37
A usage-based approach to grammatical development Holger Diessel University of Jena holger.diessel @uni-jena.de http://holger-diessel.de/

A usage-based approach to grammatical development Holger Diessel University of Jena [email protected]

  • View
    218

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

A usage-based approach to grammatical development

Holger Diessel

University of Jena

[email protected]

http://holger-diessel.de/

Language learning

• Connectionism (Rumelhart & McClelland 1986;

Elman et al. 1996; Lewis and Elman 2001).

• Corpus studies of the ambient language (Redington

et al. 1998; Mintz et al. 2002; Monaghan et al. 2005).

• Experimental studies with infants (Saffran et al. 1996;

Saffran 2002; Newport and Aslin 2004).

Construction grammar

Form

Meaning

Form

Meaning

Form

MeaningForm

Meaning

Form

Meaning

One-word utterances / holophrases

Daddy. [Adam 1;4]

Mommy. [Adam 1;4]

Doggy. [Adam 1;5]

Milk. [Adam 1;5]

Allgone. [Adam 1;6]

Take this key off .

Take this paper off.

Take that off .

Take this dress off.

Take that belt off me.

Take it off.

More corn.

More cookies.

More mail.

More popsicle.

More jump.

More Peter water.

Block get-it.

Phone get-it.

Mama get-it.

Bottle get-it.

Towel get-it.

Books get-it.

Lexically-specific constructions

Emergence of schematic constructions

Get doggy Get milk Get him Get Billy

Get __

VERB __

Hypotheses

• Relative clauses form a network of related

constructions that children acquire in a piecemeal,

bottom-up fashion.

• The development originates from relative constructions

that are only little different from simple sentences.

• The development can be seen as an example of

‘abductive constructivist learning’.

Study 1 (Diessel 2004)

Age range Finite Nonfinite

AdamSarahNinaPeterNaomi

2;3-4;102;3-5;11;11-3;41;9-3;21;8-3;3

1783262258

12036714416

1;9-5;1 305 287

Head of the relative clause

(1) The man who we saw was reading a book. SUBJ

(2) He noticed the man who was reading a book. OBJ

(3) He saw to the man who was reading a book. OBL

(4) The man who was reading a book. NP

(5) That’s the man who was reading a book. PN

Head of relative clause (total)

48,5

23,821,5

5,6

0,70

10

20

30

40

50

60

PN NP OBJ OBL SUBJ

prop

orti

ons

Head of relative clause (earliest)

80

2,57,5

10

00

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

PN NP OBJ OBL SUBJ

prop

orti

on

Head of relative clause (development)

PN

OBJ

NP

OBL

OBL

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

3;0 4;0 5;0

age

pro

po

rtio

n

PN

OBJ

NP

OBL

SUBJ

Input frequency

PN-relatives are among the most frequent relative

clauses in the ambient language, but they are not as

frequent in the ambient language as in the children’s

data.

Semantic complexity

(1) Here’s the tiger that’s gonna scare him.

> The tiger is gonna scare him.

(2) This is the sugar that goes in there.

> The sugar goes in there.

(3) It’s something that you eat.

> You eat something.

Semantic complexity

(1) You left this toy I’m playing with.

> You left this toy. + I’m playing with the toy.

Information structure

The information structure of PN-relative constructions

is similar to the information structure of simple

sentences, i.e. they do not include presupposed

information.

Pragmatic function

PN-relatives are pragmaticlly very useful in parent-

child speech: They occur in constructions that focus

the hearer’s attention on elements in the surrounding

situation.

Conclusion

PN-relatives are the earliest relative clauses that

children learn because:

(1) they suit the communicative needs of

young children

(2) they are semantically similar to simple

sentences.

Syntactic amalgams

(1) That’s doggy turn around. [Nina 1;11]

(2) That’s a turtle swim. [Nina 2;2]

(3) Here’s a mouse go sleep. [Nina 2;3]

(4) That’s the roof go on that home. [Nina 2;4]

(5) That’s the rabbit fall off. [Nina 2;4]

Relativizsed syntactic role

(1) The man who met the woman. subj

(2) The man who the woman met. obj

(3) The man who the woman talked to. obl

(4) The man who the girl gave the book to.io

(5) The man whose dog bit the woman. gen

Relativized syntactic role (total)

57,3

37

5,7

0 00

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

subj obj obl io gen

pro

po

rtio

n

Relativized syntactic role (development)

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

>3;0 3;0-4;0 4;0-5;0

age

pro

po

rtio

n obj

subj

obl

Diessel & Tomasello (2005)

Das ist der Mann, der mich gestern gesehen hat.Das ist der Mann, den ich gestern gesehen habe.Das ist der Mann, dem ich das Buch gegeben habe.Das ist der Mann, mit dem ich gesprochen habe.Das ist der Mann, dessen Hund mich gebissen hat.

subjdoiooblgen

This is the girl who saw Peter on the bus this morning.This is the girl who the boy teased at school yesterday.This is the girl who Peter borrowed a football from.This is the girl who Peter played with in the garden.This is teh girl whose horse Peter heard on the farm.

subjdoiooblgen

Results

71,1

40,5

31 31,5

2,40

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

subj do io obl gen

subj vs. do p =. 001

do vs. io p = .173

Do vs. obl p = .169

68,5

32,8

21,4

12

0,50

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

subj do io obl gen

subj vs. do p =. 001

do vs. io p = .061

io vs. obl p = .001

English German

Subj-relatives

Do-, io-, and obl-relatives were often converted to subj- relatives.

English

ITEM: This is the girl who the boy teased at school.

CHILD: This is the girl that teased … the boy … at school.

German

ITEM: Da ist der Mann, den das Mädchen im Stall gesehen hat.

CHILD: Da ist der Mann, der das Mädchen im Stall gesehen hat.

Subj-relatives

However, children were not consistent in their performance.

In addition, they often repaired their conversion errors before they reached the end of the sentence:

(1) This is the girl who bor/ Peter borrowed a football from.

(2) Da ist der Junge, der/ dem Paul … die Mütze weggenommen hat.

Hypothesis

The conversion errors are due to the fact that subj-

relatives are more easily activated than other types of

relative clauses.

Frequency and ease of activation

The more frequently a grammatical construction occurs, the more deeply entrenched it is in mental grammar, and the easier it is to activate in language use.

Input frequency

35.6

53.8

7.7

0 00

10

20

30

40

50

60

SUBJ OBJ OBL IO GEN

(Diessel 2004)

Subj-relatives and simple sentences

AGENT VERB PATIENT. Simple clause

PRO is AGENT rel VERB PATIENT. subj

PRO is PATIENT rel AGENT VERB. do / io / obl

Children’s good performance on subject relatives can be

explained in terms of the similarity between subject

relatives and simple sentences.

Word order in English relative clauses

NP [V …] subj

NP [NP V …] do

NP [NP V …] io

NP [NP V …] obl

NP [[GEN N] V …] gen

Relative pronouns in German relative clauses

Der Mann, der … subj

Der Mann, den … do

Der Mann, dem … io

Der Mann, mit/von dem … obl

Der Mann, dessen N gen

Gen- and io-relatives

Both gen- and io-relatives are basically absent from the

ambient language.

Io-relatives caused fewer errors than gen-relatives

because they are similar to do-relatives.

Summary

Important is the similarity between constructions:

• Subj-relatives caused few problems because they are similar to simple sentences.

• English do-, io-, and obl-relatives caused basically the same amount of problems because they have the same word order.

• Io-relatives caused relatively few problems because they are similar to direct do-relatives.

• Gen-relatives and German obl-relatives caused great problems because they are dissimilar to other relative clauses.

Why does similarity matter?

Relative clauses are constructions (i.e. form-function pairings) that are related to each other in a network like lexical expressions.

Children acquire this network in a piecemeal, bottom-up fashion by relating new relative clause constructions to constructions they already know.

A network of relative constructions

Simple Sentences

That is N [subj-relative]

…-relatives

…-relatives

…-relatives

… [gen-relative]

References

Diessel, Holger. 2004. The Acquisition of Complex Sentences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Diessel, Holger & Tomasello, Michael. (2005). A new look at the acquisition of relative clauses. Language.