DK-2400 Copenhagen Telephone +4588889859 Mail [email protected] Website www.dpu.dk/about/ohh or www.dpu.dk/bld Ole Henrik Hansen MA (ed). PhD Fellow Aarhus University 1

A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche –

Paper presented at the OMEP World Conference in GothenburgAugust 11th - 13th 2010

By Ole Henrik HansenPhD fellowDanish School of EducationAarhus UniversityDepartment of Curriculum ResearchResearch Unit of Childhood, Learning and Curriculum TheoryOffice B234Tuborgvej 164,DK-2400 CopenhagenTelephone +4588889859Mail [email protected] www.dpu.dk/about/ohhor www.dpu.dk/bld

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

1

Page 2: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

Abstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche1, one challenge is to identify educational content that takes into consideration the child’s well-being, as well as personal development, social abilities, language acquisition and cognitive development, and in the process provide basis for democratic youth by seeing, hearing and respecting the individual child (Broström, 2006b; UNESCO, 1989).

The field of research is preschool children, who acquire care, norms, values and skills, trying to learn about and understand themselves as beings in the surrounding world and environment (Broström, 2006a; Lindahl & Pramling Samuelsson, 2002, p. 15).

This paper is based on the dominant theories of language acquisition and Nordic theories of curriculum research (Broström, 2006a; Broström & Vejleskov, 2009; Lindahl, 2002; Lindahl & Pramling Samuelsson, 2002), and discuss why and how language acquisition can be facilitated in the Danish crèche, for the child aged eight to fourteen month of age (Stern, 1998, 2004; Tomasello, 2009). The paper discusses pedagogical research between post-humanist (Barad, 2007) and postmodern language psychology (Pinker, 1994; Tomasello, 2009).

Keywords: Crèche; Language; Curriculum theory; Socio-cultural; Post-humanism; Child’s perspective.

Introduction

One could argue that pedagogy in the Danish crèche is a light version of pedagogy in the Danish preschool, and one reason for that claim would be that research concerning the child’s stay in the Danish crèche is inadequate (Nordenbo, 2010), and consequent reflection concerning a pedagogic structure in the crèche may be limited. It seems beside the point, that Denmark has a national curriculum for the crèche that in six themes emphasizes the child’s acquisition of competences 2. On the contrary the national curriculum seems to have had a slightly different effect. On the one hand it has been part of a good and needed professionalisation of the pedagogues. These themes have infused the profession with comparable nationwide educational content 1 In 2007, 122,000 children aged one to three years of age attended daycare: 13,000 children, attended a crèche, 65,000 a municipality day-care home and 44,000 a mixed age institution. That is approximately 90% of all children aged one to three years. 2 The six themes in the Danish national curriculum for day care, preschools and leisure time centres are: 1. Language. 2. Social competences. 3. Personal competences. 4. Cultural expressions and values. 5. Nature and phenomena of nature. 6. Body and physical movement (Retsinformation, 2007).

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

2

Page 3: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

for early childhood. On the other hand, in order to implement political agendas and developmental standards for some of the themes, in particularly the language theme, there has been a tendency to narrow down the pedagogical approach, assuming that e.g. the process of language acquisition for small children in the crèche is somewhat similar to that in preschool or even in school.

The main problem may be an unreflected academic insight in how the small child acquires language. In Denmark there are two main theories of language acquisition, each one claiming its validity: A functional linguistic concept (Bleses, 2007), and a usage based concept (Tomasello, 2009). The linguistic concept has the present political support, probably because this concept provides developmental standards, that can be tested and thereby contain some degree of assumed accountability. The other concept is a socio-cultural, or postmodern social-pedagogic approach, where language is believed to be acquired as individual and contextualised competences. This concept does not provide a basis for standards or fixed scales that can be used as a parameter for testing. On the other hand, the socio-cultural stand needs to emphasize e.g. the symbolic gestures, in order to extend language and mind (Clark, 2008). But as illustrated in the end of the paper by preliminary data, the Danish crèche seems reluctant to facilitate that kind of dialogue.

The pedagogic tradition in the Danish crèche is, inspired by humanistic psychology and reform pedagogic tradition, a practice where development is the most important goal in early childhood education (Broström, 2006a). This tradition implies that the child’s self-governed activity provides the best development, which in practice leaves the smallest children with a mainly caring environment, and little educational content. Combined with a linguistic concept where the children are stimulated according to development psychological scales, and tested for their language performances at the age of three, this becomes a problem for those children who don’t perform well.

It is a paradox that children, even before they can celebrate their third birthday, must perform at a test in order to pass to the next level and at the same time, for two years, have been attending a pedagogical system, that only to a certain degree provides relevant educational content or, as seen in some of my empirical observations, provides educational content which is more relevant to the preschool child and, due to the fact that 12 children and two to three adults are in the same room, the system can only provide limited adult attention to the individual child. The consequence is, that some children risk lacking a) linguistic challenges that can help their linguistic and cognitive development or b) sufficient empathic care. Instead they are left with a custodial kind of care that primarily ensures their physical wellness.

These paradoxes are the substantial topics for my PhD project, where it is a main ambition to outline prescriptive and differentiated curricula, based on video-graphic and ethnographic research concerning the crèche’s ability to facilitate a

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

3

Page 4: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

relevant language in the child.The project is based on themes of the national curriculum for the crèche, more

specifically on the theme about language acquisition. However, investigating the child’s language involves two other themes; firstly the theme of social competences and secondly the theme of personal competences. These three themes are naturally connected, and it is not possible to separate them in a usage based language pedagogy (Tomasello, 2009). One of the main reasons for this is that language and thereby cognition, as described below, evolves from socio-cognitive and socio-motivational reasons and furthermore as a guided and contextualized process involving the child’s own activity in the social environment. In this concept, language facilitates cognition, identity and a self-awareness which helps the child to transform from an object to a subject (Merleau-Ponty, 1984). And when children communicate with other children they learn to control social situations, and to extend their mind and cultural knowledge (Tomasello, 2009).

But infant communication is challenging to pedagogues. Mainly because preverbal children do not master an iconic verbalized language, which means that they are at the mercy of the caregiver’s ability to appreciate the child’s perspective, using intuition and experience in reading and interpreting children’s gabble, body language, gestures and eye movements in trying to understand their thoughts and intentions.

A curriculum concerning language acquisition for the children from eight to fourteen month of age must therefore contain three main elements: 1) A theorized reflected standpoint which may facilitate a content of learning. 2) A clarification of the pedagogues role, and 3) A specific balancing of the three main pedagogic elements, that are expected to facilitate the child’s well being and development: Care; upbringing and teaching (Broström, 2006a; Lindahl, 2002).

The main goal of this paper is hereby to discuss a setting for a caring and educational environment, where the small child can decode contextual meaning, and thereby interpret into words and thoughts.

Part one – a concept of learning

The global war on language acquisition

In the past three decades the study of the child’s first language has been divided into two opposing fields, one that builds upon functional linguistic skills (among others Bleses & Højen, 2009; Chomsky, 1975; Pinker, 1994) and one that emphasize context-sensitive, psychological processes (among others Bruner & Watson, 1983; Tomasello & Todd, 1983; Vygotsky, 1962). There has been little communication

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

4

Page 5: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

between the disciplines, and periods where each group drew upon the theories and methods of the other in limited and often limiting ways.

The linguistic approach understands language acquisition as an ongoing progression, where the child develops language according to universal rules, step by step as though the child climbed a ladder. And children do so, generation after generation, not because it is useful to them, but because it is an instinctive process, and they really just can’t help doing it. (Pinker, 1994, p. 20).

The fact that language is described as a universally instinctive phenomenon which develops in predictable steps, is in Denmark a politically popular language concept (Bleses, 2007; Bleses & Højen, 2009). Not only because the concept is useful to explain reproduction of knowledge but also because, from a laypersons point of view, it claims that language acquisition is an individual cognitive act, which seems a logical and effective approach. And as a politically popular side effect, it becomes possible to test the child for linguistic, age-determined cognitive skills.

If, instead, language is conceptualized as a socially developed, contextualized relation to individual time, place and culture, gender and socio-economic conditions, then there is no either natural or universal language to measure (Barad, 2007; Burman, 2008; Dahlberg, Moss, & Pence, 2007). In that concept children’s learning is a contextualized phenomenon and the voices of the children are important, as are their participation in constructing and determining their lives as they possess agency (Dahlberg, et al., 2007, p. 49). The infant becomes a subject from the very start, rather than a naturally developing object which can be reduced to separate and measurable categories. It is from the start a sentient being rather than a foetus. And language acquisition becomes …

… a cooperative and communicative activity, in which children (…) make meaning of the world, together with adults and equally important, other children (…) Learning is not transmission of knowledge taking the child to preordinated outcomes, nor is the child a passive receiver and reproducer (…) ‘a child has got a hundred languages and is born with a lot of possibilities and a lot of expressions and potentialities’ (Wallin, Mæchel, & Barsotti, 1981) (Dahlberg, et al., 2007, p. 50).

In this concept, language acquisition is not a question of developing an existing language but more a question of constructing a cultural equivalent language, that imparts the child with a sense of accordance between themselves and the surrounding world. In this process the language induces contextual meaning in the consciousness of the child. This language concept is to a greater extent in accordance with the Danish pedagogical tradition, where it is due to social activities that the child develops social and linguistic abilities (Broström, 2006a):

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

5

Page 6: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

The different concepts of learning imply different views on the same field of research. Here is an example where first the linguistic viewpoint, and then the postmodern viewpoint describes the same central principle in the child’s first language, but with opposite conclusions:

The linguistic viewpoint:Language is not a cultural artefact that we learn the way we learn to tell time or how the federal government works. Instead it (…) develops spontaneously, without conscious effort or formal instruction, and it is deployed without awareness of its underlying logic, it is qualitatively the same in every individual, and it is distinct from more general abilities to process information or behave intelligently. (…) It conveys the idea that people know how to talk in more or less the sense that spiders know how to spin webs. (…) a three year old (…) is a grammatical genius, but is quite incompetent at the visual arts (Pinker, 1994, pp. 4-5).

The linguistic hypothesis is: the small child understands words, and thereby decodes contextual meaning.

And then a Usage-based viewpoint:

It is emphasized that it is the caregiver’s different use of language, which creates different language. Caregivers use of language to describe the outside world, generates language in the child, primarily as a tool to create meaning in the surroundings. And children that experience language as a tool of regulation and prescription, learn language as something they can use to manipulate their social interactions (Nelson, 1985; Tomasello, 2003).

Tomasello emphasizes that it is not as much what the caregiver says to the child that affect the child’s language, but rather how she says it and how she respond (Tomasello & Todd, 1983). Also Nelson (2007) stresses that language occurs as a consequence of use and of pragmatic reasons rather than linguistic. Language becomes an individual tool for thought and a medium for understanding the connection between “the world” and “me”, but not a random tool where the child determines the conditions. It is the adults care-giving, the adults use of language, and the adults ethical guidance, that makes language a bearer of culture and a key that fits both the social space and the cultural codes (Tomasello, 2009).

The hypothesis is: the small child decodes contextual meaning, and thereby understands words.

The two concepts of are, so to speak, standing back to back, facing the world in opposite directions. The linguistic position focusing on grammatical content and

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

6

Page 7: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

expression, claiming that language are representations, free of human intelligence and psychological abilities, independent of culture, context and linguistic interaction. To underline this position, Chomsky (1965, p. 4) describes the human ability to use language as an autonomic, innate ability, build on universal grammar, syntax, semantic, phonology and morphology. He calls the mental ability of language for a competence,3 and he calls the child’s use of language for performance. On the one hand is the structure that belongs outside the language - performance, such as skills and cleverness, learned in the social environment, and on the other hand are the structures that belong inside the language, the linguistic code and the underlying subconscious competence. The last category is the primary linguistic object (Karrebæk, 2008, p. 15). Language acquisition is thereby a question of accountability and acquiring the linguistic grammatical codes to use in interactions

This linguistic concept is opposed by a so called context-sensitive concept, where it is the ”We” or shared intentionality, that is emphasized as the basis of language (Tomasello, 1986). Human cooperative communication is in this field based on individuals engaging in forms of collaborative activity, and joint goals, joint intentions, mutual knowledge and shared beliefs, which all are elements in the context of various cooperative motives and language occurs as intra-active interference. Language thereby becomes more than words and grammar. The central difference between the two concepts can be narrowed down to:

The infant possesses an innate universal grammar as an instinct (Chomsky, 2006; Pinker, 1994, pp. 4-5)

and …The infant is biologically prepared and thereby has the potential capacity to learn language (Tomasello, 2009).

The functional linguistic focus on universal internal processes and the absence of a socio-cultural angle have, in recent years, been a target for criticism. One of the main claims is that there is no attention towards the infant’s ability to empathise; a point which Chomsky shares with Piaget, who describes the infant as:

… only concerned with himself, and ignoring more or less completely the points of view of others (Piaget, 1929, p. 188). (Lindahl, 1998, p. 16).

Criticism of this view is stated in postmodern psychology (among others Sommer, 2006; Stern, 1998; Tomasello, 1986; Trevarthen, 1980), where one of the

3 It is not competence as in the usual Danish interpretation, where competence means that the child is aware of her ability, and is able to put them in use (perform). For Chomsky, competence is a non-conscious ability.

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

7

Page 8: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

cornerstones is the infant’s ability to communicate, imitate and possess a sense for empathy, and thereby obtain a sense of self and identity; of individuality, and thus – a sense of togetherness. That feeling is initiated by the infant’s ability to separate “me” from “us”. So a postmodernist will claim, that it is the infant’s ability to distance itself as an individual, that facilitates a sense of sociability, and thereby feel a need for communication. And the post-humanistic philosopher and PhD in Quantum physics Karen Barad (2007) emphasizes a scientific need to understand these ongoing dialectics between individuality and sociability, between materialism (biology) and discourse (culture), in order to understand that learning occurs as simultaneous processes in humans and between humans in an ongoing intra-active process, (Barad, 2007, p. 227).

The material and the discursive are mutually implicated in the dynamics of intra‐activity and enfolding. Material and discursive constraints and exclusions are similarly entangled. (Barad, 2007, p. 244)

Intra-action moves beyond the way in which materialism shapes, or influences the socially constructed world. Intra-activity is an ongoing dynamism that configures and reconfigures relations of space, time and matter – where matter must be understood as both significance and substance (Barad, 2007, p. 181), which underlines the point that children are not observers of the world, using language with an universal structure. Neither are they simply located at particular places in the world; they are rather active or agency parts of the world in its ongoing intra-activity (Barad, 2007, p. 184).

Hence according to Barad, it is in the on-going intra-activity, that the child becomes a being, or as Barad puts it, “becomes matter”. But then I must explain the concept of intra-action – to give an impression of how they ”become matter”, how different spatial systems intra-act in the child. And to do so, I need to delve into the difference between Albert Einstein and Niels Bohr’s scientific understandings and their discussions about objectivity.

Einstein, like many before and after him, speaks of the real world as the external world. And he does so in a way that assumes a separation between the observed and the observer. In other words, spatial separation ensures ontological separability; and if two systems are spatially separated by so much as an infinitesimal spatial interval they always possess separately determinate states. Hence in Einstein’s way of thinking, the spatial separation of observer and observed consequently ensures objectivity.

Bohr regarded the external world as an illusion. There is no external and there is no internal – there is “the world”, and we are all part of this world. For Bohr, the so-called instantaneous communication between spatially separated systems is

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

8

Page 9: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

explained by the fact that these allegedly separated states are not really separate at all, but rather “parts” of the same phenomenon – the world itself. Furthermore, for Bohr, representational research is therefore inadequate and a result of specific spatial cuts enacted by the scientific arrangement, with a possibility of different conclusions. In Bohr’s account objectivity is therefore a matter of immediate communication (Barad, 2007, pp. 173-174).

So for Einstein, science and research are spatial systems interacting as external separated systems. For Bohr this is not possible; for him, science and research are spatial systems, connected in instantaneous communication, influencing one another because they are connected as different individual parts of the same phenomenon. They are instantaneously interacting. So …

Interactions are spatially defined entities that interact as externally separate entities. E.g.: child and crèche, child and pedagogue, child and child, and so on.

Intra-actions are spatially defined entities that interact in time, space and matter, as spatial entities of the same phenomenon, which by their intra-action become someone new or something else – or just – become something. And space, time and matter are mutually constituted through the dynamics of iterative (on-going) intra-activity (Barad, 2007, p. 181). E.g.: The child becomes someone else when entering the crèche: in fact every time she enters the crèche! The spatial intra-acting systems produce matter, and become materialized in the child.

Concerning the language war, this implies the following setup:

Biological sculpturing

I think the world is precisely what gets lost in doctrines of representation and scientific objectivity. (Donna Haraway, “The Promises of Monsters”) (Barad, 2007, p. 137).

Linguistic representationalism takes the notion of external separation as its foundation. It separates the world into ontologically external domains of words and significance, leaving itself with the dilemma of their linkage. There has been the linguistic turn, the semiotic turn, the interpretative turn, the cultural turn: it seems that every turn has turned into a matter of language or some other form of cultural representation. And all these turns emphasize that language matters. Discourse matters. Culture matters. But the only thing that doesn’t seem to matter - is matter itself.(Barad, 2007, p. 132).

But how did language become more reliable than matter? Why are language and culture granted their own agency and historicity, while biology is figured as

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

9

Page 10: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

passive or at best something that inherits a potential? Is it not the common view of representationalism that language has a mediating function that displays matter as a passive and mute phenomenon in need of an external force like culture to complete it?

Barad’s (2007, p. 32) scientific discussion implies a recognition of the human role in biological practices. But also beyond this process she marks a refusal to take the distinction between human and nonhuman or biology and culture for granted and in order to enable an analysis of how these distinctions are produced, any research must avoid this nature-culture dichotomy. Hence …

I. Postmodern early childhood education is not to intervene from outside, but to intra-act from within, as a simultaneous part of the phenomenon that needs to be described (Barad, 2007, p. 174).

II. The fact that childhood’s materialization (ontology) through meaning (epistemology) is a basic term that can be named onto-epistemology (Barad, 2007, p. 185)

III. The fact that this is inseparable from normative values (ethics), so that we may talk about ethico-onto-epistemology, expressing the effort to unify everything that the representational science has parted, everything that in childhood constitutes how the child becomes matter (Barad, 2007, p. 185).

Childhood as an intra-acting concept of biology and culture is such an ethico-onto-epistemological complex that unifies the process whereby words become matter. The word “mother”, without its materiality in the consciousness of the child - is nothing.

As Barad lines up, biological dispositions intra-act with socio-cultural representations, skills and motivations of shared intentionality, and constitute what we may call agential realism, or maybe active communication.

So … first of all it is a presumption that children are biologically prepared for communication, and some of their first communicative attempts are gestures and eye gazes (among others: Murray & Trevarthen, 1985; Stern, 1998; Tomasello, 1986). It is the social-cognitive4 and the social-motivational5 capacity; and the cultural needs

4 Social-cognitive abilities refer to the fact that children at an early stage adapt a conscious insight in social relations. A growing ability to understand that other people are individuals with emotions and, foremost, with intentions. This insight occurs as a precursor to communicative skills. 5 Social-motivational capacity refers to the unconscious evolutionary desire humans have for sociability. The complex ability to cooperate and interact can be explained by various motivations for sociability. Children need meaningful and challenging social contact to develop, and they need to create strong social bonds, embracing others who take care of them; a strong desire for belonging, for being someone and for the little child, ultimately to survive (Bowlby, 1988;

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

10

Page 11: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

that enable early forms of communication, and these capacities become platforms on which linguistic communication can be built (Donald, 1991/1993; Stern, 1998, p. 43; Tomasello, 2008, p. 2 and 60; Tomasello, et al., 2009, p. 65).

Consequently it is the intra-action of biologically driven human need for attachment and sociability, that causes language, and with that in mind, we can precede with the biological sculpturing:

We are cultural mongers [salesmen], driven by the very nature of our awareness to seek refuge and solace in the community. (…) Symbolic thought is a by-product of this fact, and so is language (Donald, 2001, p. 253).

If the development of a cognitive brain and language was an evolutionary advantage, which enabled the human species to survive variations in the physical environment, then it must imply that…

… without the human cultural niche, and the skills and motivations for participating in it, a developing human child would not become a normally functioning person at all. Human beings are biologically adapted to grow and develop maturity within a cultural context (Tomasello, et al., 2009, pp. 106-107) [Italics by the author].

Language as a neural activity, which embraces symbolic representations of the environment, facilitates an extension of the mind in the sense that the world becomes more accessible to the child, and the child and its thinking becomes accessible to the world; in that manner the language becomes a social and cultural scaffolding for the child’s thoughts (Barad, 2007; Clark, 2008; Gopnik, 2009; Nelson, 2007).

This implies that the child becomes biological shaped in the image of the contextual culture. And by shaped, it means that the child’s brain and body actually are sculptured after the culture (Donald, 1991/1993), the child becomes matter. And in that process language seems to have several functions. First of all it is through language that culture is shared, it is through language that the brain is stimulated and it is language that opens the child for the world, and the world for the child (Klafki, 1997).

The syntactically determined cognitive reach of the child begins as a growing subset of its biologically determined cognitive reach. Then, two things happen: the syntactically determined cognitive reach extends beyond the biological boundaries and a new component of cognitive reach is created like an expanding ring around the biologically determined component. This new component shares some features with

Tomasello, Dweck, Silk, Skyrms, & Spelke, 2009).

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

11

Page 12: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

each of the other components. Like the genetically determined component, much of this component remains beyond the reach of language.

So it is through language that we can attain insight in cultures in a way that transcend the natural bodily boundaries, and at the same time this new insight materializes biologically. Language provides the child with the ability to obtain thought and intentions from others, and to communicate thought and intentions to others (Klafki, 1997). Thereby language becomes a manifestation of our biological cohesion. Language points out reality for the child and gives it a sense of both togetherness and diversity, language is thereby the driving force in the intra-active sculpturing of human biology, and thereby the cultural transformation of a human being.

So, when Tomasello (2009), states that the infant is biologically prepared and thereby has the capacity to learn language, he is in fact pointing out the very basic human competence that enables human cognition. And at the same time he places language acquisition in the socio-cultural field, pointing out that: Children’s language will emerge as a consequence of the child’s social, cultural and physical environment; the child decodes contextual meaning and words thereby materialize.

Part two – The role of the pedagogue

Proto-Language – the communicative crux

Why is it that some children talk well by the time they enter preschool, whilst others lag behind? One reason probably is the fact that children’s different home environments facilitate different language, and children from language-poor home environments will develop a poor language. A second reason could be that language acquisition requires a caregiver’s emphatic presence, which can be difficult, when the child must compete with 11 peers for the necessary adult attention. A third reason could be, that preverbal language is not, contrary to verbal language, a common language with common codes. It is rather a private language developed between mother and child, you might call it a private proto-language.

Proto-language is the first communication between the primary caregiver (mother) and the child, developed whilst the child was breast-feeding, in nurturing situations, during storytelling and other intimate interactions. Protolanguage is an intimate communicative structure which, due to the strong bond of attachment and motherly care, provides the child’s first communicative experiences.

Perhaps the first intersubjective experiences are the sounds heard in the womb, the mother’s voice, her hand on the stomach as she talks to the unborn child. Perhaps the first biologically conditioned social activity - the sucking reflex is what kickstarts

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

12

Page 13: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

sociability and the feeling of belonging, both of which stimulate communication. Others are crying, smiling, eye-contact; action and reaction where the mother responds with smiles, care and comfort. It is possible that such communicative logic, created through this and much other daily social practice, build a structure for all further communication. One can perhaps go so far as to say that the child utilises these individualised structures each time it communicates. It is possible that these structures form a proto-language which in this interpretation is the ”point of communication” for the child.

And if we, in usage-based language understanding, consider language as the socio-cultural variable in all facets, then children from strong linguistic backgrounds will have a qualitatively better relationship to pædagogues than children from homes where emotional communication with the child is less. There are children who have experienced more care combined with interactive communicative contact than others who have been more left to their own devices or have experienced much less care or a lesser degree of linguistic communication in the relationship. So the usage-based proto-language is individual and therefore children’s communicative potential on starting in the crèche can vary greatly.

This means that there is talk of two essential points: 1) That the child’s proto-language is individual and developed in a close relation between mother and child. Thus the mother is the only one who “speaks” the child’s ”language”, and 2) that the quality of that “language” is socio-culturally variable, which thus gives children a variety of linguistic potential.

Even if this is only an approximation of the truth, the next question is how can a pedagogue learn this individual language? How can they interpret a poorly developed proto-language? And how can it be transformed into a universal method which can be professionalised and used in practice?

An intelligent guess would be that this is scarcely possible. Unique for communication with a little child is that the person caring for it must respond immediately. It is of little help if the pedagogue first has to consult a colleague or analyse a video-recording to be able to interpret the child’s wishes. The communicative child needs an immediate response. With just a few seconds delay in responding, the child’s focus has moved on to something else. One viewpoint is suggested by Taguchi (2010, p. 65) who, inspired by Regio Emilia and Karen Barad, writes that “in an intra-active pedagogy the emphasis is on an interdependent and mutual listening and observing that expands the focus from merely dealing with the intra- and inter-personal relationship (…) to be inclusive of the performative agency”, but Taguchi continues, “we (…) lack a language and concepts to use in order to make visible or actualise the intra-active processes in between organisms, objects, matter and things.” Her point is my point; pedagogy is understood to deal with internal

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

13

Page 14: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

cognitive processes in the individual child, or emerging through an encounter with another child, a teacher or a pedagogue who knows what to learn.

Perhaps, with this in mind, one should recognise that the key to the problem is that the child communicates contextually and that we, as humans, possess senses which are intuitively capable of receiving the contextual signals which appear and , not least, the communicative signals which our conscious observance overlooks. So the pedagogue has to use his own intuition and interpret the contextual impressions with his professional ability. This, however, implies several problems. One is that people are different; we all have our own cultural inheritance, which produces different values and thus different intuitive experiences; we only see what is visible for within our own individual horizon; the rest is invisible. The combination of intuition and interpretation will furthermore result in what is more or less a qualified guess! Admittedly a guess from the context, but nevertheless a guess. And yet this may be a useable method even though its starting-point demands the ability to see events in the child’s perspective. First and foremost it requires sensitivity, observation and contextual empathy. So to facilitate an adequate usage-based language acquisition, the pedagogue needs to be aware of the child’s perspective, of the child’s thoughts and the child’s individual way of communicating, of the child’s proto-linguistic style and the child’s intra-active contextual being.

The child’s perspective

Halldén (2003) separates the two concepts, childperspective (in one word) and child’s perspective (in two words). Childperspective concerns conditions for childhood, such as political decisions, socio-economic conditions etc. Child’s perspective is the desire to see and hear the world through the eyes and the ears of the child. This perspective is an individual perspective that requires knowledge about the complexity of the individual child, its background, its language abilities, its way of communicating and its contextual intentions. It is a very difficult task. And the younger the child is, the harder it will be for an adult to comprehend the child’s intentions.

Taking the child’s perspective demands first and foremost observation, empathy, humility and a sensitivity to the child’s means of expression (Lindahl, 2002, p. 27; Tomasello, 1986, 2003, 2009); an attempt to understand movements in the child’s consciousness here and now, when it responds to different situations, but also to be aware of each individual child’s background, how it slept the preceding night, how it reacted to being left by its parents in the morning and so on (Åberg & Taguchi, 2007), in other words giving the child observant care.

Being observant in this situation refers to the fact that the little child learns everything contextually; what is happening now has a meaning for the child. From

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

14

Page 15: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

birth, the child’s brain is phylogenetically developed so that it immediately a carer’s intentions by observing her deliberate actions. When a carer reaches out a hand, the child will do the same. When the child smiles, the carer do the same and are filled with warmth and happiness. The carer can immediately feel the same emotions as the child by observing facial expression, listening to oral expression and watching body language. And when they talk together, they also listen in silence and unconsciously register changing reactions to what is said, by observing micromovements in the face, direction of sight, tone of voice and sounds. These changes express what is being thought and felt. We experience each other as if we were in the other’s body. There is an immediately open empathic channel for feelings to and from each other, and we take part in their reasoning and experience just as they take part in ours (Nelson, 2007; Stern, 2004). Thus our minds are not independent, separate, or isolated. We are not alone in our state of mind. The border between child and carer are physically defined; they experience themselves as separate subjects, which at the same time, is the reason that they can meet as two individuals. But the borders can be overrun by intentions, emotions and thoughts, which are constantly changing in a dialogue with other’s intentions, emotions and thoughts, even if the dialogue is only with oneself. The child’s mental life develops in intra-activity, and the concept of a psychological singularity or individually developed psychological phenomena is not possible in the postmodern psychology of development. In fact postmodern psychology according to Bohr and Barad, says that psychological development takes place in a dialogue with the whole of society, with culture, as in a dynamic melting-pot of development-psychology. We have channels open to all those we are in contact with. Interactivity is not just a relation between two people, but between all the members of a society and, ultimately, the whole world. (Stern, 2004).

Childhood is always contextualized in relation to time, place and culture, and is therefore never a natural or universal childhood. And children are recognized as social agents with a voice of their own, and should be seen and heard in a democratic dialogue of understanding for childhood (Broström, 2006b; Dahlberg, et al., 2007, p. 49).

Intuition and interpretation

Intuition is the direct understanding of a situation, as an uncensored experienced background. It is not guessing or mysterious impressions, but a holistic understanding, which enables humans to make expert decisions (Benner, 1995, p. 197).

Dreyfus og Benner, who both follow thinkers such as Heidegger, Merleau-Ponty og Wittgenstein, propound that rationalism is not the optimal working method. Dreyfus

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

15

Page 16: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

bases this theory on Merleau-Ponty’s thesis that it is our ability to adopt flexible ”styles” and behaviour which creates a basis for perception and understanding and not our ability to comprehend rules (Dreyfus, Dreyfus, & Athanasiou, 1986, p. 5).

Once a decision intuitively presents itself, rationalization in the first sense describes the attempt to find a valid explanation by identifying the elements of the situation and combining those elements by a decision rule to justify the chosen decision (Dreyfus, et al., 1986, p. 34).

The point is that the pedagogue can benefit by using his intuition, which, in relation to preverbal children, has the quality that intuitive feelings are both contextual and based on all his professional experience as a pedagogue. Intuition has furthermore the innate quality that it cannot be false. Cohn (1968) describes three aspects of the human impression of reality:

1) Perception: Based on the senses and previous experience. One links up what one sees and what one knows.

2) Deduction: Based on perception and the linking up of experiences.One links up what one sees, what one knows and what has happened or will happen.

3) Intuition: Based on unproven end-games which provide an answer. It is built on the knowledge and experiences of the whole being, linked up to the senses. Thus intuition is not verbal assumptions, verbal understanding or cognitive images.

None of the three statements contain an inherent truth. This means that the pedagogue who wants to understand a child’s body language must resort to contextual interpretations. And interpretations, like intuition, build on previous experiences with communicative actions. But they also build on contextual logic. For example, if one-year-old Freddy was frightened of a horse on a trip out with his crèche, then his pedagog knows what he is “talking about” when the child points to a picture of a horse. A stranger, on the other hand might well believe that Freddy likes horses. In that way contextual interpretation can be a decisive method of understanding the preverbal child. This relates to Barad’s idea of moving beyond how materiality 'shapes', or 'influences' the social constructed world (Barad, 2007). Intuition and interpretation are an important part of all human relations. But as suggested earlier it involves great uncertainty and elements which are very difficult to professionalise.

However, research has been made in methods of avoiding intuitive interpretation, or at least of putting them into a system. According to Tomasello, the ability to communicate intuitively with the little child has a lot to do with reading gestures. Toddlers do a lot of gesturing; pointing, waving bye-bye, raising their arms to be

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

16

Page 17: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

picked up, and an American research project indicates that the more the small child gestures, the better its vocabulary will be (Iverson & Goldin-Meadow, 1998). And inspired by M. Merleau Ponty, the preverbal child’s body-language is a means of taking part in the infant’s daily life, early experiences and intentional actions (among others: Løkken, 2005; Tomasello, 2003). With that in mind, Tomasello (2003), underlines that the infant’s intentional language starts out with gestures. And he has isolated three main types of gestures:

1. Ritualizations are gestures in which the infant simply employs an effective procedure for getting something done. For example many infants learn to request being picked up, by raising their arms over their head, while approaching an adult. The act is not symbolic, but learned directly by doing so. When the child reaches for the adult’s arm, or grasps the adult’s finger, it engages in an activity designed to bring about the desired physical result. The adult understands what the infant wants and responds immediately. On future occasions when the adult sees the infant approach and prepares for this same activity, her arms begin to go up, and thus she responds appropriately to these very first signs of intentional movements. The infant for its part, notices that as soon as it raises its arms, the adult responds, and so she learns that just the initial part of the sequence is sufficient to obtain the desired result, not only physically, but also socially and communicatively through adult assistance. The process does not involve cultural codes or imitative learning of any sort; it does not involve shared communicative symbols. Ritualized gestures are thus not symbolic, because the gestures are only a way to achieve some concrete result, using a behaviour that originally was designed to be physically effective (Tomasello, 2003, p. 33).

2. Deitic 6 gestures: When the gesture is deitic, there is usually a third object or person involved. Normally this gesture requires joint understanding and contextual knowledge. The infant pointing at an apple, requires the presence of an adult who knows what the infant means. Maybe the child is pointing to induce the adult to share attention, because the child is hungry, or maybe the child wants to play with the apple or maybe the infant just points with an index finger to orient its own attention to the apple. The appropriate adult response will be to follow the finger, and get the apple. So once again, the child understands and learns that gesture is a procedure for getting things done, and not necessarily an invitation to share attention, using a mutually understood communicative symbol. The alternative is that the infant sees an adult pointing and comprehends that the adult is trying to share attention. And the infant learns that when it does the same, the adult responds as desired. Thereby they have made an intersubjective symbol act for sharing attention.

6 Deitic refers to the phenomenon that the meaning of an utterance requires contextual meaning.

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

17

Page 18: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

3. Symbolic gestures are iconic gestures. For example raising arms to imitate an aeroplane, blowing at hot things, leaning one’s head and smiling at something pleasant. According to Tomasello, the infant learns most of such symbolic gestures, by imitating the adult. That is to say; they are learning to point symbolically via imitative learning or use of linguistic symbols: by first understanding an adult’s communicative intention in using the gesture and then engaging in “role reversal imitation” to use the gesture herself – when she has the same communicative intention (Tomasello, 2003, p. 34).

The fact that gestures run on a scale from symbolic to non-symbolic, and emerge alongside the first linguistic skills, implies that children’s ability to communicate symbolically is not tied specifically to language, but rather emanates from the more fundamental set of social-cognitive and social motivational skills. And as a consequence, gestures seem to pave the way for early language at least in a functional way. So although infants can vocalize soon after birth, it is primary gestures that seem to carry the one-year-old’s communicative intentions, and thereby pave the way for language. And the evolutionary benefit is the mastering and inheritance of culture.

When the child reaches the age of 8 to 10 months, it begins intentionally to use the adult as a social reference point, and to use objects in the same way as adults (imitating). This behaviour is triadic, in the sense that it involves infants coordinating their interactions with both objects and other humans (Barad, 2007; Stern, 1998, 2004; Tomasello, 2003). Such behaviour would seem to indicate an emerging understanding of other people as intentional agents, with goals and the ability to make choices to obtain those goals. Such interactions require, beside intuition and attention, some kind of joint attention. And according to Tomasello (2003), the ability to learn language, depends on the ability to create zones of joint attention:

Frames of Joint attention

A zone of joint attention is defined intentionally, which means, that the child and adults’ common understanding of focused interactions and activities outlines the zone. The child’s ability to decode and understand these communicative intentions is actually language. Joint attention becomes an intersubjective zone of interactivity, in which the child can monitor the communicative intentions of the adult and at the same time understand not only the adults’ role but also its own role in the interaction from the same “outside” perspective. This will be the foundation of being able to imitate communicative structures and cultural patterns, a phenomenon described by Tomasello as: “Role reversal imitation” (Tomasello, 2003).

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

18

Page 19: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

Three elements are in play:1. The framework is the zone of joint attention, the space, the time, the activity

and the interaction. 2. To understand each other’s communicative intentions, implicates

intersubjectivity in different forms and the ability to observe the interaction “from within”.

3. Cultural learning through “role reversal imitation”, in which the child imitates the intentional actions of the adult. It is to be perceived as simple, mechanical imitation. The child must learn to use a communicative symbol directed to the adult, in the same way as the adult used it on the child. This means that the child has not only to imagine itself in the position of the adult, but also make the adult imagine himself in the position of the child. This is according to Stern’s concept, “intersubjectivity”:- ‘I know that you know that I know’ (Stern, 1998). The result of the process can be a linguistic symbol or a communicative tool; intersubjectivity understood by both parties. The process also ensures that the child understands that she has acquired a symbol that is culturally accepted and socially dividable in the sense that it knows that the listener both understands it and is able to produce the same symbol.

As argued by Tomasello (2003, p. 22), joint attention first and foremost implicates the active intentional participation of the participants. A participation that has a meaningful purpose and content. In this perspective, joint attention is much more than just eye contact or gesticulating, or being aware of the same object; each party has to be aware of the other’s awareness and each party has to be aware of the other’s intention.

Attention is a biologically based competency, which for example shows when a child focuses on an object in the immediate environment (Bauer, 2006; Bråten, 2007, 2009; Bråten & Trevarthen, 2007; Hansen, 2002/2005; McDonald, 2006; Merleau-Ponty, 1984).

Attention can occur in two situations:1. Passive (unconscious) attention: When something sudden occurs, for example

a loud noise, attention will unconsciously be apparent in the child. 2. Active (conscious) attention: The child is a part of an intentionally planned

process, which involves intersubjectivity. For example to have a story read out loud, in which the joint attention can be the story. To achieve an active, joint attention, the child must attract the attention of someone else – this happens in the zone of joint attention. Joint attention is thus most often associated with a situation in which two individuals look at each other or at the same object.

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

19

Page 20: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

In the cases below, role reversal imitation is not discussed.a. Passive attention: To look at the same object at the same time, attracted for

example by a loud noise.b. Random joint attention: At the same time and independently discovering a

problem or something which is different. Two children look at a box of balls. All the balls are blue except for one, which is pink. The attention of both children is attracted to the pink ball.

c. To follow another’s gaze: A child follows the gaze of an older child or an adult expecting that this often leads to something new and exciting. But it has nothing to do with joint attention.

d. Coordinated attention to an object: Both children see the same toy and are aware that the other sees it. Immediately it appears as joint attention, but one of the children wants to play with the toy, the other child just notices the toy because it is pink.

Active attention occurs when a child is involved in an intentional, relational process. In order to realize this, the child focuses selectively, in the sense that the attention is selectively, intentionally directed (Tomasello, 1986).The only way that the pedagogue can decode the selective intention of the child is to observe its gesticulation, behaviour and attention and then imagine (associate) what the behaviour and attention could mean; that is to say, what intentions the child has. This imagining can be either a cognitive decoding or an affective decoding (balancing) (Stern, 1998).

The pedagogue must be aware of at least four pre-stages of joint attention:1. To discover attention. The child must be capable of perceiving the attention of

the others. This can involve following the other’s gestures and gaze. 2. To underline attention. The child must be capable of gesticulating or maybe

even verbalizing the attentive behaviour in itself and the adult.3. Social coordination. The child must be capable of participating in coordinated

interactions with other children. This involves mastering social competencies such as waiting for your turn, role-division and ritualized games.

4. Intentional standpoint. The child must see itself and others as intentional participants. It must understand that other people can have intentions differing from its own. By presuming that others’ behaviour is focused towards an end, they can interpret and predict that behaviour.

Part three – The emotional, meaningful and ethical dialogue

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

20

Page 21: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

In the nineties, the element of teaching was toned down in the pedagogic structure in the crèche and the common pedagogic approach became more a relational and growth oriented approach. Michelsen, Edsberg and Posborg (1992) assess this approach to be of a positive nature albeit underlining the lack of challenging content of learning. Diderichsen, Thyssen & Jacobi (1991) furthermore argue that the child in the caring approach must be presented to content which can aid the child in exploring the world. Also Andersen & Kampmann (1988) argue that the pedagogue must support the child in both exploring and creating its own culture, just as Broström does in multiple writings (among others 2006a, 2006b, 2006c; Broström & Vejleskov, 2009). The number of children in the crèche rose quickly until the year 2000, in which most regions had a guaranteed placing, and in 1999, 55% of the 0 to 2-year-olds were being taken care of in crèches. In 2007, this number increased to 92%, which equals 13.339 children. Following this development, came the 1998 Social Services Act, which, for the first time ever, put the aims of crèches into legislation. Aims which like those of kindergartens, were to provide the children with care and furthermore support them in acquiring and developing social and cultural skills as well as contributing to the stimulation of the child’s fantasy, creativeness and linguistic development. Keeping in thread with the reform-pedagogic tradition, it ensured the child sufficient space to play and learn and also for physical expression, togetherness and the opportunity to explore the environment (Retsinformation, 2007).

In 2004, the Act on Educational Curricula was put into effect, stating that each crèche must work out a plan for all the children affected, providing space to play, to learn and to develop. Thus, the word “learning” was introduced to the crèche.

Along with the daycare legislation from 2007, the intentions have changed and learning will be introduced as an element to prevent exclusion:

§1. The purpose of this law is to:1) Promote the wellbeing, development and learning of children (…)2) Prevent negative social inheritance and exclusion (…)

(Retsinformation, 2007).

Because there are only very few new investigations of the crèche’s pedagogical praxis (Nordenbo, 2010), it is not possible safely to state anything qualified about whether the previously mentioned research is still relevant today. But, in praxis, it can appear as if the crèche is still affected by a relational and growth-oriented approach: caregiving, carework and professional care (Davies, 1998) as compensation for the absence of the parents, which in everyday life can have a tendency to become custodial watching over and babysitting – or as described by Hundeide (2004/2006); show a character of external caring. This type of utilized

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

21

Page 22: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

caring does not contain incitement for joint attention, joint focus of interest or joint commitment in challenging the world. In a meaningful and transcending context, the pedagogue will follow the attention of the child; the caregiver will experience an empathic approach to the child; make use of his sensitivity in order to understand the child from within and at the same time challenge the child; respond to the child’s initiative in exploration and answer the child’s curiosity regarding the environment and the symbolic world. Thus the interplay becomes providing – and curricular.

Care as a part of the discourse of relational pedagogics in today’s crèche can prove to have a tendency to repress learning (Broström, 2006a) and especially linguistically challenged children will suffer from the lack of challenge (Craig, 1994; Reynolds, 2009). In the same way, a specific linguistic focus can suppress care, providing the same result.

In other words, a balanced complex of the three elements, care, upbringing and learning, become coherent pedagogic elements, which open the child for culture and vice versa. It becomes a pedagogic foundation, which means that the well-being of the child is in greater focus, along with the child’s increased opportunity to learn and thereby an increased opportunity to evolve. One could state that the interactions between child and pedagogue are based on care, upbringing and development and that, put together with the interactions observed among the children, as well as the individual effect of the child and the active pedagogic environment, will facilitate the child’s well-being, its learning and its development. For the small child in the crèche, this means that the care, the nearness and the one-on-one contact, together with the opportunity for multimodal decoding, will bring the child into a state of learning. The learning-objective of the crèche for the child’s language acquisition could thus be to create the correct interactive conditions for the language of the child.

Care - as a frame for intersubjectivity

There is no convincing evidence that young children necessarily suffer harm or that their relationship with their mother is inevitably undermined if care is shared (Dahlberg, et al., 2007, p. 47)

Care is the upmost important ingredient in the planned approach of the crèche. Care is characterized by one person’s attention on another persons physical, mental and emotional needs (Davies, 1998, p. 126). In that manner, care, as a professional act, will to some degree provide a replacement for the small child’s mother, and give the child a sense of attachment. Besides that, care will contain key attitudes in the language acquisition process, such as imitation, empathy and guidance. And as the Dahlberg quotation above indicates, there is no evidence that children in day care

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

22

Page 23: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

suffer any harm, if care is shared. The important word here, is IF. There is evidence that if care is not shared, the child will suffer physically, mentally and emotionally; will not develop, will not learn language (Bernth, 1972; Spitz & Cobliner, 1965).

The foundation for care in a phenomenological perspective is an unbreakable relation between man and his surrounding world as well as intersubjectivity: In interaction with others we meet each other as two parts of the same piece. We enter each other’s living-space and confirm as well as correct each other. We understand each other from reactions: another person’s body language, facial expression, gesticulation and words – all of which come together as a whole – an expression for a world we can relate to. In that way, thoughts, senses, physicality, emotions and ways to relate to others and to the world – become a whole.

The caregiver’s attention is directed against and sensitive towards the one who is in need of care. At the same time, the recipient’s reactions and needs are determining for which way the care-relation will progress. So it is about “seeing” the other’s perspective while, in the relation, trying to expand the other’s competency (Noddings, 1986).The relations of care make use of three dialectic forms of dialog:

1) The emotional dialog – in which the adult spontaneously follows the initiative of the child (affective balancing).

2) The meaningful and transcending dialog – in which the pedagogue is a fellow traveller in the child’s discoveries of its own world – the adult mentions what he sees and follows the child’s exploration.

3) The boundary-defining and ethical dialog – in which the pedagogue supports, guides and provides the child with help to move on. Not negative, judgemental help, but a positive and constructive form for help.

Here, the part about care is obvious: The pedagogue is engaged in the world of the child – and at the same time there is a pedagogic strategy directed towards expanding the competencies of the child. The pedagogue becomes the challenger of the learning of the child, the moral guide of the child and in that process, the caregiver. Focus does not become a static picture of how the child is, but more an interactive process where the child and the caregiver can enter a still-altering process of change. The pedagogue and child meet in the child’s sphere of reference. This meeting requires tenderness, emotional presence – and coherence.

Upbringing – a frame for ethical culturation

Children’s upbringing is a topic very many people have an opinion on. According to Baumrind (1989, p. 349), children’s upbringing can be structured in two different methods: The upbringing can be characterized by a demand dimension and a

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

23

Page 24: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

responsive dimension. The demand dimension shows that parents either have high ambitions and high norms for their children or at the other extreme, avoid making demands at all. The responsive dimension shows parents that have great acceptance of and empathy for the children, and parents who are uninterested or even rejecting towards their children. These categories can be narrowed down to four different styles of upbringing: Authoritative; Authoritarian; Allowing or Non-involving. The authoritative make demands, but are also responsive. This implies that the styles are democratic and rational, due to the fact that both parents’ and children’s rights are respected. Research (Baumrind, 1989) implies that children in this category have a passion for life, an ability for cooperation, are self-confident and open to new challenges. These children can resist socially destructive actions. The parents make development demands and are obvious and clear in making limits for behaviour. But at the same time they possess consideration for the child and invite them to participate in the family’s decision-making (Lindahl, 2002, p. 29).

Such an approach meets criticism at a time when children are often confronted by a greater degree of self-determination or non-upbringing (Ziehe & Stubenrauch, 2008); an independence which to a certain extent excludes the challenges of the parents. Within the family, this will not necessarily have consequences for the child. But in the crèche, where the child is obliged to be part of a social community, it can prove a considerable challenge for a child who has never learned how. This can result in the child being disadvantaged as far as learning is concerned. Broström (2002) uses the metaphor to ”bring in” and introduce the child to and involve the child in culture, rather than to ”bring up.” By this he means that the child must, by non-authoritarian means. This point is supported by William Damon (1990, p. 146):

If (…) we want democratic citizens, we should provide for them relationships in which they can argue, and freely make choices (…) This does not mean that schools should pretend to be value-neutral; or that teachers and ministers should refrain from clear moral [and ethical] instruction and explanation. Children need adult guidance. They also need to develop morally responsible habits.

The overall goal with upbringing must therefore be to help the children to ethical acceptable reasoning autonomously. And doing this by participation and guidance. No indoctrination will prepare children for the many diverse situations which they will have to face. The child must learn to find the ethic issue in an ambiguous situation, to apply basic moral values to unfamiliar problems, and to create ethic solutions when there is no one around to give the child direction. The only way to master these key challenges is to develop an autonomous ability to interpret, manage and understand ethical problems.

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

24

Page 25: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

This does not mean that ethical aware adults should practice value neutrality. In fact displays of such neutrality from professionals have the opposite effect than intended (Damon, 1990).

Concluding remarks

Learning for the child eight to fourteen month of age, is as mentioned several places in the paper, not something that occurs without intentions and efforts. It is a process that needs an individualized combination of care, upbringing and content, and a child perspective that implies humility and responsiveness for different enterpretations of the child’s individual expressions. A direct authoritative language-education will for the small child become an obstacle for the small child’s language acquisition.

Imitation is a very central element in the small child’s learning process (among others Bauer, 2006; Bråten, 2009; Myowa-Yamakoshi & Tomonaga, 2009; Vygotsky, 1978); imitation is for the small child also a way to attain a sense of self as well as a crucial mechanism in communication. Imitation involves the combination of perception and action, visual attention and motivation; to observe how others do and then trying to do the same. The small child possess such an automatic competence (Decety & Meyer, 2009), and in the linguistic approach it is the unconscious early imitation, that is the overall important element of learning.

The ability to imitation is an aspect of the brains resonance system, which is connected to an area in the brain that facilitate ideas and notions of actions (Bråten, 2007). When a child sees a caregiver perform an action e.g. a movement with the mouth, the activity in the area of the brain of the child becomes active in the same areas as in the brain of the caregiver, that perform the movements. The child’s brain “practice” thereby in performing the movements that comes with the word from the caregiver. When the caregiver says “food” the child’s brain do not simply process the word, but also reproduce the neural activity of the mouth movements, the lips tension and so on, a drill that later on helps the child shape the mouth cavity in order to create the correct acoustic space and thereby perform the right sound to the right word to the right meaning. The brains ability to mirror movements combines sensory perception with movements and meaning, and thereby help the child to read the intention from the caregiver. It is not enough that someone opens her mouth and says a word in front of the child, it need to be a known caregiver, and she need to connect a communicative intention to the sound in order to awake the child’s mirror system. The mechanism helps as mentioned the child to understand the meaning of the caregiver’s action, and thereby the caregiver’s mouth movement and the word comes to represent a goal in the child’s consciousness, “Food” = Bodily contentment = Emotional satisfaction.

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

25

Page 26: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

One of the reasons why the child can read, comprehend and respond emotional corresponding to what other experience and feel, or should experience and feel in the same situation, is the ability to share emotions; a unconscious ability to mirror another persons emotional and bodily status. A process that entail the child due to internalized, imitative representations can imagine how the other persons intentions are at the present moment.

To imagine, that is to take the other persons perspective, involves cognitive competences, based on own experiences, presence and multi modal reading. The small child’s experience of intersubjectivity, takes place due to facial expressions, mimic, gazes and glances that reveal emotions and thereby intentions and attention. The ability to receive and interpret such emotional cues is important all life through. In the early childhood are the caregivers emotional display used to regulate behaviour and not least to underline the importance of vocalizations. Bodily expressions and emotional intention are two sides of the same phenomenon, side by side explaining the message. In other words it is a multimodal system that via empathy and the phenomenon of cerebral resonance, creates the ability to connect an emotion to facial mimic, to a voice, to a meaning of a word.

Facial mimic causes besides reading of lips, emotional procedures in the brain, an activity that amplify the experience for the speaker as well as the listener. Mimic communicates both emotional messages, but are as well movements that entail semantic decoding.

Educative structures for the small child involves thereby not just word, but also paralinguistic messages such as mimic, emotional prosody, which contain educational informations that in connection with not pronounced words, as grunt, accentuations and laughter, plays a vital role in mediation of intentions. The educational content for the small child is development of ability to integrate visual and vocal cues.

Learning as an element, hereby becomes a question about the quality of the modal interactions with the little child. To share attention and to facilitate role reversal imitation. To invite the small child to combine her self and the social world, learning to imitate its codes and cues. And to facilitate symbolic linguistic gesturing. The child learns the zone of joint attention is intentional defined, and the child must learn to monitor the linguistic intentions in the zone. This will give the pedagogue the opportunity to challenge the child.

Appendix: Preliminary data concerning the character of the usage-based dialogue between the child and the caregiver.

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

26

Page 27: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

Method

Eleven typically developing infants six males and five females, all from middle class monolingual Danish speaking families. The children are observed longitudinally between the ages of 10 to 16 month. I focus here on sessions between the onset of gesture speech (range: 10-14 month) and the emergence of one-word speech.

Procedure

The infants are videotaped monthly. On average, each child is observed 10 minutes, replicated six times over six month. The observations take place in their crèche, during ordinary activity, planned by the pedagogues.

Coding

I focus on gestures used in dialogue. The child has to make an effort to direct the listener’s attention through eye-gaze, vocalisation, for behaviour to be considered as communication. Such behaviour could be gesture on its own or gestures and speech. Each gesture is classified into one of three categories: Ritualized gestures, Deitic gestures or symbolic gestures.

Reliability

Accordance between three independent peer coders will at the end of the project be assessed for 10% of the sessions. 100% agreement between the coders must occur in at least 80% of all observations.

My Preliminary findings are

The observed children only use symbolic gestures in the crèche in less then 10% of all observations.

Final remarks

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

27

Page 28: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

Bodily gesturing is not merely a neural activity, but serve as an embodied signal to the caregiver that the child is ready for a particular kind of verbal input – a cognitive extension so to speak. The gesture is the bodily expression of the child’s thoughts. And the dynamic loops through which mind and world interact are a symbolic activity that runs from brain through body and world and back again and actually constitutes cognition. And symbolic gestures, is the embodiment of the child’s imaginative mind, of her wannabe thoughts, and extends her mind in the process. That kind of gesturing opposes the ritualized gestures. Consider a child who points at his or hers father saying “Dada” – the child’s caregiver might respond by saying: “Yes, that’s father” – translating the child’s gesture and offers word combinations in a sentence. The child imitates, read lips, eye-gazes, tone of voice and so on. In my video recordings, I observe many such contextual ritualized gestures translated into sentences, but very little symbolic gesturing. Why is that? It could imply a lack of attention from the pedagogues on the symbolic language, maybe because such symbolic gesturing is not part of the common culture of custodial care in the crèche, but may be seen as a less important form of dialogue.

The consequence might be that part of the children’s cognitive development is on hold while they attend the crèche. You might even say that when the children’s dialogue with the caregivers, is in a deprived language, using ritualized commands. This means that children becomes limited in extending their potential sharing their ideas, their dreams their humour and their thoughts, instead they express their contextual needs in ritualized manifestations.

The connection between the development of the cognitive brain and the symbolic language are well documented (Carpenter, Nagell, & Tomasello, 1998). We know that it is the symbolic language that mediates the ability to think, it is the process that is supersizing the mind, and we know that the human species is the only one, capable of this process (Tomasello, 2009; Tomasello, Dweck, Silk, Skyrms, & Spelke, 2009). This might not present a problem for the children from linguistic well-stimulating homes. But the children from linguistic deprived homes will be lost, with insufficient linguistic stimulation.

So, when I in my paper, states that the infant is biologically prepared and thereby has the capacity to use symbolic language, I am in fact pointing out the very basic human competence that enables human cognition. And at the same time I place language acquisition in the socio-cultural field, pointing out that: Children’s language and cognition will emerge as a consequence of the child’s social, cultural and physical environment. Which requires attention from the caregiver to symbolic gesturing

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

28

Page 29: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

dialogue with the infant. A huge responsibility for the professional caregivers. The question is weather or not they can embrace that responsibility.

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

29

Page 30: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

References

Andersen, P., & Kampmann, J. (1988). Vuggestuen hverdag og utopi. København: Socialpædagogisk bibliotek, Munksgaard.

Barad, K. (2007). Meeting the universe halfway. Quantum Physics and the Entaglement of Matter and Meaning. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

Bauer, J. (2006). Hvorfor jeg føler det, du føler (1. udgave ed.). Valby: Borgen.Baumrind, D. (1989). Rearing competent children. In W. Damon (Ed.), Child development today

and tommorow. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers.Benner, P. (1995). Fra novice til ekspert. Viborg: Munksgaard.Bernth, I. (1972). Institutionsbørn og hjemmebørn. København: Munksgaard.Bleses, D. (2007). Tidlig kommunikativ udvikling. Odense: Syddansk Universitetsforlag.Bleses, D., & Højen, A. (2009). Når børn lærer sprog. Odense: Syddansk Universitetsforlag.Bowlby, J. (1988). A Secure Base. London: Routhledge.Broström, S. (2002). Opdragelse, undervisning og omsorg. In S. Henriksen & A. D. Bentzen (Eds.),

Hvis er barnet. København: Forlaget børn og unge.Broström, S. (2006a). Care and Education: Towards a New Paradigm in Early Childhood Education.

35, 391-409Broström, S. (2006b). Children's Perspectives on Their Childhood Experiences. In J. E. J. Wagner

(Ed.), Nordic Childhoods and Early Education. USA: IAP Inc.Broström, S. (2006c). Pædagogik i daginstitutionen med henblik på udvikling af børns

handlekompetence. [Kbh.]: Danmarks Pædagogiske Universitet.Broström, S., & Vejleskov, H. (2009). Didaktik i børnehaven (1. udgave ed.). Frederikshavn: Dafolo.Bruner, J. S., & Watson, R. (1983). Child's talk. New York: Norton.Bråten, S. (2007). On Being Moved - From Mirror Neurons to Empathy. Amsterdam/Philadelphia:

John Benjamins Publishing Company.Bråten, S. (2009). The intersubjective mirror in infant learning and evolution of speech. Amsterdam

Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Bråten, S., & Trevarthen, C. (2007). From infant intersubjectivity and participant movements to

stimulation and conversation in culktural common sense. In S. Bråten (Ed.), On Being Moved From Mirror Neurons to Empathy. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.

Burman, E. (2008). Deconstructing developmental psychology (2. ed.). London: Routledge.Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge: The Massachusetts Institute of

Technology.Chomsky, N. (1975). Reflections on language. London: Temple Smith.Chomsky, N. (2006). Language and Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Cohn, R. (1968). Training intuition. In H. Otto & J. Mann (Eds.), Ways of growth. New York:

Grossman.Craig, C. F. R. (1994). Effects of Early Intervention on Intellectual and Academic Achievement: a

Follow-up Study of Children from Low Income Families. Child Development, 65(2), 684-698.Dahlberg, G., Moss, P., & Pence, A. R. (2007). Beyond quality in early childhood education and care

(2. ed.). London: Routledge.

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

30

Page 31: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

Damon, W. (1990). The moral child (Paperback ed.). New York: Free Press.Davies, C. (1998). Caregiving, carework and professional care. In A. B. J. W. J. K. S. Peace (Ed.), Care

Matters. London: Sage Publications.Decety, J., & Meyer, M. (2009). Imitation as a Stepping Stone to Empathy. In M. d. Haan & M. R.

Gunnar (Eds.), Handbook of Developmental Social Neuroscience. New York: The Guilford Press.

Diderichsen, A., Thyssen, S., & Jacobi, A. (1991). Omsorg og Udvikling. København: Danmarks Pædagogiske Institut.

Donald, M. (1991/1993). Origins of a modern mind. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.

Donald, M. (2001). A mind so rare.Dreyfus, H. L., Dreyfus, S. E., & Athanasiou, T. (1986). Mind over machine. Oxford: Blackwell.Gopnik, A. (2009). The Philosophical Baby - What Children's Minds Tell Us about Thruth, Love & the

Meaning of Life London: The Random House.Halldén, G. (2003). Barnperspektiv som ideologisk eller metodologisk begrepp. Pedagogisk

forskning i Sverige, 8(1-2), 12-23.Hansen, M. (2002/2005). Børn og opmærksomhed. København: Gyldendal.Hundeide, K. (2004/2006). Relationsarbejde i institution og skole. Frederikshavn: Dafolo.Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1998). The nature and functions of gesture in children's

communication. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Karrebæk, M. S. (2008). At blive et børnehavebarn. København: Det humanistiske fakultet,

Københavns universitet.Klafki, W. (1997). Kritisk-konstruktiv didaktik. In M. Uljens (Ed.), Didaktik. Lund: Studentlitteratur.Lindahl, M. (1998). Lärande småbarn. Lund: Studentlitteratur.Lindahl, M. (2002). Vårda - vägleda - lära. Göteborg: Acta Universitatis Gothoburgensis.Lindahl, M., & Pramling Samuelsson, I. (2002). Et barneperspektiv (1. udgave ed.). [Kbh.]:

Gyldendal Uddannelse.Løkken, G. (2005). Toddlerkultur. København: Hans Reitzels Forlag.McDonald, J. (2006). Hearing Lips and Seeing Voices: Illustration and Serendipity in Auditory -

Visual Perception Research. In J. Atkinson & M. Crowe (Eds.), Interdisciplinary Research. Diverse Approaches in Science, Technology, Helath and Society. Sussex: Wiley.

Merleau-Ponty, M. (1984). Consciousness and the Acquisition of Language. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

Michelsen, V., Edsberg, P., & Posborg, R. (1992). Vuggestuekultur. København: Børn & Unge.Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulation of interactions between two-month-olds

and their mothers. In T. M. Field & N. A. Fox (Eds.), Social Perception in Infants (pp. 177-197). Norwood NJ.: Ablex Publishers.

Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., & Tomonaga, M. (2009). Evolutionary Origins of Social Communication. In M. d. Haan & M. R. Gunnar (Eds.), Handbook of Developmental Social Neuroscience. New York: The Guilford Press.

Nelson, K. (1985). Making sense. Orlando [Fla.]: Academic Press.Nelson, K. (2007). Young minds in social worlds. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Noddings, N. (1986). Caring A Feminie Approach to Etics & Moral Education. Berkeley: University

of California Press.

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

31

Page 32: A Usage Based Language Acquisition in the Danish Crèche · Web viewAbstract: In curriculum research concerning language acquisition in the Danish crèche In 2007, 122,000 children

DANISH SCHOOL OF EDUCATION, AARHUS UNIVERSITY - RESEARCH UNIT CHILDHOOD, LEARNING AND CURRICULUM THEORY

Nordenbo, S. E. (2010). Forskningskortlægning og forskervurdering af skandinavisk forskning i året 2008 i institutioner for de 0-6 årige (førskolen). Dansk Clearinghouse for Uddannelsesforskning.

Piaget, J. (1929). The child's conception of the world. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York: William Morrow.Retsinformation. (2007). Dagtilbudsloven. from https://www.retsinformation.dk/Reynolds, A. L. M. J. T. (2009). Do Early Childhood Interventions Prevent Child Maltreatment?

Journal of Early Childhood Research, 14(2), 182-206.Sommer, D. (2006). Barndomspsykologi. København: Hans Reitzels Forlag.Spitz, R. A., & Cobliner, W. G. (1965). The first year of life. New York: International Universities

Press.Stern, D. N. (1998). The interpersonal world of the infant. London: Karnac Books.Stern, D. N. (2004). The present moment in psychotherapy and everyday life. N.Y.: W.W. Norton.Taguchi, H. L. (2010). Going Beyond the Theory/Practice Divide in Early Childhood Education

Introducing an intra-active pedagogy. New York: Routhledge.Tomasello, M. (1986). Joint Attention and Early Language. Child development, 1454-1463.Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Tomasello, M. (2009). The usage-based theory of language acquisition In E. L. Bavin (Ed.), The

Cambridge Handbook of Child Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Tomasello, M., Dweck, C., Silk, J., Skyrms, B., & Spelke, E. (2009). Why We Coorperate. Cambridge,

Massachusett: MIT Press.Tomasello, M., & Todd, J. (1983). Joint attention and lexical acquisition style. First Language,

4(197), 197-212.Trevarthen, C. (1980). The Foundations of Intersubjectivity: Development of Interpersonal and

Cooperative Understanding in Infants. In D. R. Olson (Ed.), The Social Foundations of Language and Thought Essays in Honor of Jerome S. Bruner. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.

UNESCO. (1989). Convention of the child's rights. from www.unesco.org/education/pdf/CHILD_E.PDF

Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and Language. Cambridge: The M.I.T. Press.Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in Society. Harvard: Harvard University Press.Wallin, K., Mæchel, I., & Barsotti, A. (1981). Ett barn har hundra språk. Stockholm:

Utbildningsradion.Ziehe, T., & Stubenrauch, H. (2008). Ny ungdom og usædvanlige læreprocesser (2. udgave ed.).

Kbh.: Politisk Revy.Åberg, A., & Taguchi, H. L. (2007). Lytningens pædagogik. Vejle: Kroghs Forlag.

Ole Henrik HansenMA (ed). PhD FellowAarhus University

32