12 collyer s

  • Upload
    claupy

  • View
    219

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/12/2019 12 collyer s

    1/10

  • 8/12/2019 12 collyer s

    2/10

    (eg,7) but it also has strong advocates (eg,4). The lack of percep-

    tual study of the abdomen-ouT directive, in particular, means

    these criticismsare untested.

    Swank10(p13) tested the following abdominal-inward

    directive:

    As you sing the next tones, feel the muscles of the abdominal

    wall contract, with gradual compression, beginning very low

    and moving inward and upward gradually. Let the muscleslow in the abdomen help you lift the airflow upward and out.

    Swank10 found that this directive was less effective in elicit-

    ing vocal change on a range of acoustical measures, including

    tone duration and energy in theregion 28003300 Hz associ-

    ated with the singers formant.16 Two points arise from this

    observation. Firstly, the relationship between acoustical mea-

    sures and listener preferences is highly complex and not yet

    fully understood.1719 Thus, the acoustical results reported by

    Swank cannot be assumed to imply a deterioration in vocal

    quality. For example, an increase in tone duration can occur

    when a singer increases vocal-fold medial compression, but

    a voice that sounds pressed would not be considered as animprovement in classical singing. This means that the percep-

    tual implications of the acoustical changes observed by Swank

    are unclear.

    Secondly, Swank10 did not measure respiratory behavior. In

    an earlier study,20 we investigated the effects on female classi-

    cal singers CWK behavior of abdomen-iN and abdomen-ouT

    directives. Our abdomen-iN directive was comparable with

    that of Swank10 and asked that the singers steadily pull the

    abdominal wall inward for each phrase whilst singing, whereas

    the abdomen-ouT directive asked that the singers steadilyexpand the abdominal wall during singing. The abdomen-ouT

    directive was of interest given the conflicting opinions of its

    appropriateness in singing pedagogy, discussed above. We

    found that highly trained singers can alter aspects of their

    CWK behavior under directive and can sustain these changes

    throughout a song. However, the changes that occurred were

    a function of the interaction of the directive and the singers

    habitual (ie, undirected) CWK behavior. Also, the amount ofchange varied among singers, with some singers showing large

    change and some very little change under each directive. Our

    results showed that assessment of the influence of any directive

    on vocal quality must take into account how and to what extent

    CWK behavior has actually changed, especially given the wide

    differences in singers kinematic strategies that have been

    observed.

    2123

    It seems reasonable to assume that the singersstudied by Swank10 would have shown a similar interactive

    effect of habit and directive, but without CWK data the extent

    to which this might have affected the acoustical results cannot

    be determined.

    The relationship between CWK behavior under a physiolog-

    ically based directive and perception of voice does not appear to

    have been investigated. Abdominal directives have been studied

    for inhalation, withmixed outcomes in terms of effect on vocal

    quality. Thomasson24 found no significant effect for profes-

    sional singers on a range of voice source parameters, including

    vertical laryngeal position. Iwarsson,25 however, found a higher

    vertical laryngeal position for speakers in the abdomen-ouT

    condition (after controlling for LV), which post hoc investiga-

    tion associated with changes in posture. Again, neither of the

    studies incorporated an audio perceptual component.

    Studies of nonphysiologically based directives have found

    perceptible changes in vocal quality with changes in respiratory

    behavior. Foulds-Elliott et al26 found that instructing profes-

    sional singers to perform with emotional connection led tohigher LV at the start of phrases and lower at the end and con-

    sequently larger expenditure of air than when merely singing

    loudly. They also found that experienced listeners could distin-

    guish audio samples with and without emotional connection,

    although interestingly the perceptual cues reported by the

    listeners did not include any assessment of changes in breathing

    during phonation.

    Another nonphysiological directive often used in pedagogy

    is to support thevoice with the breath, considered essential

    for the singing voice13 despite the elusiveness of an agreed def-

    inition for the term support. Thorpe et al asked singers to

    imagine they were projecting their voices over a large or

    small orchestra,27(p100) a directive which the authors associ-ated with greater and lesser abdominal support, respectively.

    Kinematic patterns were similar in both conditions, but greater

    projection was associated with higher LV at the end of phrases

    and concomitantly smaller expenditure of air. Despite the asso-

    ciation of projection with abdominal activity, they found LV

    changes to be mostly because of changes in ribcage dimension.

    Even so, changes in sound level and frequency spectrum char-

    acteristics could not be correlated with changes in respiratory

    parameters.

    Griffin et al28 (p51) found that the direction to sing with sup-port elicited no changes in respiratory behavior, despite noting

    that the subjects in this study clearly believe that a supported

    singing voice is.

    produced by managing breathing activity

    differently. They did, however, find significant changes in

    acoustic and laryngeal measures (including peak glottal air-

    flow) that implied changes in voice quality. Although they did

    not conduct a perceptual study, strong listener agreement in as-

    sessing the presence and degree of support present in audiosamples has beenreported by Sand and Sundberg.29

    Sonninen et al30 also found laryngeal and acoustic differ-

    ences in singers asked to sing with and without support, but lis-

    tener perception was more complex. Although listeners were

    generally able to identify supported and unsupported singing

    samples, their assessment of the degree of support (0100%)

    present in a sample varied widely. Indeed, three of the sevensingers could not differentiate between supported and unsup-

    ported singing when listening to their own recordings 2 months

    after data collection. The authors noted that [t]hese samples

    also caused difficulties to other listeners.30(p231) Respiratory

    behavior was not measured, so the extent of change in the

    singers breathing under directive is unknown. Interestingly,

    they found no distinction between ratings for voice quality

    and for support, suggesting that the terms and concepts were

    interchangeable.

    In summary, studies of nonphysiologically based directives

    have produced mixed results in terms of changes in respiratory

    Journal of Voice, Vol. 25, No. 1, 2011e16

  • 8/12/2019 12 collyer s

    3/10

    behavior and consequent changes in vocal quality. The diffi-

    culty with nonphysiologically based directives from a singing

    training perspective is that they lack clear and agreed definition.

    This has led singing teachers to rely on more direct, physiolog-

    ically based instruction, but the perceptible effects of such in-

    structions are unknown.

    This study asked classical singing teachers and vocal coaches

    to assess the standard of singing and of breath management ofsingers performing under three conditions: habitually (ie, undi-

    rected) and under two simple, specific, and dichotomous

    abdominal kinematic directives: abdomen-iN and abdomen-

    ouT. The audio samples were derived from the companion

    study20 that analyzed the type and extent of change in respira-

    tory behavior elicited by the directives. That study found that

    the singers habitual abdominal behavior lay on a continuum

    between the extremes elicited by the two directives, generally

    resembling one directive more than the other. Therefore, it

    was hypothesized that listeners would be able to detect a differ-

    ence in standard of singing and standard of breath management

    proportionate to the degree of CWK change exhibited by the

    singer. It was further hypothesized that listeners would ratehabitual singing higher than nonhabitual singing and, in turn,

    would rate singing under the abdomen-iN directive higher

    than under the abdomen-ouT directive.

    METHOD

    The study was approved by the Human Ethics Committee of

    The University of Sydney. Fourteen singing teachers and vocal

    coaches experienced with classical singing voice assessed

    audio samples of five female classical singers under three

    breathing conditions. All participants received an information

    sheet and gave written informed consent.

    Audio samples

    Audio and respiratory recordings were made of five profes-

    sional female classical singers: three sopranos (Singers A, B,

    and E), one mezzo-soprano (D), and one contralto (C), ranging

    from 31 to 44 years of age. All singers held postgraduate qual-

    ifications in singing and had between 7 and 20 years of profes-

    sional performing experience. At the time of recording, Singer

    D was in her 19th week of pregnancy but was maintaining her

    full performance schedule. Recordings were made at the audio-

    recording laboratory at the Sydney Conservatorium of Music

    (The University of Sydney), measuring 4.843 4.553 2.64 m

    and hung with sound-absorptive curtains (http://www.jands.com.au/). Respiratory measurements were made using respira-

    tory inductance plethysmography (Inductotrace; Ambulatory

    monitoring system Inc., Ardsley, NY). Audio recordings were

    made with a pair of cardioid microphones (Neumann, SKM

    140, Georg Neumann GmbH, Berlin) in ORTF (Office de

    Radiodiffusion Television Francaise) configuration31 placed

    2.75 m from the singer. Sound intensity has been found to influ-

    ence listener assessment of voice.32 Analysis of the audio

    recordings20 confirmed that the equivalent sound level calcu-

    lated from long-term average spectra of the last four phrases

    was consistent across breathing condition (F2,8 2.795,

    P 0.12), that is, any listener preferences in this study could

    not be attributed to differences in sound level between perfor-

    mances.

    Each singer sang two takes of Caccinis Ave Maria in g minor

    (unornamented, no repeat) without instruction (Habitual) and

    two takes under each of the following directives:

    N Abdomen-iN: steadily pull the abdominal wall inwardduring phonation, using an additional inward pull if re-

    quired for additional support and

    T Abdomen-ouT: steadily expand the abdominal wall dur-

    ing phonation, using an additional outward expansion if

    required for additional support.

    CWK plots of ribcage and abdominal dimensions during the

    last four phrases of the song were made for each singer.20

    Examples aregivenin Figure1. CWKplots show thecontribution

    of abdominal (abscissa) and ribcage (ordinate) dimensional

    change to change in LV, with the axes scaled to contribution

    capacity (after22). Kinematic traces that run parallel to the

    45 isovolume lines represent a change in ribcage:abdomenconfiguration but no change in LV. Traces that move to the left

    of an isovolume line represent a net decrease in LV even where

    the ribcage or abdominal dimension might be increasing. The

    small cross represents ribcage:abdomen configuration at end-ex-

    piratory level, that is, at the end of expiration in quiet breathing.

    The plots were used to select the three takes (one per breathing

    condition) that showed the greatest variance in kinematic behav-

    ior, and the audio samples from these takes were used for the per-

    ceptual study. That is to say, the audio samples were chosen

    according to respiratory behavior not according to the sound.

    Two audio CDs were constructed using the last four phrases of

    the chosen take under each breathing condition (H, N, and T).

    The order of singers was different on each CD, and the order of

    breathing condition was randomized within that. Audio samples

    ofthe firsttwosingerson eachCD (SingersA and B onCD#1 and

    E and D on CD#2) were repeated. Odd-numbered listeners heard

    CD#1 (Singers A, B, C, A, D, E, B) and even-numbered listeners

    heard CD#2 (Singers E, D, C, B, E,A, D).Repeating two singerssamples allowed us to assess how consistently listeners assessed

    the same sample on different hearings. Randomizing and using

    two CD sets minimized the possibility that results reflected sam-

    ple order rather than inherent vocal quality. Furthermore, the

    threeindividual samples for each singer were preceded by a com-

    posite track that concatenated the threesamples (with a 4-s pause

    between) in the same order. Thus, each CD contained 28 tracks:sevensingers3 fourtracks. Studieshave found thatlistenerstendto use the first sample heard as a context for subsequent sam-

    ples17,33 and that listeners internal standards of voice quality

    are inherently unstable.34 Playing the concatenated track first

    was designed to have all three samples set the comparative con-

    textand to providea wash-out of theinfluenceof assessment of

    a previous singer on a subsequent singer.35

    Listener data

    The 14 listeners consisted of 11 classical singing teachers, of

    whom six maintained a national and/or international

    Sally Collyer, et al Listener Perception of Abdominal Directives in Singing e17

    http://www.jands.com.au/http://www.jands.com.au/http://www.jands.com.au/http://www.jands.com.au/
  • 8/12/2019 12 collyer s

    4/10

    performance schedule, two chorus-masters, one accompanist,

    and one singing-voice researcher specializing in classical voice.

    All but one held tertiary qualifications in music, the exception

    being a singing teacher who had retired from performing after

    a career, including leading roles at Covent Garden and the Met-

    ropolitan Opera, and all were employed full-time professionally

    in their respective fields. Hearing was not tested, but all re-

    ported normal hearing and all rely on accurate hearing for their

    professions. Data collection was conducted in quiet rooms with

    only the participant and one researcher present, using a CD

    player (Sony Model CFD V8; Sony CFD S300; or Sony CD

    Walkman DEJ885 W, Sony Corp., Tokyo) and headphones

    (Sennheiser, HD 650, Sennheiser Electronics GmbH, Wede-

    mark-Wennebostel, Germany). Listeners first heard the com-

    posite track containing all three samples in the same order in

    which they would be presented individually. They then heard

    the first sample that they assessed using five visual analog

    (VA) scales with gradations numbered 010 from left to right.

    The greater resolution of continuous scales has been found to

    improve listener agreement over equal-interval scales by not

    forcing a choice between ratings on samples perceived to fall

    between intervals.3537 Listeners were asked to rate the first

    sample by placing a vertical mark and the number 1 above

    it on each scale, before moving to the second sample. During

    explanation of the task, a dummy completed sheet was used

    to illustrate how to complete the form. Sample assessments

    on this dummy form included a mix of widely and closely

    spaced marks, so as to encourage but not to prescribe that lis-teners make full use of a scales range. Listeners were not con-

    strained from marking between gradations (eg, at a location

    equating to 6.3) or from giving two samples the same score.

    The first scale asked, Please assess the standard of singing

    for each sample and the second asked, Please assess the stan-

    dard of breath management for each sample. (The three re-

    maining scales addressed tone color, vibrato rate, and vibrato

    extent and are not included in this study.) Standard of singing

    was positioned first as Stanley et al38 found judges make an

    overall assessment before attending to specific criteria. Scales

    also had the cues poor (underneath 0), average (under

    5), and excellent (under 10). Listeners were given as much

    time as they wished to assess each sample before hearing thenext sample, but tracks were not repeated. Fresh sheets were

    provided for each set of three samples, and listeners could not

    refer to previous sheets. Data collection generally took 11.5

    hours.

    The listener criteria standard of singing and breath man-

    agement were deliberately general in nature, and no clarifica-

    tion or further explanation of the terms was provided. Listeners

    were invited, but not required, to add comments in the space

    provided beneath each scale if they wished to clarify aspects

    of their assessments. Intra- and interjudge reliability in assess-

    ing singers have been found to be higher for general (overall)

    assessmentsthan for specific criteria in singing39 and in speech

    pathology.17 Listeners have also been found to differ as to

    whether they assess on performance or intrinsic vocal quality,39

    and it was intended that the term standard of singing should

    encompass both aspects. The implicit inclusion in listeners

    assessments of factors not relevant to this study, such as diction

    and phrasing, was not considered significant because compari-

    sons were between performances by the same singer.

    Listeners were made aware of the purpose of the study and of

    the breathing directives that had been given to the singers. This

    ensured that listeners worked from an equal knowledge base

    and encouraged them to focus on the breathing behavior of

    the singers. They were informed that respiratory measurements

    had been made of the singers but not whether the directives hadelicited changes in respiratory behavior. Because only five

    singers were used, it was anticipated that experienced listeners

    would be likely to recognize a repetition. Therefore, listeners

    were explicitly told that two singers samples would be heard

    twice, again to ensure that all listeners worked from an equal

    knowledge base.

    Data analysis

    The score for each listener criterion for each sample was mea-

    sured by ruler in millimeters and input into a customized

    spreadsheet (Excel 2003, Microsoft) that converted the result

    % Abdominal capacity

    yticapacegacbiR

    %

    25 50 75

    75

    50

    25100%LV

    0%

    LV

    Decreasing

    LV

    Singer A Singer D

    Singer B Singer E

    Singer C

    FIGURE 1. Examples of the changes in CWK behavior elicited by

    abdominal kinematic directive for the five singers. (Thick solid line

    habitual, broken line abdomen-iN, fine solid line abdomen-ouT;

    LV, lung volume.) Construction of the plots is described in Method:

    Audio samples. These plots compare the second-last phrase of the

    first or second take (whichever was used in the perceptual study; refer

    to Method: Audio samples) under the three conditions, showing the

    nature and extent of kinematic change elicited by directive and under-lying the audio samples. Axes are scaled to percentage of abdominal

    and ribcage capacity.14

    Journal of Voice, Vol. 25, No. 1, 2011e18

  • 8/12/2019 12 collyer s

    5/10

    into a percentage. This was checked against a visually esti-

    mated score. Comments were also recorded on the spreadsheet.

    Statistical analysis usedlinear mixed modelingin SPSS(v14.0

    for Windows) (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL) with repeated measures.

    Model testing for best-fit of covariance structure used the cor-

    rected Akaike Information Criterion40 because of the small sam-

    ple size. A criterion ofP < 0.05 for pairwise comparisons was

    adjusted (Bonferroni) for three multiple comparisons (H vs T,N vs T, and H vs N), giving a significance criterion ofP < 0.017.

    Intralistener (test-retest) repeatability was assessed by corre-

    lation (Pearsons r) and by modeling with singer and breathing

    condition as fixed factors and hearing (first and second) as the

    repeated measure. (Only scores from the second hearing were

    used in the other analyses.) The relationship between the two

    listener criteria was similarly tested by correlation and by mod-

    eling, with the score for each criterion as the repeated measure.

    Interlistener consistency for each listener criterion was assessed

    from covariance parameters obtained by a mixed effects model,

    with singer and listeneras random factors and breathing condi-

    tion as a fixed factor.41

    Listener scores were analyzed using linear mixed modelingwith fixed factors of singer and breathing condition. Data

    were arranged in the order T-N-H to reflect assumptions that

    the abdomen-ouT behavior is the least-favored pedagogically

    and that habitual behavior would score higher than the nonha-

    bitual behaviors. For both listener criteria, the scaled identity

    covariance structure was the best fit for both random and re-

    peated effects and singer (but not breathing condition) was

    a random factor. Significant interactions were assessed by pair-

    wise comparisons.

    RESULTS

    CWK patterns

    Examples of the CWK patterns of the singers from the audio

    samples used in this study are plotted in Figure 1. The plots

    show the second-last phrase drawn from the three takes (one

    per breathing condition) used for the perceptualstudy. Respira-

    tory analysis reported in the companion study20 found that the

    directives led the singers to alter the abdominal and ribcage di-mensions at the start but not at the end of phrases and that LV

    measurements were unchanged.Figure 1illustrates how habit-

    ual kinematic strategy changed with directive for each of the

    singers, highlighting similarity and difference between habitual

    and directive patterns. This information allowed the perceptual

    results to take into consideration how much behavioral changewas actually elicited.

    Singer As habitual pattern lay very close to the abdomen-iNend of the continuum; accordingly, the abdomen-ouT directive

    had a major effect on her kinematic behavior, completely re-

    moving ribcage paradoxing and disrupting smooth coordination

    between abdomen and ribcage. Singer B showed clear differ-

    ences between the three conditions: abdominal paradoxing (ex-

    pansion during phonation) under N suggested that she found the

    sharp initial decrease unsustainable but there was no such dis-

    ruption under T, although it is clear that she was resisting her

    habitual tendency for abdominal contraction. Singer C showed

    the most striking differences between conditions, with ribcage

    paradoxing under N and abdominal paradoxing under T. By

    contrast, Singer D showed the least change across condition,

    although adjustments that were characteristic of other singers

    (increased ribcage paradoxing in N, delayed abdominal con-

    traction in T) were clear in her kinematic plots. Lastly, Singer

    Es kinematic trace for N generally paralleled her habitual trace.

    In summary, habitual patterns seemed to lie toward the abdo-men-iN end of the continuum for Singers A, D, and E, toward

    the abdomen-ouT end for Singer B, and midway for Singer C.

    Intra- and interlistener consistency

    For both listener criteria, test-retest correlations were moderate

    (standard of singing r 0.683,P < 0.001; breath manage-

    mentr 0.674,P < 0.001), and there were significant differ-ences between scores for the first and second hearing. Model

    testing resulted in a scaled identity covariance structure, with

    singer (but not breathing condition) as a random factor. For

    standard of singing, main effects of singer (P< 0.001),

    breathing condition (P 0.016), and hearing (P< 0.001) were

    all significant, as were interactions of singer3condition(P< 0.001) and singer3hearing (P 0.034). For breath man-

    agement, singer was the only significant main effect

    (P< 0.001), but interactions of singer3condition (P 0.003)

    and singer3hearing (P 0.008) were again significant. The in-

    teraction of singer3condition3hearing was not significant for

    either criterion and was removed from the final models.

    Pairwise comparisons for the singer3hearing interaction

    found that listeners rated standard of singing higher on

    second hearing for two of the four singers (Singer D mean

    difference12.2, standard error [SE] 2.9, P< 0.001;

    Singer E mean difference 6.9, P< 0.001), whereas listeners

    rated the breath management higher on second hearing for

    Singer D (mean difference 10.1, SE 3.4, P

  • 8/12/2019 12 collyer s

    6/10

    two scales being vertically aligned on the page, so that the

    disparity would have been visible to the listeners.

    Criterion scores

    Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests were nonsignificant (standard of

    singingZ

    0.51; breath managementZ

    0.39) and visualexamination of residual plots identified no trends, confirming

    normality of residual distribution. Tests of fixed effects are

    set out in Table 1. For both criteria, there was a significant

    main effect for singer (P< 0.001). Pairwise comparisons found

    that the scores for Singers B and E were significantly higher

    than for Singers A, C, and D, and this can be seen in Figure 2.

    There was a significant main effect of breathing condition for

    standard of singing (P 0.019) but not for breath manage-

    ment (P 0.102). However, the interaction of singer3breath-

    ing condition was significant in both criteria (P< 0.001), that is,

    there were significant differences between condition but only

    for some singers.

    These differences are broken down in the pairwise compari-

    sons for the singer3breathing condition interaction in Table 2

    and can also be seen in Figure 2. Pairwise comparisons were

    tested at a Bonferroni-adjusted significance of P < 0.17. For

    standard of singing, Singer A scored significantly lower in

    the abdomen-ouT condition than in the other conditions, whereas

    Singer D scored higher in the habitual condition than under eitherdirective. For breath management, Singer A again scoredlower in the abdomen-ouT condition than in the other conditions.

    Singer D again scored higher in the habitual condition than under

    the abdomen-iN directive, but scores under the abdomen-ouT di-

    rective were not significantly lower, as they were for standard of

    singing. Singer B scored higher under the abdomen-ouT direc-

    tive than under the abdomen-iN. Her mean score under T was

    also higher than for her habitual samples but failed to reach Bon-

    ferroni-adjusted significance (P 0.17). Singer Es habitual

    singing was rated more highly than her singing under T for

    both criteria but again the differences were not statistically

    significant after adjusting for multiple comparisons.

    TABLE 1.

    Linear Mixed Modeling of the TwoCriteria Found a Significant Interaction of Breathing Condition and Singer, That Is, There

    Were Significant Differences in Ratings on Both Criteria Between Breathing Condition but Only for Some Singers

    Standard of Singing Standard of Breath Management

    Fixed effects

    Breathing condition F2,128.9 4.085, P 0.019 F2,128.6 2.327,P 0.102

    Singer F4,68.6 22.947,P < 0.001 F4,70.4 15.645,P < 0.001

    Condition3singer F8,128.9 4.969, P < 0.001 F2,128.6 3.425,P 0.001

    0

    20

    40

    60

    80

    100

    T N H T N H T N H T N H T N H

    A B C D E

    Singer (by breathing condition)

    )001fotuo(erocS

    FIGURE 2. Estimatedmarginalmeans and standard errors of listener scores (out of 100) for standard of singing (solidsquares) and standard ofbreath management (crosses). Scores are grouped by singer (AE), then by breathing condition (abdomen-ouT, abdomen-iN, and habitual). Singers

    B and E scored significantly higher than did Singers A, C, and D on both listener criteria. Scores for the two criteria were highly correlated, but

    listeners rated breath management more critically (lower) than standard of singing for all singers except Singer C.

    Journal of Voice, Vol. 25, No. 1, 2011e20

  • 8/12/2019 12 collyer s

    7/10

    DISCUSSIONBreath management is acknowledged as fundamental to sing-

    ing, but its training relies on the assumption that a singers

    breathing behavior has a direct and proportionate effect on

    the singers vocal quality. The corollary, that changes elicited

    by physiologically based CWK directives can be monitored im-

    mediately by the singing teachers assessment of vocal quality,

    is especially important because breathing instruction is becom-

    ing increasingly defined in terms of physiology. This studytested whether the type and degree of change in CWK pattern

    elicited by two simple, specific, and dichotomous abdominal ki-

    nematic directives proportionately matched the assessment ofstandard of singing and of breath management of female profes-

    sional classical singers, assessed from audio samples by

    listeners experienced with the classical singing voice. The

    singers sang under three breathing conditions: without instruc-

    tion (habitually), pulling the abdominal wall inward duringphonation (abdomen-iN), and expanding the abdominal wall

    during phonation (abdomen-ouT).

    The first hypothesis was that listeners would be able to detect

    a difference in standard of singing and standard of breath man-

    agement proportionate to the degree of CWK change exhibited

    by the singer. Key to this was to take into consideration the

    degree of similarity between each directive and the singers

    habitual CWK strategy. The results for Singers A and B wereconsistent with the hypothesis. Singer As habitual pattern

    was very similar to her strategy under the abdomen-iN directive

    and both were markedly different from the abdomen-ouT direc-

    tive. Likewise, listener ratings found no difference between her

    singing in H and N but found both differed from her singing un-

    der T. Singer Bs CWK pattern differed less between conditions

    than did Singer As, but her habitual pattern tended to be closer

    to T. Likewise, listeners rated her breath management to be bet-ter in T than N but not different under directive than habitually.

    Although ratings for the standard of singing showed the same

    trend, the smaller differences were not statistically significant

    after Bonferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons.

    Strictly speaking, Singer Es results could also be considered

    as consistent with the hypothesis. Her habitual pattern more

    closely resembled that under the abdomen-iN directive, and

    listener scores averaged highest for her habitual singing and

    lowest for her abdomen-ouT singing. However, the differences

    between CWK patterns were small and between listeners scores

    were not significantly different after Bonferroni adjustment.

    Her results, therefore, do not contradict but also do not support

    the hypothesis.

    The results for Singers C and D were not consistent with thehypothesis. Singer C showed the most change in CWK strategy

    under directive, yet there was no significant difference in

    listener ratings. Singer D showed the least change in CWK pat-

    tern, yet listeners found the standard of both singing and breath

    management in the nonhabitual conditions to be inferior to

    habitual behavior. Thus, the results for Singers A, B, and E

    appeared consistent with the hypothesis but the results for

    Singers C and D were not.

    It was also hypothesized that listeners would rate habitual

    singing higher than nonhabitual singing and, in turn, would

    rate singing under the abdomen-iN directive higher than under

    the abdomen-ouT directive, following pedagogical opinion that

    abdomen-ouT behavior is deleterious to vocal quality. Again,

    the results were mixed. In terms of habitual versus nonhabitual

    breathing, only Singer D scored significantly higher for H than

    under either directive. However, the strong similarity between

    Singer As H and N CWK strategies meant that her significantly

    lower scores under Talso support this hypothesis. Lastly, Singer

    Es results showed higher listener preference for habitual than

    nonhabitual breathing but the differences were not statistically

    significant. Mean ratings for Singers B and C went against the

    hypothesis but again the differences were not statistically

    significant.

    TABLE 2.

    Pairwise Comparisons of the Interaction of Breathing Condition and Singer

    Singer

    H vs N H vs T N vs T

    Mean Difference, P Mean Difference,P Mean Difference,P

    Standard of singing

    A 3.2,P 0.326 10.6, P 0.001* 13.8, P < 0.001*

    B 5.2, P 0.106 2.5,P 0.437 7.8, P 0.017**C 4.2,P 0.194 4.2,P 0.200 0.1, P 0.988

    D 11.6, P 0.001* 9.3, P 0.005* 2.3, P 0.482

    E 2.9, P 0.375 7.2, P 0.027** 4.3, P 0.180

    Standard of breath management

    A 1.9, P 0.629 12.1, P 0.003* 10.1, P 0.011*

    B 3.5, P 0.381 6.5, P 0.100 10.0, P 0.012*

    C 3.6, P 0.362 3.7, P 0.349 0.1, P 0.981

    D 11.6, P 0.004* 6.7, P 0.093 4.9, P 0.219

    E 2.6, P 0.509 8.5, P 0.033** 5.9, P 0.138

    *Significant at P < 0.17; **Significant without Bonferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons.

    Sally Collyer, et al Listener Perception of Abdominal Directives in Singing e21

  • 8/12/2019 12 collyer s

    8/10

    In terms of ratings under abdomen-iN being higher than un-

    der abdomen-ouT, only Singer As ratings supported this and

    only at face value. That is to say, the strong similarity between

    her H and N CWK strategies noted above suggests that the

    lower listener rating associated with the abdomen-ouT directive

    might occur with any nonhabitual directive, as was the

    case with Singer D. Singer Bs results did not support the

    hypothesis, showing a significantly higher listener preferencefor T than N. Again, habitual CWK strategy needs to be taken

    into consideration, as Singer Bs H pattern was much closer to T

    than to N. Listeners also preferred T to N for Singer D but not

    significantly so.

    In summary, we found that the ability of experienced

    listeners to detect changes in breathing behavior depended on

    the individual singer and on the extent to which the directive

    deviated from the singers habitual pattern. Furthermore, we

    found no support for the pedagogical assertion that the abdo-

    men-ouT directive necessarily results in a perceptibly lower

    standard of singing. Rather, our findings suggested that the stan-

    dard of singing was compromised by any directive that elicited

    CWK behavior antithetical to habitual behavior.Our results raise four issues with respect to the study of

    breathing training methods in singing: habitual kinematic

    behavior, limitations of body-surface measurement, participant

    pool, and improving reliability in perceptual studies in singing.

    Habitual kinematic behavior

    Any assessment of a breathing directive should consider CWK

    strategy throughout the entire phrase, not just excursion-only or

    LV measures, and should incorporate in the assessment the type

    and extent of change in CWK strategy elicited by the directive

    in the context of the singers habitual CWK strategy. By refer-

    ring to habitual behavior, the apparently contradictory results

    for Singers A, B, and D were in fact consistent in terms of

    any directive eliciting nonhabitual behavior.

    Limitations of body-surface measurement

    It must be remembered that body-surface measurement has lim-

    itations in describing underlying change in respiratory behav-

    ior. This was clearly a factor in the results for Singer D,

    whose CWK strategies showed small differences between

    breathing conditions, yet were quite audible to experienced lis-

    teners. It is possible that her pregnancy reduced the range of her

    abdominal movement that could be detected by body-surfacemeasurement, although we are not aware of any studies into

    the effect of pregnancy on CWK behavior in singers, and the

    singer was maintaining a full performance schedule. Techno-

    logical limitations mean that the relationship between CWK

    strategy, muscular activity, and respiratory proprioception is

    poorly understood. Likewise, from the teachers perspective,

    the relationship between CWK strategy defined in terms of con-

    tribution to LV change (as used in respiratory research) and in

    terms of dimensional change (as observed in the teaching

    studio) needs much clarification before results of CWK studies

    can be applied in the studio.

    Participant pool

    It could be argued that the highly trained singers in our study

    have already optimized their habitual abdominal behavior. If

    so, mean scores for breath management for Singers A, C,

    and D (

  • 8/12/2019 12 collyer s

    9/10

    management is in effect a subset of the scale for standard of

    singing. A subscale would suggest a wider range of scores for

    breath management. It would also suggest that a given in-

    crease in rating for breath management should lead to

    a smaller increase in standard of singing because it repre-

    sents only a portion of potential improvement. Although our re-

    sults (Figure 2) did not show these trends, our study was not

    designed to investigate these issues. Another possibility isthat listeners made an overall assessment that they then justi-

    fied, consciously or unconsciously. The greater consistency of

    general criteria as used in our study comes at the cost of iden-

    tifying and ranking the vocal characteristics that are important

    to listeners. Our results suggest that teasing out the components

    of vocal assessment might be complicated by an initial general

    assessment (solicited or unsolicited) providing a contextual

    influence similar to the effect on comparing audio samples

    discussed above.

    CONCLUSION

    In summary, our results question pedagogical assumptions

    about direct relationships between breathing behavior, breath-

    ing training directives, and perceptible changes in vocal quality.

    There is little examination in research or pedagogy of the cir-

    cumstances under which training approaches fail or are less

    successful, despite the value of such information to understand-

    ing and refining methods and practices in any domain. Although

    the enormous variability in habitual CWK behavior in singers

    has been well documented in research studies, it is yet to be

    integrated into pedagogical thinking. Our study begins the im-

    portant work of identifying why certain directives work for

    some singers and not for others, so that training can be tailored

    and streamlined.

    Acknowledgments

    The authors are grateful to the participants and to Dr Helen

    Mitchell for her advice on data collection. This study was sup-

    ported by an Australian Research Council Discovery Grant

    (DP066559) to Professor Dianna T. Kenny and Dr C. William

    Thorpe.

    REFERENCES1. Blades-Zeller EL. A Spectrum of Voices: Prominent American Voice

    Teachers Discuss the Teaching of Singing. Lanham, MD: Scarecrow; 2003.

    2. Chapman JL.Singing and Teaching Singing: A Holistic Approach to Clas-

    sical Voice. San Diego, CA: Plural Publishing; 2006.

    3. Duey PA. Bel Canto in Its Golden Age: A Study of Its Teaching Precepts .New York, NY: Da Capo; 1980.

    4. Greene A.The New Voice: How to Sing and Speak Properly . Milwaukee,

    WI: Hal Leonard; 1985.

    5. Hemsley T. Singing and Imagination: A Human Approach to a Great

    Musical Tradition. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 1998.

    6. Hines J. Great Singers on Great Singing. New York, NY: Limelight

    Editions; 1982.

    7. Miller R. The Structure of Singing:Systemand Art in VocalTechnique. New

    York, NY: Schirmer; 1996.

    8. Monahan BJ.The Art of Singing: A Compendium of Thoughts on Singing

    Published Between 1777 and 1927. Metuchen, NJ: Scarecrow; 1978.

    9. Spillane KW. Breath support directives used by singing teachers: a Delphi

    study.NATS J. 1989;45:921. 57.

    10. Swank H. Some verbal directives regarding support concepts and their

    effects upon resultant sung tone. NATS J. 1984;40:1218.

    11. Miller R.Solutions for Singers: Tools for Performers and Teachers. Breath

    management. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2004. 14.

    12. Hixon TJ, Hoffman C. Chest wall shape in singing. In: Lawrence V, ed.

    Transcripts of the Seventh Symposium Care of the Professional Voice,

    1978 June. New York, NY: The Voice Foundation; 1979:910. Part 1.

    13. Hixon TJ. Respiratory Function in Singing: A Primer for Singers and

    Singing Teachers. Tucson, AZ: Redington Brown; 2006.

    14. Leanderson R, Sundberg J, Von Euler C. Role of diaphragmatic activity

    during singing: a study of transdiaphragmatic pressures. J Appl Physiol.

    1987;62:259270.

    15. Sundberg J, Leanderson R, Von Euler C. Voice source effects of diaphrag-

    matic activity in singing.J Appl Physiol. 1986;14:351357.

    16. Sundberg J.The Science of the Singing Voice. DeKalb, IL: Northern Illinois

    University Press; 1987.

    17. Kreiman J, Gerratt BR, Precoda K, Berke GS. Individual differences in

    voice quality perception.J Speech Hear Res. 1992;35:512520.

    18. Ekholm E, Papagiannis GC, Chagnon FP. Relating objective measurements

    to expert evaluation of voice quality in western classical singing: critical

    perceptual parameters.J Voice. 1998;12:182196.

    19. Mitchell HF, Kenny DT. The effects of open throat technique on long term

    average spectra (LTAS) of female classical voices. Logoped Phoniatr

    Vocol. 2004;29:99118.

    20. Collyer S, Kenny DT, Archer M. The effect of abdominal kinematic direc-tives on respiratory behavior in female classical singing.Logoped Phoniatr

    Vocol. 2009;34:100110.

    21. Collyer S, ThorpeCW,Callaghan J, Davis PJ.The influence offundamental

    frequency and sound pressure level range on breathing patterns in female

    classical singing. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51:612628.

    22. Watson PJ, Hixon TJ. Respiratory kinematics in classical (opera) singers.

    J Speech Hear Res. 1985;28:104122.

    23. Watson PJ, Hixon TJ, Stathopoulos ET, Sullivan DR. Respiratory kinemat-

    ics in female classical singers. J Voice. 1990;4:120128.

    24. Thomasson M. Belly-in or belly-out? Effects of inhalatory behavior and

    lung volume on voice function in male opera singers. Speech Transm Lab

    Q Status Prog Rep. 2003;45:6174 [Internet]. Available at: http://www.

    speech.kth.se/prod/publications/files/qpsr/2003/2003_45_1_061-074.pdf,

    2003.Accessed May 4, 2009.

    25. Iwarsson J. Effects of inhalatory abdominal wall movement on vertical la-ryngeal position during phonation.J Voice. 2001;15:384394.

    26. Foulds-Elliott SD, Thorpe CW, Cala SJ, Davis PJ. Respiratory function in

    operatic singing: effects of emotional connection.Logoped Phoniatr Vocol.

    2000;25:151168.

    27. Thorpe CW, Cala SJ, Chapman J, Davis PJ. Patterns of breath support in

    projection of the singing voice. J Voice. 2001;15:86104.

    28. Griffin B, Woo P, Colton R, Casper J, Brewer D. Physiological characteris-

    tics of the supported singing voice: a preliminary study.J Voice. 1995;9:45

    56.

    29. Sand S, Sundberg J. Reliability of the term support in singing. Logoped

    Phoniatr Vocol. 2005;30:5154.

    30. Sonninen A, Laukkanen A-M, Karma K, Hurme P. Evaluation of support in

    singing. J Voice. 2005;19:223237.

    31. Farina A, Lamberto T. Measurements and reproduction of spatial sound

    characteristics of auditoria. Acoust Sci Tech. 2005;26:193199.

    32. Kempster GB, Kistler DJ, Hillenbrand J. Multidimensionalscaling analysis

    of dysphonia in two speaker groups.J Speech Hear Res. 1991;34:534543.

    33. Repp B, Crowder RG. Stimulus order effects in vowel discrimination.

    J Acoust Soc Am. 1990;88:20802090.

    34. Gerratt BR, Kreiman J, Antonanzas-Barroso N, Berke GS. Comparing in-

    ternal and external standards in voice quality judgments. J Speech Hear

    Res. 1993;36:1420.

    35. Shrivastav R, Sapienza CM, Nandur V. Application of psychometric theory

    to the measurement of voice quality using rating scales. J Speech Lang

    Hear Res. 2005;48:323335.

    36. Kreiman J, Gerratt BR, Kempster GB, Erman A, Berke GS. Perceptual

    evaluation of voice quality: review, tutorial, and a framework for future

    research.J Speech Hear Res. 1993;36:2140.

    Sally Collyer, et al Listener Perception of Abdominal Directives in Singing e23

    http://www.speech.kth.se/prod/publications/files/qpsr/2003/2003_45_1_061-074.pdfhttp://www.speech.kth.se/prod/publications/files/qpsr/2003/2003_45_1_061-074.pdfhttp://www.speech.kth.se/prod/publications/files/qpsr/2003/2003_45_1_061-074.pdfhttp://www.speech.kth.se/prod/publications/files/qpsr/2003/2003_45_1_061-074.pdf
  • 8/12/2019 12 collyer s

    10/10

    37. Kreiman J, Gerratt BR, Ito M. When and why listeners disagree in voice

    quality assessment tasks. J Acoust Soc Am. 2007;122:23542364.

    38. Stanley M, Brooker R, Gilbert R. Examiner perceptions of using criteria in

    music performance assessment. Res Stud Music Educ. 2002;18:4656.

    39. Wapnick J, Ekholm E. Expert consensus in solo voice performance evalu-

    ation.J Voice. 1997;11:429436.

    40. Clifford MH, Tsai CL. Regression and time series model selection in small

    samples. Biometrika. 1989;76:297307.

    41. Hakkesteegt MM, Wieringa MH, Brocaar MP, Mulder PGH, Feenstra L.

    The interobserver and test-retest variability of the Dysphonia Severity In-

    dex. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2008;60:8690.

    Journal of Voice, Vol. 25, No. 1, 2011e24