85
A THEORY OF SIGNAL DETECTION BASED UPON HYPOTHESIS ANALYSIS Item Type text; Dissertation-Reproduction (electronic) Authors Fobes, James L. Publisher The University of Arizona. Rights Copyright © is held by the author. Digital access to this material is made possible by the University Libraries, University of Arizona. Further transmission, reproduction or presentation (such as public display or performance) of protected items is prohibited except with permission of the author. Download date 25/06/2021 23:53:44 Link to Item http://hdl.handle.net/10150/282911

A THEORY OF SIGNAL DETECTION BASED UPON ......75-19,590 FOBES, James Lewis, 1946-A THEORY OF SIGNAL DETECTION BASED UPON HYPOTHESIS ANALYSIS. The University of Arizona, Ph.D., 1975

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    3

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • A THEORY OF SIGNAL DETECTIONBASED UPON HYPOTHESIS ANALYSIS

    Item Type text; Dissertation-Reproduction (electronic)

    Authors Fobes, James L.

    Publisher The University of Arizona.

    Rights Copyright © is held by the author. Digital access to this materialis made possible by the University Libraries, University of Arizona.Further transmission, reproduction or presentation (such aspublic display or performance) of protected items is prohibitedexcept with permission of the author.

    Download date 25/06/2021 23:53:44

    Link to Item http://hdl.handle.net/10150/282911

    http://hdl.handle.net/10150/282911

  • INFORMATION TO USERS

    This material was produced from a microfilm copy of the original document. While the most advanced technological means to photograph and reproduce this document have been used, the quality is heavily dependent upon the quality of the original

    submitted.

    The following explanation of techniques is provided to help you understand

    markings or patterns which may appear on this reproduction.

    1. The sign or "target" for pages apparently lacking from the document photographed is "Missing Page(s)". if it was possible to obtain the missing page(s) or section, they are spliced into the film along with adjacent pages. This may have necessitated cutting thru an image and duplicating adjacent

    pages to insure you complete continuity.

    2. When an image on the film is obliterated with a large round black mark, it is an indication that the photographer suspected that the copy may have moved during exposure and thus cause a blurred image. You will find a

    good image of the page in the adjacent frame.

    3. When a map, drawing or chart, etc., was part of the material being photographed the photographer followed a definite method in "sectioning" the material. It is customary to begin photoing at the upper

    left hand corner of a large sheet and to continue photoing from left to right in equal sections with a small overlap. If necessary, sectioning is

    continued again — beginning below the first row and continuing on until

    complete.

    4. The majority of users indicate that the textual content is of greatest value, however, a somewhat higher quality reproduction could be made from "photographs" if essential to the understanding of the dissertation. Silver prints of "photographs" may be ordered at additional charge by writing the Order Department, giving the catalog number, title, author and specific pages you wish reproduced.

    5. PLEASE NOTE: Some pages may have indistinct print. Filmed as received.

    Xerox University Microfilms 300 North Zeeb Road Ann Arbor, Michigan 48106

  • 75-19,590

    FOBES, James Lewis, 1946-A THEORY OF SIGNAL DETECTION BASED UPON HYPOTHESIS ANALYSIS.

    The University of Arizona, Ph.D., 1975 Psychology, experimental

    Xerox University Microfilms , Ann Arbor, Michigan 48106

    THIS DISSERTATION HAS BEEN MICROFILMED EXACTLY AS RECEIVED.

  • A THEORY OF SIGNAL DETECTION BASED

    UPON HYPOTHESIS ANALYSIS

    by

    James Lewis Fobes

    A Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of the

    DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY

    In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements For the Degree of

    DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

    In the Graduate College

    THE UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA

    19 7 5

  • THE UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA

    GRADUATE COLLEGE

    I hereby recommend that this dissertation prepared under my

    direction by James Lewis Fobes .

    entitled A THEORY OF SIGNAL DETECTION BASED UPON

    "HYPOTHESIS ANALYSIS

    be accepted as fulfilling the dissertation requirement of the

    degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

    n ̂ f . pL, , H /1 A S~~ f Dissertation Directory Date

    \

    After inspection of the final copy of the dissertation, the

    follovring members of the Final Examination Committee concur in

    its approval and recommend its acceptance-.-'*

    Hh hf

    4/ ihf -y/g/76

    This approval and acceptance is contingent on the candidate's

    adequate performance and defense of this dissertation at the

    final oral examination. The inclusion of this sheet bound into

    the library copy of the dissertation is evidence of satisfactory

    performance at the final examination.

  • STATEMENT BY AUTHOR

    This dissertation has been submitted in partial fulfillment of requirements for an advanced degree at The University of Arizona and is deposited in the University Library to be made available to borrowers under rules of the Library.

    Brief quotations from this dissertation are allowable without special permission, provided that accurate acknowledgment of source is made. Requests for permission for extended quotation from or reproduction of this manuscript in whole or in part may be granted by the head of the major department or the Dean of the Graduate College when in his judgment the proposed use of the material is in the interests of scholarship. In all other instances, however, permission must be obtained from the author.

    SIGNED:

  • This dissertation is dedicated to my wife

    Jacqueline, and to my major professor James King, in

    appreciation of their continuous encouragement and support.

    iii

  • ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    I would like to acknowledge the contributions of:

    Dr. James E. King whose assistance was invaluable in all

    stages of this dissertation; other major commitee members

    Drs. Sigmund Hsiao and Ronald H. Pool; minor committee

    members Drs. Terry C. Daniel and Dennis L. Clark; Theresa

    Burton and Beth Wenzel who assisted in data collection;

    Evan Thomas who made conceptual contributions; Charles

    Davison who aided with equipment maintenance; and Robert

    Dylan who cautioned that nothing is revealed. This research

    was supported by Training Grant MH-112 86 from the United

    States Public Health Service.

    iv

  • TABLE OF CONTENTS

    Page

    LIST OF TABLES vi

    LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS viii

    ABSTRACT lx

    INTRODUCTION 1

    THEORETICAL CONCEPTUALIZATION AND ANALYTICAL

    PROCEDURE 9

    METHOD 25 Subjects . . . 25 Apparatus . , 25 Procedure 26

    RESULTS 29

    DISCUSSION 39

    APPENDIX A. FORMULAS FOR ESTIMATION OF HYPOTHESES . . 52

    APPENDIX B. FORMULAS FOR ESTIMATION OF K 61

    REFERENCES 67

    y

  • LIST OF TABLES

    Table Page

    1. Probability of Detection and Nondetection States for Stimuli Intersected with Attention and Nonattention States, as a Function of Procedure 11

    2. Probability of Response Outcome for Detection and Nondetection States as a Function of Procedure 11

    3. Probability of Response Outcome for Stimuli Intersected with Attention and Non-attention States, as a Function of Procedure . 12

    4. Characteristics of Hypotheses Involving Detection States , . . . . 14

    5. Characteristics of Hypotheses not Involving Detection States 16

    6. Thirty-Two Unique Problem Sequences 17

    7. Signal Level Presentation Order 28

    8. Percentage of Correct Responses as a Function of Procedure and Signal Level , 29

    9. Proportion of Variance Explained as a Function of Procedure and Signal Level , « , , 34

    10. Sensitivity as a Function of Procedure and Signal Level 35

    11. Probability of Detection and Nondetection States per Trial Given Attention, as a Function of Procedure and Stimulus 35

    12. Sensitivity Estimates Obtained with Two-Alternative Forced-Choice, and Values Predicted from Yes-No, as a Function of Signal Level 37

    vi

  • vii

    LIST OF TABLES—Continued

    Table Page

    13. Probability of Entering an Attention State per Trial, and of Zero, One, Two, and Three Attention States per Problem, as a Function o f Procedure a nd Signal Level . . . . 37

  • LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS

    Figure Page

    1. Proportional Strengths of Hypotheses Involving Detection States, as a Function of Procedure and Signal Level 30

    2. Proportional Strengths of Hypotheses not Involving Detection States, as a Function of Procedure and Signal Level 31

    viii

  • ABSTRACT

    This dissertation consisted of the description and

    ^application of a new theory of detection processes. The

    sensory portion featured detection of noise as well as

    signal stimuli and an absolute threshold for recognition of

    these stimuli. This recognition threshold for noise or

    signal was never exceeded by the other stimulus alone. It

    was also assumed that an observer either attended or failed

    to attend to the stimulation presented on each trial. When

    the observer entered an attention state, either a detection

    state or a nondetection state resulted. If a nonattention

    state occurred, a detection state was precluded.

    The theory's response portion assumed that the com

    bination of an attention and detection state resulted in

    correct responding, the combination of an attention and non-

    detection state resulted in random responding, and that a

    nonattention state resulted in nonrandom responding that was

    determined by a response bias. The false alarms that

    occurred were considered to be independent of the sensory

    mechanism and to have resulted from either random responding

    accompanying the combination of a nondetection and attention

    state or from a response bias.

    Therefore for the yes-no procedure model, an

    attention state in the presence of a signal resulted in

    ix

  • X

    either a detection of the signal with correct responding or

    a nondetection state accompanied by random responding.

    Likewise, when an attention state occurred in the presence

    of noise, the observer entered a detect noise state or a

    nondetect state. For the two-alternative forced-choice pro

    cedure model, an attention state resulted in either a detec

    tion of the difference between signal and noise with correct

    responding or a nondetection state accompanied by random

    responding. For both psychophysical procedures, a non-

    attention state always resulted in responding determined by

    a response bias.

    This theory was applied to data obtained by pre

    senting three-trial brightness discrimination problems of

    varying difficulty to capuchin monkeys. The outcomes of

    these problems were assumed to be combinations of states

    attention and detection, attention and nondetection, or non-

    attention that occurred on each trial of a problem. These

    states resulted in manifestations that reflected various

    modes of systematic responding which permitted estimation of

    their relative occurrence with an adaptation of the hypoth

    esis and analysis technique.

    This analytical procedure was used to determine the

    strength of three modes of detection and four sequentially

    dependent response biases. These values were then used to

    determine the sensitivity index that was expressed in terms

    of the probability of detection given attention. This

  • sensitivity index was considered to be independent of non-

    sensory response factors and its method of calculation

    included estimation of the probability of an attention state

    per trial as well as the probabilities of the various

    possible combinations of attention and nonattention states

    on three-trial problems.

    Support was provided for this theory which featured

    a variety of information that was not included in previous

    approaches. Specifically, a more detailed description of

    detection processes was provided that included estimation of

    the amount of responding due to specific nonsensory response

    biases. This conceptualization formed the basis for non-

    parametric estimation of sequential biases whose effects

    were isolated from the nonparametric sensitivity index.

    Further, the theory was not tied to a specific family of

    receiver operating characteristic curves. A concept of

    sensory thresholds was also included and a procedure was

    suggested for determination of absolute recognition

    thresholds that are not confounded with the effects of

    response bias.

  • INTRODUCTION

    Various classical psychophysical methods have

    afforded considerable information concerning the relation

    ship between perception and physical characteristics of

    stimulation (Guilford, 1954). Typically, however, the usage

    of these techniques has emphasized only the percentage of

    correct responses with each stimulus value. With such pro

    cedures, the reputed sensitivity of the subject, based upon

    this single variable, confounded detection with response

    biases. While variability and reduced discrimination

    accuracy due to nonsensory response biasing factors have

    been recognized for some time, attempts to deal with such

    biases have generally been inadequate, particularly so with

    single stimulus psychometric methods. Frequently, the

    attempt to control response biases consisted of either

    extended training of subjects or experimental designs such

    as counterbalancing (Engen, 1971).

    A common psychophysical approach to estimating

    sensitivity involved detection tasks that favored almost

    exclusive presentation of a stimulus to be detected, which

    was designated as signal (S), while trials containing only a

    stimulus designated as noise (N) were infrequently presented.

    When subjects incorrectly reported the presence of a S on

    the few catch trials containing only N that were presented,

    1

  • subjects were typically advised to pay increased attention.

    Such false alarms were typically not included in data

    analysis. A more recent procedure involved more frequent

    presentation of trials containing only a N stimulus and the

    proportion of false alarms on these trials was utilized in

    an attempt to obtain the proportion of detections corrected

    for guessing (Blackwell, 1953, 1963) . However, the pro

    cedure for correction resulted in sensitivity estimates that

    were not independent of response biases; the assumption of

    statistical independence between the proportion of detec

    tions corrected for guessing and false alarms was unjusti

    fied (Green and Swets, 1966; Swets, 1973).

    Techniques for the separation of certain response

    biases and the sensitivity estimate were considerably ad

    vanced by a theory of decision making based upon the trans

    lation of that portion of statistical decision theory

    dealing with situations in which a choice is made between

    two alternatives on the basis of an observed event (Smith

    and Wilson, 1953; Tanner and Swets, 1954). This approach

    conceptualized the detection process as a problem of S-to-N

    ratio (Tanner and Swets, 1954; Swets, Tanner, and Birdsall,

    1961; Green and Swets, 1966; Swets, 1973). When a sensory

    event occurred that was due to either N or to S, the observer

    was thought to choose between normal distributions of N and

    S along which the effects of stimulation continuously varied

    as a single dimension of increasing magnitude. Therefore,

  • 3

    the sensory effect of an observation was thought to arise,

    with a specific probability, from either the N or the S

    probability density function.

    It was further assumed that observers assigned con

    ditional probabilities that a particular sensory effect

    arose from N and that it arose from S. The continuum of

    sensory effects was thus characterized as a continuum of

    likelihood ratio values, each of which expressed the likeli

    hood that a particular observation arose from the S distri

    bution relative to the likelihood that it arose from the N

    distribution. Thus, on the basis of the likelihood ratios

    that varied in magnitude on the basis of their sensory

    effect, or some monotonic function of the ratios, observers

    were thought to decide which of two populations was sampled.

    The actual choice between populations was considered to be

    determined by a decision rule involving whether or not a

    particular observation's likelihood ratio value exceeded

    some fixed response criterion or response threshold value

    along the continuum of increasing likelihood ratio values.

    For example, if the likelihood ratio value of the sample was

    below the criterion likelihood ratio value, the subject

    indicated that the observation came from the N distribution;

    samples with corresponding likelihood ratio values above the

    critical value resulted in an indication that the observa

    tion was from the S distribution.

  • 4

    The d' index of S detectability or sensitivity was

    defined as the difference between the means of the N and S

    density functions, expressed in terms of standard deviation.

    Both this d1 sensitivity estimate and the response threshold

    value (3) were estimated with the conditional probability of

    saying S given (|) S and of saying S|N. For d1, a table of

    normal curve functions gave the z score corresponding to the

    conditional probability of saying S|N. This z value indi

    cated the distance along the dimension of sensory effects

    that the criterion was above the mean of the N density

    function. Similarly, the z score corresponding to the con

    ditional probability of saying s|s indicated the distance

    that the criterion was below the mean of the S density

    function. The value of d' equaled the combination of these

    two z scores and was computable regardless of the location

    of the criterion along the dimension of sensory effects; d'

    was independent of the location of the criterion. The

    measure 3 was the ratio of the ordinate of the S density

    function to the ordinate of the N density function, at the

    location of the response criterion. The numerator and de

    nominator in this ratio were estimated from the two condi

    tional probabilities (S|S and S|N) with a table of ordinates

    of the normal curve.

    Thus, d' was considered to be a function of de

    tectability and monotonically related to S strength. For a

    given S strength, the location of the variable decision

  • 5

    criterion along the continuum of likelihood ratio values has

    been found to be affected by such things as the information

    an observer has concerning relevant variables of the situa

    tion and situational goals. However, the method of deter

    mination of d' resulted in an estimate of sensitivity that

    was relatively unaffected by certain types of biases; d' was

    relatively independent of: (1) instructions (response

    criteria from "be careful" to "be careless," or simultaneous

    usage of multiple response criteria), (2) the a priori

    probability of a S being presented (expectations), (3) the

    values and costs associated with various decision outcomes

    (motivation), and (4) the psychophysical procedure used to

    estimate sensitivity (single stimulus or forced-choice).

    However, the generality of these findings has been ques

    tioned (Pike, 1973).

    In addition to these biases, various types of

    sequential response dependencies have been reported (Senders

    and Soward, 1952; Verplank, Collier, and Cotton, 1952;

    Howard and Bulmer, 1956; Speeth and Mathews, 1961;

    Carterette and Wyman, 1963; Kinchla, 1964; Parducci, 1964;

    Parducci and Sandusky, 1965; Haller, 1969; Baumstiler, 1970;

    Sandusky, 1971; Sandusky and Ahumada, 1971). The lack of

    response independence between trials can reasonably be

    expected to affect the assumed stability of the response

    criterion and accordingly the estimated value of d' (Pastore

    and Scheirer, 1974). While signal detection theory (SDT)

  • 6

    has provided for an estimate of sensitivity that was rela

    tively unaffected by certain response biases that affect the

    response criterion, it did not include a technique for

    dealing with sequential dependencies nor did it estimate the

    strength of specific response biases (Green and Swets, 1966).

    Further, this approach to detection systematically

    excluded a sensory threshold with a lower limit on sensi

    tivity. Thus, rather than assisting in the determination of

    sensory thresholds unaffected by response biases, some

    exponents of the SDT under discussion concluded that a

    determination of the level at which a threshold may possibly

    exist is neither critical nor useful. Such a view ignores

    the intent of a considerable body of literature.

    This dissertation consists of the description of a

    proposed theory of sensitivity as well as its application

    with two psychophysical procedures. The sensory portion of

    the theory includes detecting or not detecting N stimuli as

    well as S stimuli, and the occurrences of an attention (A)

    state is though to be accompanied by either a detection (D)

    state or a nondetection (D) state. If a nonattention (A)

    state is entered, a D state cannot occur. A sensory

    threshold for recognition of both N and S is assumed, with

    continuous gradation of sensation above the threshold and

    no gradation below. The recognition threshold for N or S

    is thought to be never exceeded by the other stimulus alone.

  • 7

    The response portion of the theory assumes that the

    intersection (A) of an A and D state results in correct

    responding, A A D states result in random responding, and a

    A state results in responding that is determined by a

    response bias. False alarms are considered to be independent

    of the sensory mechanism and result from either random re

    sponding accompanying A A D states or from a .A state with

    its ensuing response bias responding.

    As an illustration of the proposed theory, data from

    three-trial brightness discrimination problems, presented by

    two psychophysical procedures, were analyzed by a hypothesis

    (H) analysis technique similar to Levine's (1965) Method II.

    This analytical technique was used to determine the relative

    proportion of four response biases and three modes of D

    states. The strengths of these hypotheses (Hs), in combina

    tion with other values to be described, were then used to

    estimate the sensitivity parameter K, the probability of

    D|A. The calculation procedure to determine sensitivity

    also resulted in estimation of the probabilities of partic

    ular combinations of A, A, D, and D states on three-trial

    problems. Further, estimation of all measures was accom

    plished without assumptions concerning possible probability

    distributions of the sensory effects of S or N.

    An important advantage to the proposed approach

    resulted from the inclusion of sequential responding infor

    mation and nonsensory response .bias emerged as

  • 8

    multidimensional. These dimensions included: (a) the

    tendency to perseverate with yes (Y) or no (No) responses

    in yes-no (Y-N) tasks, or with a particular position in two-

    alternative forced-choice (2AFC) tasks, (b) the tendency to

    alternate responses, as well as (c) the win-stay:lose-shift

    and win-shift:lose-stay syndromes. These particular non-

    sensory factors are considered to be especially appropriate

    for subhuman observers. The type and amount of these

    response biases were specifically estimated and their

    effects were isolated from the sensitivity estimate. A

    technique is also suggested for estimation of sensory recog

    nition thresholds which are not confounded with the effects

    of response bias.

    The theory's empirically testable consequences that

    have been investigated indicate support for the theory.

    This is noteworthy in that the theory includes a high-

    threshold concept of absolute sensory recognition thresholds.

  • THEORETICAL CONCEPTUALIZATION AND ANALYTICAL PROCEDURE

    The proposed theory of sensitivity assumes that S as

    well as N stimuli may be detected or not detected, and a

    momentarily varying, absolute sensory threshold for the

    recognition of S or N stimuli is proposed. The recognition

    threshold for N and S stimuli is thought to feature con

    tinuous gradation of sensation above the threshold and no

    gradation below. The recognition threshold for S is thought

    to be never exceeded by N alone and the recognition threshold

    for N is thought to be never exceeded by S alone. Thus,

    observers can detect or not detect only that which is

    presented.

    The sensory portion of the theory also assumes that

    on each trial subjects enter a state of either A or A to

    stimulation. For the Y-N model, the occurrence of an A

    state in the presence of S is thought to result in either a

    Dg state or a D state. Likewise, the occurrence of an A

    state in the presence of N results in the subject entering

    state DN or state D. It is assumed that the probability

    (P) of (D IS A A) = P(D |N A A) = K,. In the model for ̂ IN -L

    2AFC, a D state is entered when the subject detects the

    difference between the S and N conditions, with the

    P(D|A) - ̂ 2' For both models, the occurrence of an A state

    9

  • 10

    precludes a D state. A proposed relationship between and

    K2 will be discussed later.

    These assumptions concerning the sensory portion of

    the theory are presented in Table 1 which depicts the

    probability of a D or D state on each trial given a

    particular stimulus condition A A or A states. The Y-N

    technique involves presentation of only one stimulus (S or

    N) per trial. Therefore, subjects may evidence Dg A A or

    D A A when S is presented and DN A A or D A A when N is

    presented. Conversely, the A state always results in a D

    state. The 2AFC procedure entails presentation of both

    stimuli (S and N) per trial and, with the spatial 2AFC used

    (King and Fobes, 1973), S occurred on either the right (R)

    or left (L).side. With this procedure, subjects may or may

    not enter a state of D relative to the difference between S

    and N. The A state in 2AFC also results in a D state.

    Table 2 depicts the response aspects of the theory.

    It is assumed that a D state always results in a correct

    response and that a D state always results in random

    responding. An A state is thought to always result in a D

    state that is accompanied by nonrandom responding which is

    determined by a response bias. For both D A A and A

    states, outcomes are uncorrelated with S and N and the

    probability of a correct response is one-half when the a

    priori probability of S is one half. False alarms (saying

    S|N) are considered to be independent of the sensory

  • 11

    Table 1. Probability of Detection and Nondetection States for Stimuli Intersected with Attention and Non-attention States, as a Function of Procedure

    Y-N 2AFC

    State State

    Event DS °N D Event Ddifference D

    S A A K1 0 1 - K1 s (L) A A K2 1 K2

    N A A 0 K1 1 - K1 s (R) A A k2 1 - K2

    S A A 0 0 1 s (L) A A 0 1

    N A A 0 0 1 s (R) A A 0 1

    Table 2. Probability of Response Outcome for Detection and Nondetection States as a Function of Procedure

    Y-N 2AFC

    State Correct Incorrect State Correct Incorrect

    DS 1 0 Ddifference 1 0

    dn 1 0

    D 1/2 1/2 D 1/2 1/2

  • 12

    mechanism and are assumed to arise from either random

    responding accompanying states D A A or from a response bias

    accompanying state A.

    Table 3 depicts the probabilities of response out

    comes for a particular stimulus A A as well as A. These

    probabilities are based upon the results of matrix multi

    plication of Table 1 times Table 2. When an A state occurs,

    a correct response results with probability (K + l)/2 and an

    incorrect response occurs with probability (1 - K)/2. Again,

    an A state always results in a D state with outcomes un-

    correlated with the presence or absence of S. The proba

    bility of a correct or incorrect response is one-half when

    the a priori probability of S equals one-half. It should

    also be noted that the values of K determined with the Y-N

    and 2AFC procedures will generally not be the same.

    Table 3. Probability of Response Outcome for Stimuli Intersected with Attention and Nonattention States, as a Function of Procedure

    Y-N 2AFC

    Outcomes

    Correct Incorrect

    S A A S (L) A A (K + l)/2 (1 - K)/2

    N A A S (R) A A (K + l)/2 (1 - K)/2

    S A A s (L) A A 1/2 1/2

    N A A S (R) A A 1/2 1/2

  • 13

    The results of three-trial problems are assumed to

    be combinations of states D A A, D A A, and A that occur on

    each trial. Further, these states result in manifestations

    that reflect various modes of systematic responding which

    can be referred to as Hs. The unfortunate nature of the

    term H with respect to unintentional connotations is

    acknowledged, but it is used in order to be consistent with

    Levine (1959, 1965). Thus, Hs are considered to be

    sequences of internal states that determine behavior and

    are manifested by a specificable pattern of responses to

    selected patterns of stimulation. As such, the H is <

    regarded as a dependent variable.

    The Hs that include at least one D state on a

    three-trial problem are listed in Table 4. If D states

    occur on all three trials of a problem, the only possible

    manifestation is three correct responses (+++), as indi

    cated for H A in Table 4. With a D state on exactly two

    trials, two correct responses result on these trials. The

    outcomes of H B will also include correct as well as

    incorrect responses (-) for the one trial on which states

    D A A or A occur. Therefore, outcomes to sequences involv

    ing two D states include: +++, ++-, +-+, and - + +.

    Likewise for H C, exactly one D state occurs and the out

    comes include correct and incorrect responses for the two

    trials on which states D A A or A occur. Possible

  • 14

    Table 4. Characteristics of Hypotheses Involving Detection States

    State H Sequence Manifestation

    A: 3 Ds D D D + + +

    B: 2 Ds D D DAA + + + or + + —

    or D D A.

    D DAA D + + + or + - +

    or DA D.

    DAA D D + + + or - + +

    or A D D

    C: 1 D D DAA DAA, + + + , + — + ,

    D A A , + + - or + - -

    D A DAA,

    or D DAA A.

    DAA D DAA, + + + / - + + ,

    A D A , + + - or - + -

    A D DAA,

    or DAA D A.

    DAA DAA D, + + + , — + + ,

    A A D, + - + or - - +

    A DAA D,

    or DAA A D

  • 15

    manifestations of C include all sequences except - - - ,

    since at least one correct response results with the single

    D state.

    The Hs that do not involve a D state on any of a

    problem's trials are depicted in Table 5. Response bias Hs

    are defined when an A state is entered on all three trials

    of a problem and the residual H is defined by all other

    sequences of A and Astates not involving D. The manifesta

    tions of these Hs are specified in terms of response

    sequences, where I is the type of response (Y or No) on the

    f irst trial of a problem and 0 is the other type of response.

    The results of three-trial problems are entirely

    specified by the stimulus, response, and outcome sequences

    depicted in.Table 6. The eight possible stimulus sequences

    can be grouped into symmetrical pairs, viz, SSS and NNN,

    SSN and NNS, SNS and NSN, as well as SNN and NSS. In

    Table 6 these pairs are represented by the sequences XXX,

    XXV, XVX, and XW, where X is the stimulus (S or N) on the

    f irst trial and-V is the other stimulus. The outcome of a

    particular stimulus sequence, in terms of being correct or

    incorrect, is determined by the response sequence (III, 110,

    101, or 100) evidenced. The lower case letters in each cell

    in Table 6 are the symbols that indicate the Hs that can

    result in each particular outcome given those stimulus and

    response sequences. The number in each cell will be used

    for reference purposes.

  • Table 5. Characteristics of Hypotheses not Involving Detection States

    H Definition State Sequence Manifestation

    D: Triple Response Repetition

    Type of Response Stays the Same for Three Consecutive Trials

    A A A I I I

    E: Double Response Repetition

    Type of Response Stays the Same for Two Consecutive Trials

    A A A I I 0 or I 0 0

    F: Win-Stay: Lose-Shift

    Response Type the Same as Preceding Rewarded Trial

    A A A I+I+I, I+I-O

    1-0+0 or I-O-I

    G: Win-Shift: Lose-Stay

    Response Type Opposite from Preceding Rewarded Trial

    A A A I+O+I, 1+0-0,

    I-I+O or I-I-I

    R: Residual Sequences Not Defined Above

    A DAA

    DAA DAA DAA,

    DAA, DAA A DAA,

    All Sequences

    r

    DAA DAA A, A A DAA,

    A DAA A or DAA A A

  • Table 6. Thirty-Two Unique Problem Sequences

    17

    Stimulus Sequence

    Response Sequence Stimulus Sequence I I I I I 0 I 0 I 10 0

    X X X 1) d g r

    2) c e

    - - + r

    3) - + c r

    - 4) — + + b 2c e f r

    5) + + + a 3b 3c d f r

    6) b 2c

    + + -e r

    7) + -b 2c r

    + 8) + - -c e g r

    X X V 9) - - + c d g r

    10) e r

    _ _ _ 11) - + b 2c r

    + 12) - + -c e f r

    13) + + -b 2c d f r

    14) a 3b r

    + + + , 3c e

    15) + -c r

    16) + - + b 2c e g r

    X V X 17) - + -c d r

    18) b 2c r

    - + + e g

    19) - -f r

    - 20) + c e r

    21) + - + b 2c d r

    22) c e

    + - -f r

    23) + + a 3b 3c r

    + g

    2 4 ) + + — b 2c e r

    X V V 25) - + + b 2c d r

    26) c e

    - + -g r

    27) - -c f r

    + 28) e r

    29) + - -c d r

    30) b 2c

    + - + e f

    31) + + -b 2c g r

    32) + + + a 3b 3c e

  • 18

    Table 6 indicates that a given response sequence is

    always consistent with more than a single H. For example,

    the response sequence I-I-I- is consistent with Hs d, g, and

    r in cell number one. Although a given response sequence is

    always consistent with more than one H, estimation of the

    strength of each H can be accomplished by a technique

    similar to Levine's (1965) Method II. However, the particu

    lar Hs presented here, as well as their solution equations,

    differ from Levine's. An additional important difference

    from Levine's procedure is the present use of weighting

    coefficients in Table 6 which reflect the l ikelihoods of

    different response sequences when one or two D states occur

    on a problem. As indicated for H B in Table 4, when two D

    states occur on a problem, either two or three correct out

    comes can result depending upon whether a correct response

    occurs on the trial involving states D A A or A. Therefore,

    the possible manifestations are: +++, ++-, +-+, and

    - + +. If all manifestations of two D states are assumed to

    be equally l ikely, then + + + is three times as l ikely to

    occur as any other manifestation of B. Therefore, b is

    assigned a coefficient of three in those cells containing

    + + + and assigned a coefficient of one in all cells con

    taining exactly two correct outcomes. The value of b is

    equal to the probability that H B occurs and is manifested

    by, for example, +•

  • 19

    Likewise for C, al l outcomes with exactly one D

    state are possible and include: + + +, + + - , + - +, - + +,

    - - + , + - - , a n d - + T h e + + + s e q u e n c e i s a s s i g n e d a

    coefficient of three since it is three times as l ikely as

    - - or + - given H C. Sequences ++-,+-+,

    and - + + are similarly assigned a coefficient of two since

    they are twice as l ikely as +, - + - , or + - - .

    The analysis of systematic patterns of response

    sequences begins with categorization of the frequencies

    with which each of the 32 unique sequences in Table 6

    occurs. The relationship between these resulting 32

    frequencies and the probability of each H is determined by

    the assumptions that: (a) the Hs are mutually exclusive,

    (b) their effects are additive, and (c) al l Hs whose

    probability exceeds zero are included. Summing across any

    row in Table 6 results in the statement

    a + 6b + 12c + 2d + 4e + 2f + 2g + 8r = 1, (1)

    where a = A, 6b = B, 12c = C, 2d = D, 4e = E, 2f = F, 2g =

    G, and 8r = R, Line (1) may also be stated as

    A + B + C + D + E + F + G + R = l , ( 2 )

    Therefore, each term in the two above equations may be

    interpreted as a probability or proportion with the terms in

    Equation (2) being the proportions of three*-trial sequences

    accounted for by the corresponding H.

  • 20

    The relative frequencies with which each of the 32

    three-trial sequences occur can be used to estimate the

    probabilit ies in Equations (1) and (2). For example, the

    theoretical probability of three incorrect outcomes given

    the stimulus sequence XXX (cell number one) equals the

    sum of the probabilit ies of the associated Hs since the Hs

    are assumed to be mutually exclusive and additive.

    P t(- - - |X X X) = d + g + r. (3)

    The theoretical probability of all 32 sequences in Table 6

    may similarly be expressed by a particular l inear combina

    tion of certain H probabilit ies.

    The obtained frequencies of each three-trial

    sequence may then be used to estimate the probability of

    each H. Continuing with the sequence from cell number one,

    P (- - - | X X X) = n(X- X- X-)/n(X X X), (4)

    where n(X-X-X-) is the frequence with which X~X*-X- occurs

    and n(X X X) is the frequency with which XXX occurs. To

    estimate the theoretical from the observed probability, i t

    follows from Equations (3) and (4) that

    d + g + r = n(X- X- X-)/n(X X X). (5)

    Levine's (1965) Method II may then be used to

    evaluate the probability of Hs in such a way as to minimize

    the sum of the squared differences between the theoretical

    and obtained cell frequencies (see Levine, 1959). While a

  • 21

    complete derivation of H analysis solution formulas is

    presented in Appendix A, a brief description of the pro

    cedure for obtaining solutions is i l lustrated by the solu

    tion of H A.

    From the 3 2 equations, those containing A include

    (.cell #5) a+3b + 3c + d + f + r = Q^,

    (cell #14) a+3b + 3c + e + r =

    (cell #23) a+3b+3c+g+r= , and

    (cell #32) a + 3b + 3c + e + r = (6)

    where each Qa is the observed proportion of the particular

    outcome sequence. It should be noted that in the present

    case each stimulus sequence was presented equally often.

    These equations in l ine (6) involving A totalled

    EQ = 4a + 12b + 12c +d+2e+f+g+ 4r. (7) a.

    Applying Equation CI) and solving for a = A results

    in

    2EQa = 8a + 24b + 24c + (1 - a - 6b - 12c) , ( .8)

    which reduces to

    2£Q - 1 = 7a + 18b + 12c a

    (9)

    which may be rewritten

    a = (2EQa - 18b - 12c - l) /7 CIO)

  • 22

    The solution in l ine (10) for A is thus found to be a

    function of Hs B and C. Applying Equation (1) and solving

    for B and C in an analogous manner results in

    b = (l /2EQb - a - 6c - l) /6 (11)

    and

    c = (2EQc - a - 6b - 7)/12. (12)

    These three l inear equations (10, 11, 12) with three

    unknowns may then be solved by Cramer's rule with these

    resulting unique solutions

    A = a = l /2ZQa - l/2IQb + l /2£Q c - 1, (13)

    B = 6b = 6(l/4£Qb - l/12IQa - 5/12EQ c - 1), (14)

    C = 12c = 12 (1/3EQ - 1/12XQ, - 1). (15) —~ C JD

    The solution for D is a function of the EQ^ as well

    as E. These two equations with two unknowns may be simul

    taneously solved with the unique solutions

    D = 2d = 2(l/4ZQd + l /8IQ e - 1/2), (16)

    and

    E = 4e = 4(3/16EQ e + l /8ZQd - 1/2). (17)

    The solutions for F and G are similarly a function

    of each other with the unique solutions

  • 23

    F = 2f = 2(3/16ZQ f + 1/16ZQ - 1/4) (18)

    and

    G = 2g = 2(3/16EQ + 1/16ZQ- - 1/4). y

    (19)

    Finally, from Equation (2), R is simply a residual,

    Once the H strengths are obtained, they may then be

    used to estimate the sensitivity parameter K, the P(D|A).

    This is accomplished by solution of the f ive equations

    l isted below which contain five unknowns (K, M^, t , and

    T ) . V a l u e s f o r A, B, C, D, E, F, G, and R are H strengths

    and the remaining terms are defined as:

    T = P (Ah A An + 1 A An + 2) , K = P (D | A) , M1 = P (A A A)

    + P (A A A) + P (A A A) , M2 = P (A A A) + P (A A A)

    + P (A A A) , t = P(An) , (J) = P(A|D) = (t - tK) / (1 - tK) ,

    0 = P(A|D) = (1 - t) /( l - tK) , hits = P(Y|S), and

    false alarms = P(Y|N).

    A = TK3 , (1)

    B = 3TK2(1 - K) + M-^K2 , (2)

    C = 3TK (1 - K) 2 + 2M ] ,K(1 - K) + M2K, (3)

    R = L = ( A + B + C + D + E + F + G ) . ( 2 0 )

  • 24

    T = A + B ( p + C ( p 2 + R{{l-{ [30(j)]/ l - [ (D + E + F + G) /

    ( D + E + F + G + R ) ] } } } , ( 4 )

    Hits - False Alarms = tK. (5)

    The derivation of these equations is contained in Appendix B.

    Since the equations containing K are l inear when K is

    constant, a computer may be used to obtain solutions for

    values of K from 0.0 to 1.0 for Equations (1), (2), (3), and (5) .

    While this set has an infinite number of solutions, only

    one particular solution gives a value of T in Equations

    (1), (2), and (3) that is equal to the value of T in

    Equation (4), for a particular value of K. This value of

    K defined the solution selected.

  • METHOD

    Subj ects

    Four adult male capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)

    served as subjects. These feral animals had extensive prior

    experience with sameness-difference learning-set (King and

    Fobes, in press, 1975) and 2AFC brightness discriminations

    ( K i n g a n d F o b e s , 1 9 7 3 ) .

    Apparatus

    The apparatus featured stimulus presentation behind

    a one-way-screen (7.5 by 6.5 cm) through which subjects

    viewed stimuli only during i l lumination behind the screen

    with a 4 0-watt frosted incandescent l ight. Stimuli con

    sisted of block mounted pigmented papers (3.8 by 3.8 cm)

    previously presented in the 2AFC investigation of brightness

    discrimination (King and Fobes, 1973).

    Stimuli became visible at the start of each trial

    and i l lumination automatically ceased following: (a) a

    correct Y response of interrupting a photocell beam, which

    was recessed and centered in front of the screen, before

    f ive seconds had elapsed (a "go" condition); (b) a correct

    No response of not breaking the photocell beam for five

    seconds (a "no-go" condition); or (c) an incorrect response.

    Correct responses were followed by a one second tone and

    dispensation of one-half raisin into a receptacle mounted

    below the screen,

    25

  • Procedure

    Sixteen three-trial problems a day were presented

    throughout pretraining, training, and testing. Eight

    stimulus sequences of S and N were used throughout and each

    of these stimulus sequences (viz. SSS, NNN, SSN, NNS, SNS,

    NSN, SNN, and NSS) appeared twice daily in random order with

    the restriction that no stimulus was presented for more than

    three consecutive trials. The S condition consisted of a

    grey stimulus and a white stimulus and the N condition con

    dition consisted of a pair of white stimuli . Subjects were

    pretrained by being rewarded for a Y response to a black

    next to a white stimulus and for a No response to two side-

    by-side white stimuli . This Y-N pretraining continued until

    subjects achieved 90 per cent correct responding with both

    types of responses for two consecutive days,

    Subjects were then trained with a t itration pro

    cedure that involved the same series of stimuli (white to

    dark grey in 60 increments) previously presented in the

    2AFC phase. This training (King and Fobes, 1973) began with

    a brightness discrimination between the darkest grey-white

    and white-white. After three consecutively correct

    responses of a Y to grey-white with a particular intensity

    of grey, the intensity of grey was increased by one step;

    failure to achieve three consecutively correct responses

    resulted in a one step decrease in the intensity of the

    grey. Subjects were thus tested to determine the grey

  • 27

    stimulus intensity around which responding stabilized.

    Three intensity steps below this point of stable responding

    was arbitrarily selected as the base intensity value of

    grey for each individual subject. The log^Q intensity

    difference between each subject's base grey and white

    was then divided by three in order to create three sub

    jectively equal proportions (Guilford, 1954), and a third

    of the intensity difference was selected as A grey for that

    subject. Varying around a mean A intensity of 0.062 foot/

    candle, the intensity values of three previously presented

    greys that were included in the present investigation were:

    (a) 1/2A, (b) A, and (c) 2A. Since the base value was

    individually determined for each subject the actual inten

    sity of A differed among subjects.

    In the present investigation, this t itration pro

    cedure was presented as a training task with a Y-N pro

    cedure. To assure that Y-N performance had stabilized,

    t itration continued until each subject responded correctly

    with a Y to grey-white and a No to white-white on a problem

    consisting of a discrimination between its 2A grey and white

    versus white and white.

    Testing consisted of three phases. In each phase,

    subjects were tested with one of the three (1/2A, A, and 2A)

    grey-white S levels and white-white N for 12 days. Each

    phase was preceded by two days of training with the

    particular intensity of grey to be tested, The order in

  • 28

    which each of these grey-white S conditions was presented is

    depicted in Table 7 and was determined for each subject by

    random assignment of one of the six permutations of three

    quantities.

    Table 7. Signal Level Presentation Order

    Phase

    Subject 1 2 3

    1 1/2A 2A A

    2 2A A 1/2A

    3 A 2A 1/2A

    4 A 1/2A 2A

    Thus, on each trial subjects were presented with one

    of two stimulus alternatives and could respond with one of

    two response alternatives. The S consisted of a condition

    wherein white was presented next to one of three grey

    stimuli that varied in brightness. The appropriate response

    to indicate S detection was a Y response of interrupting a

    photocell beam within five seconds. The N condition con

    sisted of white-white stimuli and a No response of not

    breaking the beam before f ive seconds had elapsed indicated

    N detection.

  • RESULTS

    Table 8 depicts the percentage of correct responses

    with each S intensity level of grey for both the present Y-N

    procedure and the previous 2AFC technique (King and Fobes,

    1973). Percentages of correct responses were virtually

    identical for the two procedures and were higher for A and

    2A than for the 1/2A condition.

    Table 8. Percentage of Correct Responses as a Function of Procedure and Signal Level

    Y-N 2AFC

    1/2A ' A 2A 1/2A A 2A

    74% 83% 83% 72% 80% 81%

    Figures 1 and 2 depict the proportional strength of

    Hs which was determined with the computational formulas

    presented in the Theoretical Conceptualization and Analyti

    cal Procedure Section as well as in Appendix A. Figure 1

    depicts the obtained proportion of Hs that involved one or

    more D states in a three-trial sequence, as a function of

    procedure and S level. With both procedures, the proportion

    of three D states per problem (A) increased as the task

  • 30

    Y-N 2AFC

    .5 r

    .4

    x I— g 3 LU cr h~ CO

    < .2 O I— QC O Q_

    § J Q_

    0

    0 O B D • • c ®

    -•

    1

    1/2 A A

    BRIGHTNESS

    2A

    Figure 1. Proportional Strengths of Hypotheses Involving Detection States, as a Function of Procedure and Signal Level

  • 31

    .5

    4

    X I— -2 o .O -Z. LLI or h-i f )

    < 2 o i-a: o Q_ 0 01 Q_

    . 2

    Y-N 2 AFC • 9 D • • O O E • •

    • — — p S

    O -o G

    A A R A -A

    1 1/2 A A

    BRIGHTNESS 2A

    Figure 2. Proportional Strengths of Hypotheses not Involving Detection States, as a Function of Procedure and Signal Level

  • 32

    became easier with decreasing intensities of grey, the

    proportion of two D states per problem (B) showed an

    inverted U shaped curve and the proportion for a single D

    state per problem (C) showed a U shaped curve.

    Figure 2 depicts the obtained proportions of Hs not

    involving any D states, as a function of procedure and S

    level. The triple response repetition (D) and residual (R)

    Hs accounted for the bulk of these D state manifestations.

    The proportions for double response repetition (E), win-

    stay : lose-shift (F) , and win-shift:lose-stay (G) were

    negligible. The D H displayed a U shaped curve that was

    more pronounced with the Y-N technique and the other

    prominent H, R, decreased as the brightness of grey

    decreased.

    In Table 5, E was defined as response repetition for

    two consecutive trials of a problem (I00 or 110). An addi

    tional type of sequential dependency consists of response

    alternation (101). While D and response repetition or

    alternation (E2) can be estimated by H analysis, no unique

    solution exists for D with both response repetition and

    alternation. Therefore, D and response alternation were

    solved assuming response repetition to be zero while D and

    response repetition were solved assuming alternation to be

    zero. Response repetition was found to be of greater

    magnitude than alternation and is therefore presented here.

    It should be further noted that formulas for D and

  • 33

    alternation differ from those presented for D and response

    repetition.

    To check the validity of the theoretical approach

    underlying the H analysis, predicted frequencies were

    calculated for each of the 3 2 unique combinations of

    stimulus, response, and outcome sequences in Table 6. These

    frequencies were based upon the overall strength of each H

    during the testing by a given procedure with a particular

    S level (see Levine, 1965) . A measure of the amount of

    variability among the 32 obtained cell frequencies accounted

    for by the Hs in the model was obtained from the following

    formula, where CK and are the observed and predicted

    proportions of cell i , and M is the mean of the observed

    cell proportions.

    3 2 1 - 2 ( 0 i - P i ) 2 / ( 0 i - M ) 2 .

    i=l

    This statistic is similar to Levine's (1965) Proportion of

    Variance Explained (P.V.E.) but differs sl ightly since

    Levine obtained the predicted frequencies by his Method I in

    such a way that each predicted frequency was based upon data

    which did not include the corresponding obtained frequency.

    Both Levine's P,V,E, and this modified version are thought

    2 to be fairly comparable to r_ and thus give an indication

    of the amount of between cell variance predictable by the

    a d d i t i v e e f f e c t s o f t h e H s m e a s u r e d ( L e v i n e , 1 9 5 9 ) .

  • 34

    Table 9 depicts the P.V.E. values determined. These high

    values are evidence that all nonnegligible l is were included

    in the analysis and that the Hs combined additively to

    determine the frequency of each of the 32 possible combina

    tions depicted in Table 6.

    Table 9. Proportion of Variance Explained as a Function of Procedure and Signal Level

    Y-N 2AFC

    1/2 A A 2A 1/2A A 2A

    . 9 7 . 9 9 . 9 9 . 9 7 . 9 9 . 9 8

    The computed strengths of the Hs were then used in

    the K estimation formulas presented in the Theoretical

    Conceptualization and Analytical Procedure Section as well

    as in Appendix B. The values for K determined as a

    function of procedure and brightness level of S are

    depicted in Table 10. For both procedures, K increased as

    the discriminations became easier.

    In order to relate K resulting from a 2AFC pro

    cedure to K estimated by a Y-N task in such a way as to be

    able to predict one from the other, all possible outcomes

    for an attentional trial were considered as depicted in

    Table 11, Values in this table are based upon the

  • 35

    Table 10. Sensitivity as a Function of Procedure and Signal Level

    Y-N 2AFC

    1/2A A 2A 1/2A A 2A

    . 8 7 . 9 1 1 . 0 0 . 8 2 . 8 7 . 9 5

    Table 11. Probability of Detection and Nondetection States per Trial Given Attention, as a Function of Procedure and Stimulus

    y-n 2AFC

    S n Probability S n Probability

    DS |a K1 Ds |a °n 1a k2(l - K2^

    dn |a K1 Ds 1a dn 1 a k2(i - k2)

    °S |a 1 l

    h

    Ds 1a dn 1a (k2)2

    °n |A 1 " K1 °s 1a 1a (i - k2)2

  • 36

    assumption that the probability of detecting S or N in a

    2AFC task is equal to the probability of detecting these

    same events in a Y-N task. It is further assumed that the

    subject will enter a D state in the 2AFC task whenever S,

    N, or both are detected. Therefore, i t follows that

    K2 = 2K1(1 - K^) + (I

  • 37

    Table 12. Sensitivity Estimates Obtained with Two-Alternative Forced-Choice, and Values Predicted from Yes-No, as a Function of Signal Level

    K 1/2A A 2A

    Obtained .82 .87 .95

    Predicted .83 .87 .96

    Table 13. Probability of Entering an Attention State per Trial, and of Zero, One, Two, and Three Attention States per Problem, as a Function of Procedure and Signal Level

    State Sequence

    Y-N 2AFC State

    Sequence 1/2A A 2A 1/2A A 2 A

    A Per Trial . 54 . 71 . 65 . 55 . 69 . 6 6

    3 As Per Problem . 4 0 , 5 6 . 4 9 . 4 0 . 4 9 . 4 6

    2 As Per Problem . 0 6 - . 0 5 . 25 . 2 6 . 1 5 . 22

    1 A Per Problem .19 . 2 5 . 1 2 . 08 . 22 .19

    3 As Per Problem . 3 5 . 24 . 1 4 . 26 . 1 4 . 1 3

  • two, and three A states per problem for each procedure and

    S level. These values were obtained with the solution of

    the equations for K estimation presented in the Theoretical

    Conceptualization and Analytical Procedure Section. The

    values of these various A and A states across S levels were

    comparable between procedures.

  • DISCUSSION

    The data reported in the results section were pro

    vided to i l lustrate an application of the proposed theory

    with two different testing procedures, rather than to

    provide an experimental comparison of these particular

    experimental designs. Therefore, tests for statistical

    significance of the differences were not included. The pro

    posed theory contains a three-state discrete sensory model

    which assumes that on each trial subjects either attend or

    do not attend to the S or N stimulation presented. If sub

    jects enter an A state on a given trial , they also enter

    either a D or a D state. An A state is always assumed to

    result in a D state. In addition, the sensory portion of

    the theory is unique in applying the threshold concept to

    the sensory effects of both N and S stimuli , with the assump

    tion that P(D |s A A) equals P(D|N A A). This is considered

    reasonable since the designation of what is S and what is N

    is totally arbitrary and detecting one of the stimuli to be

    discriminated is just as l ikely as detecting the other.

    The response model is partly deterministic in that a

    detect S state and a detect N state are assumed to always

    result in a correct response. It is also partly probabil

    istic since the probability of a correct response with a D

    state equals the a priori probability of S being presented.

  • 40

    With Y-N presentation, an A state in the presence of S

    results in either correct responding with P( + | s A A) =

    ( K + l ) / 2 o r i n c o r r e c t r e s p o n d i n g w i t h P ( - | s A A) =

    ( 1 - K ) / 2 . S i m i l a r l y , a n A s t a t e i n t h e p r e s e n c e o f N a l s o

    results in either correct responding with P(+|N A A) =

    ( K + l ) / 2 o r i n c o r r e c t r e s p o n d i n g w i t h P ( - | N A A ) =

    ( 1 - K ) / 2 . W i t h 2 A F C p r e s e n t a t i o n , a n A s t a t e r e s u l t s i n

    correct responding with P(+|s A A) = (K + l ) /2 or incorrect i_j

    responding with P(- |S A A) = (1 - K)/2. A A state with J -L

    both Y-N and 2AFC always results in a D state accompanied by

    responding which is determined by a response bias.

    The advantage of the present approach is thought to

    be the variety of information that i t provides which was not

    included in previous approaches (Blackwell , 1953, 1963;

    Luce, 1963a, 1963b; Green and Swets, 1966). Specifically,

    the proposed theory is thought to result in a highly de

    tailed description of the detection process which includes

    an estimation of the amount of responding due to specific

    nonsensory response biases. This analysis forms the basis

    for an estimate of sensitivity that is independent of

    response bias including the effects of sequential response

    dependencies. Both the sensitivity estimate and response

    bias measures are nonparametric and a technique is sug

    gested for the estimation of sensory thresholds that are not

    confounded with the effects of response bias. Further, this

    theory includes the concept of a high-threshold and is not

  • 41

    tied to a specific family of receiver operating character

    istics (ROC) curves.

    Initially, the probabilit ies of internal state sequences

    assumed to be manifested by systematic responding during Y-N

    and 2AFC problems are estimated by the relative proportions

    of the various Hs analyzed. This determination is based upon

    H analysis of the results of three-trial problems when

    results are assumed to reflect combinations of states

    D A A, D A A, and A that occur on each trial . The Hs that

    involve various modes of D states provide a nonparametric

    estimation of the proportion of one (A), two (B), and three

    (C) D A A states per problem. Nonparametric estimation of

    the type and amount of specific nonsensory response factors

    is provided by the proportions of response bias Hs. The

    lack of response independence between trials that is

    examined is based upon a conceptualization of response bias

    as multidimensional. These dimensions include (a) the

    tendency to perseverate responses for two (E) or three (D)

    trials, (b) the tendency to alternate responses (E^), as

    well as (c) the win-stay:lose-shift (F) and win-shift:lose-

    stay (G) syndromes.

    The accuracy of prediction for the degree of

    responding accounted for on the basis of H analysis was

    quantitatively described by the P.V.E. estimations. The

    P.V.E. values presented in Table 9 indicate that the H

    analysis procedure quite accurately estimated the patterns

    J

  • of frequencies observed. Changes in the occurrence of the

    proportions of the various Hs as a function of such

    variables as testing procedure and S level may then be

    examined as depicted in Figures 1 and 2.

    The H strengths were used to estimate the sensi

    tivity index that is expressed in terms of the P(d|a).

    Evidence that this index is independent of response biases

    including those of sequential response dependencies will be

    discussed shortly. In addition, the method of calculation

    of the sensitivity index also results in determination of

    the probability of an a state per trial as well as the prob

    ability of one, two, and three a states and three a states

    on three-trial problems. These combinations of a and a

    states may also be examined as a function of such variables

    as testing procedure and S intensity, as depicted in Table

    13.

    In l ine with the concept of a sensory recognition

    threshold, the present procedure includes the following

    technique for estimation of sensory thresholds in such a

    way as to be unconfounded with a range of nonsensory

    response biases. This procedure consists of replacing the

    proportion of correct responses to various stimulus inten

    sities with probability values of K. The threshold would

    then be defined by the stimulus magnitude corresponding to a

    value of K equal to .5. This technique is presently being

  • 43

    accessed by i ts use for the determination of Macaque visual

    acuity thresholds (Fobes, in preparation, 1975) .

    Nonparametric est imation of sensit ivity and response

    bias avoids the diff icult ies with assumptions accompanying

    parametric d' est imation with SDT procedures. The measures

    of sensit ivity and response bias used with the theory pre

    sented here do not depend upon any concept of underlying

    density functions describing the effects of sensory events .

    Nor do they depend upon accompanying assumptions concerning

    normality and variance as does the frequently used d' tech

    nique of Green and Swets (1966) . Their procedure for cal

    culat ion of the sensit ivity est imate depends upon whether

    the N and S density functions are assumed to be normal with

    equal or unequal variances. Thus, the present approach is

    in l ine with increasing interest in nonparametric est imators

    of sensit ivity and response bias (Grier, 19 71; Hammerton and

    Altham, 197i; Richardson, 1972) .

    The nature of the variance assumption that i s made

    in a given case is usual ly based upon the degree of symmetry

    of an P.OC curve. Such a curve connects plots of the P (Y|S)

    on the ordinate and the P(Y|N) on the abscissa as the P(Y)

    responses i s varied with the S level held constant. How

    ever with the present data, the empirical ROC curve can not

    be determined s ince the P(Y) responses was varied between

    rather than within S levels . Thus, distribution l inked d 1

    can not be calculated on these data s ince neither the

  • 44

    distribution nor the appropriate variance assumption can be

    determined. This does not pose a problem for the est imation

    of nonparametric K.

    The concept of a sensory threshold that i s rarely

    or never exceeded in the absence of the S has been specif i

    cal ly crit ic ized by some advocates of SDT. In their

    analysis of the high-threshold concept, Green and Swets

    (1966) compared SDT with a specif ic version of high-

    threshold theory that was advanced by Blackwell (1953, 1963) .

    Blackwell 's high-threshold theory included a sensory

    threshold that was thought to be rarely i f ever exceeded in

    the absence of S and below which sensory events were in-

    discriminable from one another. While the theoretical pro

    portion of "true" false alarms [(Y|N)*3 was assumed to

    approximate zero, empirical proportions of false alarms

    greater than zero were assumed to result from a Y response

    to some sensory events that fai led to exceed the sensory

    threshold. Since subthreshold sensory events were con

    sidered to be indist inguishable from each other, these Y

    responses were guesses and were correct by chance. There

    fore, the obtained proportion of hits was assumed to con

    s ist of the proportion of "true" hits [(Y |s)*], the value

    of which depended upon S strength, plus a guessing factor

    modif ied by the opportunity for guessing.

    P(Y|S) = P(Y|S)* + P(Y|N) [ l - P(y|s)*3 ( l )

  • A procedure was therefore used to obtain the pro

    portion of "true" hits corrected for guessing which adjusted

    the obtained hits according to the obtained false alarms

    that were taken as an index of the amount by which the hit

    rate was inf lated by guessing. This correct ion for chance

    success was a rearrangement of l ine (1) and took the form

    P(Y|S)* = P(Y|S) - P(Y|N) / 1 - P(Y|N). (2)

    This correct ion attempted to normalize the obtained psycho

    metric function by e l iminating the proportion of false

    alarms from the proportion of obtained hits . Upon deter

    mination of the P(Y|S)* for each st imulus magnitude, a

    psychometric function was plotted and the st imulus magnitude

    corresponding to a .5 P(Y|S)* was selected as the threshold

    value. Since the guessing mechanism which produced false

    alarms was thought to operate only in the absence of a

    sensory basis for a response, the procedure for correct ion

    of chance success assumed ff iat the P(Y|S)* and the P(Y|N)

    were independent.

    This assumption of the stat ist ical independence

    between "true" hits and false alarms has been shown to be

    false by several l ines of evidence. One approach consisted

    of a determination of the degree of correlat ion between the

    p(y |S)* and the P(y|n ) . Stat ist ical ly s ignif icant correla

    t ion coeff ic ients for these measures were found to be on the

    order of .90 (Green and Swets , 1966) . Green and Swets also

  • noted other diff icult ies with Blackwell 's theory. Thresh

    olds determined with Y-N and 2AFC procedures were not con

    s istent with each other. In addit ion, the equation in l ine

    (1) expressed the P(Y|S) as a linear function of the P(Y|N).

    Thus, the theoretical ROC curve based upon Blackwell 's

    version of high-threshold theory was a straight l ine from

    P(Y|S)*, P(Y|N)* through P(Y|S ) = 1 , P(Y|N ) = 1 . The vast

    majority of published ROC data are not adequately f i t ted by

    a straight l ine. Although Green and Swets (1966) have

    successful ly argued against Blackwell 's (1953, 1963) de

    fect ive version of high-threshold theory, their evidence

    does not automatical ly inval idate al l formulations that

    include a high-threshold concept, in contrast to their

    strong implication to the contrary.

    While Blackwell 's (1953, 1963) theoretical ROC curve

    resulted in a poor approximation of ROC data, the theo

    ret ical ROC curve result ing from the low-threshold theory

    proposed by Luce (1963a, 1963b) occasional ly provides a good

    f i t for empirical curves (Luce, 1963a; Green and Swets , 1966;

    King and Fobes, in preparation, 1975) . Luce's (1963a, 1963b)

    theory specif ied a sensory threshold that was frequently

    exceeded in the absence of S; the theoretical proportion of

    false alarms was greater than zero. As in Blackwell 's

    (1953, 1963) theory, Luce proposed a guessing mechanism that

    resulted in an observed proportion of false alarms that was

    greater than the "true" proportion due to random Y responses

  • 47

    to some subthreshold sensory events . In addit ion, a

    guessing mechanism was also proposed that resulted in an

    obtained proportion of hits that was less than the "true"

    proportion due to random N responses to some suprathreshold

    sensory events . Luce's theoretical ROC curve consisted of

    two linear segments. One segment went from a zero P(Y|S)

    and P(y|n ) at the lower left corner of the ROC graph to the

    point representing the calculated "true" probabil i ty that S

    results in detect ion and the calculated "true" probabil i ty

    that N alone results in detect ion. From this point , the

    other segment went to a P(yjs) and P(Y|N) of one at the

    upper r ight corner of the graph.

    One of the problems with this low-threshold concept

    i s that i t had an observer frequently detect ing something

    than was not presented, that i s , the theoretical P(Dg|N) was

    greater than zero. An addit ional diff iculty with Luce's

    theory i s that while ROC curves of the predicted shape are

    sometimes obtained, the most frequently reported empirical

    shape of the best f i t t ing l ine connecting ROC data i s

    curvi l inear, the shape predicted by SDT's theoretical ROC

    curve (Green and Swets , 1966) .

    Two general points emerge from a consideration of

    theoretical and empirical ROC curves. The proposed theory

    involves more components than did previous formulations and

    consequently, s l ight emphasis i s placed upon the two con

    dit ional probabil i t ies (hits and false alarms) that form the

  • 48

    basis of ROC data. Rather than being t ied to ROC curves of

    a particular form, the proposed theory i s consistent with

    any ROC curve according to the relat ionship

    tK = P(Y|S) - P(Y|N) , (3)

    which may be rearranged

    P(Y|S) = tK + P(Y|N) , (4)

    This reduced emphasis upon ROC data gives the present theory

    more general i ty than those l inked to ROC curves belonging to

    a particular family. Data f i t ted by the curve predicted by

    Luce's theory have provided some diff iculty for SDT and the

    f inding of data f i t ted by the curve predicted by SDT are

    diff icult to account for with Luce's theory. The second

    point i s that not a l l formulations of high-threshold theory

    necessari ly imply ROC curves of the type predicted by

    Blackwell 's version of high-threshold theory.

    In addit ion to the effects of sequential response

    bias , the proposed approach i s also thought to provide a

    sensit ivity index that i s independent of other nonsensory

    biases . I t was noted in the introduction that the SDT

    approach of Green and Swets (1966) a lso provided a measure

    of sensit ivity that was independent of the effects of some

    nonsensory factors , although not independent of the effects

    of sequential response bias . Specif ical ly , d' was rela

    t ively independent of varied response criteria, the a priori

    probabil i ty of S, and psychophysical procedure.

  • 49

    While ongoing research at The University of Arizona

    has yet to be completed on al l of these areas, a con

    siderable amount of data have been col lected which are

    directed at the quest ion of independence of the sensit ivity

    index K and mult iple response criteria. King and Fobes ( in

    preparation, 1975) presented humans with visual problems by

    both 2AFC and Y-N rat ing scale procedures. Subjects used a

    confidence index that featured f ive categories of confidence

    levels which varied from absolutely certain to just guessing.

    For data that have been analyzed, calculated values of K

    varied only 2.5% as a function of decis ion criteria. This

    indicates that the sensit ivity est imate i s independent of

    response biases . I t is noteworthy that d' was calculated

    with these data and d' was found to vary systematical ly as

    much as 15% as a function of decis ion criteria. This

    evidence in support of the proposed theory is in addit ion

    to that provided by this dissertat ion. That i s , the theory

    is conceptual ly sound, est imated sensit ivity increases as

    the S level increases , a reasonable profi le of Hs emerges,

    and a relat ionship exists between sensit ivity est imates

    obtained by several psychophysical procedures.

    An invest igation featuring various a priori prob

    abi l i t ies of S i s currently being completed with pigeons

    (Thomas and King, in preparation, 1975) . As reported for

    the d' sensit ivity index (Green and Swets , 1966) , the value

  • 50

    of the sensit ivity index K i s expected to remain constant as

    the a priori probabil i ty of S i s varied.

    A theory of detect ion processes should have gener

    al i ty and not be bound to any s ingle psychophysical method.

    Therefore, a complete theory should predict performance in

    a variety of test s i tuations. Green and Swets (1966)

    derived a predicted relat ionship between performance with

    two test ing procedures using a constant S level; the area

    under the Y-N rat ing ROC curve approximated the percentage

    of correct responses with the forced-choice technique. The

    present approach includes a proposed relat ionship between

    the sensit ivity est imate result ing from the differing pro

    cedures, however the init ial predict ion had to be modif ied.

    I t i s not known i f this modif icat ion was necessitated by the

    within subject design that tested al l subjects init ial ly

    with the 2AFC procedure, or i f the usage and s ize of the

    constant correct ion is a necessary part of the formula. In

    addit ion, the comparison between K^ a n t^ was based upon a

    spatial rather than upon the temporal forced-choice pre

    sentation which was used by Green and Swets (1966) . This

    issue may be resolved after al l the human rat ing data are

    analyzed (King and Fobes, in preparation, 1975) . The com

    parison of the sensit ivity est imate obtained by the two

    psychophysical procedures wil l then include the typical

    between groups, temporal 2AFC design.

  • 51

    The trend in contemporary psychophysics i s to con

    sider a threshold in terms of a response continuum. This

    has resulted in part from SDT's separation of sensit ivity

    est imation and factors affect ing a response threshold. The

    present paper describes an approach which also permits

    separation of nonsensory bias and sensit ivity, while re

    taining the concept of a sensory threshold. This theory is

    promising at this stage and warrants addit ional examination

    of i ts empirical ly testable consequences.

  • APPENDIX A

    FORMULAS FOR ESTIMATION OF HYPOTHESES

    Table 6 indicated that a given response sequence to

    a particular st imulus pattern was not synonymous with a

    s ingle H. Therefore, formulas were developed to est imate

    the proportion of the observed frequency due to each H,

    Assumptions concerning the relat ionship between Hs' proba

    bi l i t ies and the observed frequencies of the 32 events in

    Table 6 permit the statement

    4a + 24b + 48c + 8d + 16e + 8f + 8g + 32r = 4 , (A. l )

    which reduced to

    a + 6b + 12c + 2d + 4e + 2f + 2g + 8r = 1 , (A.2)

    where a = A, 6b = B, 12c = C, 2d = D, 4e = E, 2f = F, 2g =

    G, and 8r = R.

    The solutions for "a" involved the equations that

    included "a" (cel ls 5, 14, 23, and 32) from the 32 equations

    in Table 6 , These total led

    EQ = 4a + 12b + 12c +d+2e+f+g+ 4r. (A.3)

    In order to apply Equation (A.2) , i t was rewritten

    2d + 4e + 2f + 2g + 8r = 1 - (a + 6b + 12c) . (A.4)

    52

  • 53

    The ZQ in l ine (A.3) was then mult ipl ied by two, ci

    2£Q = 8a + 24b + 24c + (2d + 4e + 2f + 2g + 8r) , (A.5) ci

    and the term on the r ight s ide of Equation (A.4) was

    substituted for the identical expression in l ine (A.5)

    2EQ = 8a + 24b + 24c + (1 - a - 6b - 12c) . (A.6) ci

    Solving for "a,"

    a = (2£Q= - 18b - 12c - l ) /7 . (A.7) CI

    The solution for "a" was thus found to be a function of the

    obtained proportion of "a" as wel l as variables "b" and

    " c . "

    The equations containing "b" (cel ls 4 , 5 , 6 , 1 , 11,

    13, 14, 16, 18, 21, 23, 24, 25, 30, 31, and 32) total led

    = 4a + 24b + 36c + 4d + 8e + 4f + 4g + 16r. (A.8)

    Applying l ine (A.2) ,

    l /2ZQb = 2a + 12b + 18c + (1 - a - 6b - 12c) . (A.9)

    Solving for "b,"

    b = ( l /2ZQb - a - 6c - l ) /6 . (A.10)

    The equations containing "c" (al l cel ls except 1 ,

    10, 19, and 28) total led

    £Q c = 4a + 24b + 48c + 7d + 14e + 7f + 7g + 28r. (A.11)

  • 54

    Applying l ine (A.2) ,

    2/7£Q c = 8/7a + 48/7b + 96/7c + (1 - a - 6b - 12c) .

    (A.12)

    Solving for "c,"

    c = (2IQC - a - 6b - 7) /12 (A.13)

    These three solution equations (A.7, A.10, and A.13)

    may be rewritten

    2£Q - 1 = 7a + 18b + 12c a

    1/220^ ~* 1 — a + 6b + 6c

    2£Q c - 7 = a + 6b + 12c, (A.14)

    and solved by the method of determinates where

    7 18 12

    A = 1 6 6

    1 , 6 12

    504 + 108 +72-72 ~ 216 - 252

    144. (A.15)

    Substituting 2£Q - 1, 1/2EQ, - 1, and 2EQ - 7 for the Ct iJ

    coeff ic ients of "a" in the matrix of l ine (A.15) ,

  • 55

    2EQ -1 a

    18 12

    Aa = l /2EQb - l 6 6

    2ZQ -7 c

    6 12

    72(2EQ a - 1) + 108(2£Q c 7) + 72(l /2EQb - 1)

    - 72(2EQC - 7) - 216(l /2EQb - 1) - 36(2EQ a - 1)

    = 36(2£Q a - 1) - 144(l /2EQb - 1) + 36(2ZQ c - 7)

    (A.16)

    Where a = Aa/A,

    a = [ 3 6 (2EQ - 1) - 144(1/2EQ. - 1) + 36(22Q„ - 7J/144 a. JJ C

    (A.17)

    which reduced to

    a = l /4(2ZQ a - 1) - ( l /2ZQb - 1) + 1/4C2EQ c - 7) . (A.18)

    Therefore,

    A =-=a = , l /2EQ a - l /2EQb + l /2EQ c - 1 (A.19)

    Substituting 22Q - 1, 1/2ZQ, - 1, and 2£Q - 7 for 'a ' ~~^b ' -"«c

    the coeff ic ients of "b" in the matrix of l ine (A.15) ,

    b =

    2X0,-1 12 cL

    1 l /2EQb - l

    1 2EQ c -7 12

    = 84Cl/2EQb - 1) + 6(2EQ a - 1) + 12(2£QC - 7)

    - 12 ( l /2£Qb - 1) - 12(2EQ a - 1) - 42(2ZQ c - 7)

  • 56

    = 72(l /2ZQb - 1) - 6(2EQ a - 1) - 30(2EQ c - 7) . (A.20)

    Where b = Ab/A,

    b = [72(l /2£Qb - 1) - 6(2£Q a - 1) - 30(2ZQ c - 7)] /144,

    (A. 21)

    which reduced to

    b = l /2( l /2ZQb - 1) - 1/24(2£Q a - 1) - 5/24(2EQ c - 7) .

    (A.22)

    Therefore,

    B = 6b = 6( l /4EQb - l /12EQ a - 5/12EQ c + 1) . (A.23)

    Final ly , substituting 2EQ a - 1, l /2EQb - 1, and

    2EQ - 7 for the coeff ic ients of "c" in the matrix of l ine c

    (A,15) ,

    Ac =

    7 18 2£Q a~l

    1 6 l /2EQb - l

    2ZQC~7 1 6

    = 42 (2£QC - 7) + 18U/2EQb 1 ) + 6 ( 2 E Q - 1 )

    - 6 (2ZQ_ - 1) - 18(2EQ - 7) - 4 2(1/2EQK - 1)

    24C2ZQ c - 7) - 24(l /2ZQb - 1) . (A.24)

    Where c = Ac/A,

    c = [24(2EQC - 7) - 24(l /2£Qb - 1) ] /144, (A. 25)

  • 57

    which reduced to

    c = 1/6(2EQC - 7) - l /6( l /2ZQb - 1) . (A.26)

    Therefore,

    C = 12c = 12(1/3ZQ - l /2EQb - 1. (A.27)

    The solution for D included the equations containing

    "d" (cel ls 1 , 5 , 9 , 13, 17, 21, 25, and 29) which total led

    = a + 6b + 12c + 8d + 2f + 2g + 8r. (A.28)

    Applying l ine (A.2) ,

    = 1 - (2d + 4e + 2f + 2g + 8r)

    + 8d + 2f + 2g + 8r. (A.29)

    Solving for."d,"

    d = (SQd - 4e - l ) /6 . (A.30)

    Thus, "d" was found to be a function of the obtained pro

    portion of "d" as wel l as variable "e."

    The equations containing "e" (al l even numbered

    cel ls) total led

    £Q e = 2a + 12b + 24c + 16e + 4f + 4g + 16r. (A.31)

    Applying l ine (A.2) ,

    l /2IQ e = 1 - (2d + 4e + 2f + 2g + 8r) + 8e + 2f

    + 2g + 8r, (A.32)

  • 58

    Solving for "e,"

    e = ( l /2ZQ e + 2d - l ) /4 . (A.33)

    Lines (A.30) and (A.33) may be rewritten

    £Qd - 1 = 6d - 4d

    l /2ZQ e - 1 = 4e - 2d, (A.34)

    and these two equations with two unknowns can be s imul

    taneously solved. For "d," the £Qd - 1 = 6d - 4e and

    l /2£Q e - 1 = 4e - 2d combined to

    4d = EQd - 1 + l /2EQ e - 1, (A.35)

    which reduced to

    D = 2d = 2( l /4£Qd + 1/8ZQ - 1/2) . (A.36)

    For "e," l /2ZQ e - 1 was mult ipl ied by a posit ive

    number ( three) to prevent cancel lat ion of "e" by combination

    of - 1 and l /2£Q e - 1. Therefore, £Qd - 1 = 6d - 4e and

    3( l /2EQ e - 1) = 12e - 6d combined to

    8e = 3( l /2ZQ e - 1) + EQd - 1, (A.37)

    which reduced to

    E = 4e = 4(3/16EQ e + l /8EQd - 1/2) . (A.38)

    To solve for F, the equations containing "f" (cel ls

    4, 5 , 12, 13, 19, 22, 27, and 30) were total led

  • 59

    £Q f = a + 6b + 12c + 2d + 4e + 8f + 8r. (A.39)

    Applying l ine (A.2) ,

    EQ f = 1 - (2d + 4e + 2f + 2g + 8r) + 2d + 4e + 8f + 8r.

    (A. 40)

    Solving for "f ,"

    f = (£Q f + 2g - l ) /6 . (A.41)

    Thus, "f" was found to be a function of the obtained

    proportion of "f" as wel l as variable "g."

    The equations containing "g" (cel ls 1 , 8 , 9 , 16, 18,

    23, 26, and 31) total led

    EQ = a + 6b + 12c + 2d + 4e + 8g + 8r. (A.42) g

    Applying l ine (A.2) ,

    £Q g = 1 - (2d + 4e + 2f + 2g + 8r) + 2d + 4e + 8g + 8r.

    (A.43)

    Solving for "g,"

    g = (£Q„ + 2f - l ) /6 . (A.44) y

    Lines (A.41) and (A.44) may be rewritten

    ZQf - 1 = 6f - 2g

    EQ - 1 = 6g - 2f , (A. 45) 9

    and these two equations with two unknowns can be s imul

    taneously solved.

  • 60

    For "f", 3(ZQ^ - 1) and EQ^ - 1 combined to

    16f = 3(EQ f - 1) + EQ g - 1, (A.46)

    which reduced to

    F = 2f = 2(3/16EQ f + 1/16EQ - 1/4) . (A.47) l. y

    For "g," 3(EQg - 1) and EQ^ - 1 combined to

    16g = 3 (EQ - 1) + EQ f - 1, (A.48) y

    which reduced to

    G = 2g = 2(3/16EQ + 1/16EQ- - 1/4) . (A.49) H J-

  • APPENDIX B

    FORMULAS FOR ESTIMATION OF K

    The formulas for the est imation of K presented in

    the Theoretical Conceptual izat ion and Analytical Procedure

    sect ion involved the fol lowing components .

    A = P(3Ds) = P(3Ds A 3As) (B.1)

    B = P (2Ds) = P(2Ds A 3As) + P(2Ds A 2As) (B.2)

    C = P ( I D )

    = P ( I D A 3 A s ) + P ( I D A 2 A s ) + P ( 1 D A 1 A ) ( B . 3 )

    D = obtained value of H D (B.4)

    E = obtained value of H E (B.5)

    F = obtained value of H F (B.6)

    G = obtained value of H G (B.7)

    R = 1 - ( A + B + C + D + E + F + G ) ( B . 8 )

    Mj^ = P (2As A 1A)

    = P ( An A An+1 A V2 1 + P ( An A Vl A W

    + P ( An A An+1 A W ( B - 9 )

    M , = P ( 1 A A 2 A s )

    = P ( A„ A An+X A + P ( An A A„+ l A An+2>

    + P ( A f l A n + 1 A A n + 2 * ( B . 1 0 ) n

    61

  • 62

    t = P ( A n ) ( B . l l )

    T = P ( A A A A A „ ( B . 1 2 ) n n+1 n+2

    K = P ( D n | A n ) ( B . 1 3 )

  • 63

    + p ( D n A V l A V 2 I 3 A S ) + P ( D n A V l A V 2 I 3 A S ) I

    + p ( An A Vl A An+2' P ( D„ A Vl A V2 lAn A An+1

    A V 2 > + P ( A n A A n + 1 A V 2 > P ( D n A V l A V 2 l A n

    A A A A , „ ) + P ( A A A . A A ) P ( D A D , n+1 n+2 n n+1 n+2 n n+1

    A D . J A A A - A A , 0 ) . ( B . 2 0 ) n + 2 ' n n + 1 n + 2

    Substituting for def init ions in (B.12) and (B.13) ,

    B = 3TK 2 (1-K) + [P(An A An + 1 A An + 2> + P(An A An + 1

    A An+2> + P ( An A An+1 A An+ 2 ) I K 2 - ( B" 2 1 )

    Substituting for the definit ion in (B.9) ,

    B = 3 T K 2 ( ( 1 - K ) + M