13
Hsu, J. S.-C., Huang, H.-H., & Linden, L. P. (2011). Computer-mediated Counter-Arguments and Individual Learning. Educational Technology & Society, 14 (4), 111–123. 111 ISSN 1436-4522 (online) and 1176-3647 (print). © International Forum of Educational Technology & Society (IFETS). The authors and the forum jointly retain the copyright of the articles. Permission to make digital or hard copies of part or all of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than IFETS must be honoured. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, to republish, to post on servers, or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from the editors at [email protected]. Computer-mediated Counter-Arguments and Individual Learning Jack Shih-Chieh Hsu 1 , Hsieh-Hong Huang 2* and Lars P. Linden 3 1 Department of Information Management, National Sun Yat-sen University, Taiwan // 2 Department of Information Science and Management Systems, National Taitung University, Taiwan // 3 Department of Information Technology, Georgia Southern University, USA // [email protected] // [email protected] // [email protected] *Corresponding author ABSTRACT This study explores a de-bias function for a decision support systems (DSS) that is designed to help a user avoid confirmation bias by increasing the user’s learning opportunities. Grounded upon the theory of mental models, the use of DSS is viewed as involving a learning process, whereby a user is directed to build mental models so as to reduce the tendency to consider only data that supports any preexisting mental models. The results of an experiment suggest that a de-bias function, called computer-mediated counter-argument (CMCA), facilitates individual learning and improves decision satisfaction. Keywords Mental model, Individual learning, Computer-mediated counter-argument, De-bias, Decision support systems Introduction A decision support system (DSS) is a platform for learning in at least two ways. First, a DSS aids the user in extending his or her knowledge about the subject matter of the decision domain. For example, a person using a stock- selecting DSS may explore the relationships among interest rates and stock prices and, while exploring, learn knowledge about the markets. Second, a DSS may present the user with an opportunity for adjusting his or her knowledge about the decision-making process itself. In this case, the system aids a person in altering the decision- making process in an attempt to improve the outcome. An example of this second way of learning is shown by a user of a stock-selecting DSS who, having been warned of an error-producing cognitive bias known as gambler’s fallacy, reacts to this warning by performing additional steps in the decision-making process, which in this case might include the reflection upon possible market outcomes, if future prices are not dependent upon historical prices. This second way of learning, an adjustment of the decision-making process, serves the decision maker by reconciling the many interlocking and incompatible beliefs about the problem domain, some of which may be based upon cognitive biases (Tverskey & Kahneman, 1974). Increasingly, attention is being focused upon cognitive theories of learning and how these theories can be understood and applied in the context of DSS, a trajectory of research that is distinct from much of DSS research, which largely focuses upon the end result of decision making: decision quality (Santana, 1995). Along these lines, our focus is upon the second type of learning and how a DSS might help to adjust a user’s decision-making process for the purposes of overcoming a cognitive bias known as confirmation bias. During DSS use, cognitive biases present a perilous downside. Users, enabled by DSS, may make poor decisions and enact seemingly limited decision-making processes, often while confident that all is well. Several examples illustrate cognitive biases arising during the use of DSS. A cognitive bias called the illusion of control occurs when a user of a DSS performs a what-if analysis and displays increased confidence, yet achieves no significant performance gain (Davis & Kottemann, 1994). The cognitive bias called confirmation bias occurs when a person who is gathering information restricts attention to only data that supports a favored hypothesis. The cognitive bias known as the illusion of knowledge occurs when a user is overconfident about having access to a greater amount of information, as is easily the case with a DSS, but makes poor decisions, despite the additional information. This last cognitive bias was detected when investors who had switched from a phone-based investing method to an online trading platform became more confident, and yet recorded poor performance despite having access to more information (Barber & Odean, 2002). Attempting to counter these cognitive biases is called de-biasing (Fischhoff, 1982). In general, de- biasing techniques, such as educating users about biases, are believed to beneficially impact decision quality (Arnott, 2006) and de-biasing techniques applied to DSS have been successfully demonstrated (Bhandari, Deaves, & Hassanein, 2008). To investigate the embedding of a de-biasing technique into the design of a DSS we adopt mental- model theory.

Document11

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Document11

Hsu, J. S.-C., Huang, H.-H., & Linden, L. P. (2011). Computer-mediated Counter-Arguments and Individual Learning. Educational Technology & Society, 14 (4), 111–123.

111 ISSN 1436-4522 (online) and 1176-3647 (print). © International Forum of Educational Technology & Society (IFETS). The authors and the forum jointly retain the copyright of the articles. Permission to make digital or hard copies of part or all of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than IFETS must be honoured. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, to republish, to post on servers, or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from the editors at [email protected].

Computer-mediated Counter-Arguments and Individual Learning

Jack Shih-Chieh Hsu1, Hsieh-Hong Huang2* and Lars P. Linden3 1Department of Information Management, National Sun Yat-sen University, Taiwan // 2Department of Information

Science and Management Systems, National Taitung University, Taiwan // 3Department of Information Technology, Georgia Southern University, USA // [email protected] // [email protected] //

[email protected] *Corresponding author ABSTRACT

This study explores a de-bias function for a decision support systems (DSS) that is designed to help a user avoid confirmation bias by increasing the user’s learning opportunities. Grounded upon the theory of mental models, the use of DSS is viewed as involving a learning process, whereby a user is directed to build mental models so as to reduce the tendency to consider only data that supports any preexisting mental models. The results of an experiment suggest that a de-bias function, called computer-mediated counter-argument (CMCA), facilitates individual learning and improves decision satisfaction.

Keywords

Mental model, Individual learning, Computer-mediated counter-argument, De-bias, Decision support systems Introduction A decision support system (DSS) is a platform for learning in at least two ways. First, a DSS aids the user in extending his or her knowledge about the subject matter of the decision domain. For example, a person using a stock-selecting DSS may explore the relationships among interest rates and stock prices and, while exploring, learn knowledge about the markets. Second, a DSS may present the user with an opportunity for adjusting his or her knowledge about the decision-making process itself. In this case, the system aids a person in altering the decision-making process in an attempt to improve the outcome. An example of this second way of learning is shown by a user of a stock-selecting DSS who, having been warned of an error-producing cognitive bias known as gambler’s fallacy, reacts to this warning by performing additional steps in the decision-making process, which in this case might include the reflection upon possible market outcomes, if future prices are not dependent upon historical prices. This second way of learning, an adjustment of the decision-making process, serves the decision maker by reconciling the many interlocking and incompatible beliefs about the problem domain, some of which may be based upon cognitive biases (Tverskey & Kahneman, 1974). Increasingly, attention is being focused upon cognitive theories of learning and how these theories can be understood and applied in the context of DSS, a trajectory of research that is distinct from much of DSS research, which largely focuses upon the end result of decision making: decision quality (Santana, 1995). Along these lines, our focus is upon the second type of learning and how a DSS might help to adjust a user’s decision-making process for the purposes of overcoming a cognitive bias known as confirmation bias. During DSS use, cognitive biases present a perilous downside. Users, enabled by DSS, may make poor decisions and enact seemingly limited decision-making processes, often while confident that all is well. Several examples illustrate cognitive biases arising during the use of DSS. A cognitive bias called the illusion of control occurs when a user of a DSS performs a what-if analysis and displays increased confidence, yet achieves no significant performance gain (Davis & Kottemann, 1994). The cognitive bias called confirmation bias occurs when a person who is gathering information restricts attention to only data that supports a favored hypothesis. The cognitive bias known as the illusion of knowledge occurs when a user is overconfident about having access to a greater amount of information, as is easily the case with a DSS, but makes poor decisions, despite the additional information. This last cognitive bias was detected when investors who had switched from a phone-based investing method to an online trading platform became more confident, and yet recorded poor performance despite having access to more information (Barber & Odean, 2002). Attempting to counter these cognitive biases is called de-biasing (Fischhoff, 1982). In general, de-biasing techniques, such as educating users about biases, are believed to beneficially impact decision quality (Arnott, 2006) and de-biasing techniques applied to DSS have been successfully demonstrated (Bhandari, Deaves, & Hassanein, 2008). To investigate the embedding of a de-biasing technique into the design of a DSS we adopt mental-model theory.

Page 2: Document11

112

The theory of mental models explains how people perform certain activities, for example, information processing. When a decision maker confronts a large amount of information, a mental model aids in the filtering of that information. The mental model fulfills a gatekeeper role for the mind by preventing unrelated and unimportant information from being consciously considered. However, along with the beneficial effects that are attributed to mental models, there are also error-producing effects. Inadequate mental models may block important information. The ability of a decision maker to change mental models is viewable as a learning process, one that may be enabled by DSS. That a person can change existing mental models is important, given that mental models may have a negative impact during decision making (Vandenbosch & Higgins, 1994). Through the mental-model theory, we seek to explain how an embedded de-bias function can induce a change in a person’s mental model, conceptualized as a type of learning, and, as a result, overcome confirmation bias. Mental models provide a theoretical explanation upon which to ground a DSS function for reducing confirmation bias. The consideration of mental models is not new to DSS research and can be recognized as a type of cognitive skill. The use of DSS is attributed to the development of cognitive skills. Intelligent tutoring tools, for example, provide the user with an opportunity to obtain cognitive skills, such as the ability to learn via self-explanation (Patel, Kinshuk, & Russell, 2000). Technology-enriched learning includes meta-learning activities, such as monitoring and sequencing one’s own learning (Sinitsa, 2000). The cognitive skill being considered in this research is the learning effect of mental-model reforming. We seek to understand if the design of a de-biasing function embedded in a DSS increases the cognitive skill of the type of learning associated with mental-model reforming. The de-bias function being explored aims to eliminate cognitive bias through the introduction of counter-arguments. The de-bias function that is embedded in a DSS is designed to present counter-arguments to the user so as to eliminate bias through a learning process. We seek to understand if users, having read counter-arguments, report to have built mental models and express a lower confidence level, as compared with users without counter-arguments. Being aware of the need to lessen the impact of confounding variables from the environment, we conducted an experiment to investigate this embedded de-bias function. In the following sections, we discuss the concepts of mental-model theory, confirmation bias, and learning. We propose a research model grounded upon mental-model theory and state our hypotheses. We discuss the research design and the procedures of the experiment. We analyze the data and discuss the results in light of their implication to DSS design and individual learning. Theoretical background and hypotheses Mental models and confirmation bias While explaining the nature of thought, Craik (1943) describes the modeling capabilities of the human mind, whereby “small-scale models” of external entities are manipulated by the mind to consider past events and to anticipate future events. These mental models are psychological representations of the environment and consist of cognitive constructs and their relationships (Holyoak, 1984). As such, mental models serve a range of purposes: when designing, mental models help a person to describe the composition of entities; when observing a system, mental models help a person to explain the current states of unseen variables; and when considering the future, mental models help a person to predict events and suitable responses to these events (Rouse & Morris, 1986). Mental models, as structured patterns, aid in classifying information and aid in simulating a set of outcomes based upon varying conditions (Cannon-Bowers, Salas, & Converse, 1993). An activity such as information processing suggests that mental models are relevant to DSS design. One example illustrates the association of mental models with cognitive biases. In the stock market, a decision maker needs to predict a future price in order to make the purchasing decision. The decision maker possesses a mental model of the impacts of various factors upon price. This mental model helps to guide the search for information required for predicting price and abandon information that is not important to the decision. If abundant amounts of information are available, the mental model serves as a filter that helps the decision maker forego consideration of inconsequential information. A mental-model-based information search can reduce processing time and minimize information overload. These benefits, however, which coexist with a mental model, may lead the decision maker to

Page 3: Document11

113

neglect, or perceive as unimportant, information critical to the analysis. Mental models contribute to a cognitive bias called confirmation bias. Wason (1960) defined the phenomenon of confirmation bias as people’s tendency to seek evidence in support of their assumptions instead of searching for evidence that challenges their assumptions. When a person contemplates concepts or verifies assumptions, a variety of biased tendencies have been described. A person may tend to treat evidence supporting existing beliefs more favorably than is objectively appropriate. A person confronted with a body of information may “see” exactly the effect that the person set out to find (Russo, Medvec, & Meloy, 1996). A person may tend to pass along information that is congruent to their beliefs (Heath, 1996). In short, the problem is that “decision-makers seek confirmatory evidence and do not search for disconfirming information” (Arnott, 2006, p. 60). The consequence of overweighting some evidence and underweighting other evidence is reaching a wrong conclusion. Confirmation bias is observed to be an exceptionally problematic aspect of human reasoning (Nickerson, 1998). How then might the design of a DSS compensate for this cognitive bias? Mental models and learning Based on mental models and learning theory (e.g., Gagne, 1977; Norman 1982; Piaget 1954), Vandenbosch and Higgins (1994) argue that mental models are closely connected to learning. Mental models direct the information-gathering process and limit the process. Reflexively, the information gathered has the potential to change mental models, confirm them, enhance them, change them, and reinforce them. Vandenbosch and Higgins (1994) state that the gathered information impacts mental models through two different processes: mental-model maintenance and mental-model building. Mental-model maintenance occurs when new information fits easily into existing mental models. Mental-model building occurs when new information from newly perceived situations and environments provokes a person to change existing mental models. The connection between mental models and learning is amplified by Vandebosch and Higgins (1994) with their comparison of mental models to theories found in the management discipline. First among these theories is Argyris and Schön’s (1978) organizational learning theory of single-loop and double-loop learning. Single-loop learning represents the learning that takes place when existing routines guide the problem-solving process. Double-loop learning represents the challenging of existing routines and the need to restructure existing norms and assumptions. These two ways of learning are similar to the learning that is explained by the use of mental models. In terms of mental models, single-loop learning maps to mental-model maintenance, both of which pertain to the confirmation and disconfirmation of new information when compared to an existing, stable theory. Double-loop learning maps to mental-model building, both of which represent a potential change in foundations of how learning is achieved. Vandenbosch and Higgins (1994) also draw parallels between mental models and another theory, March’s (1991) theory of exploitation and exploration learning. Exploitation is the act of enhancing performance by improving current practices. Exploration is the act of enhancing performance by introducing new practices. Exploitation represents mental-model maintenance as one attempts to reinforce current mental models by adding new constructs or building new links between existing constructs. Exploration represents the mental-model building process as one attempts to reconstruct the model completely (March, 1991). These management theories, because they include both the learning of individuals and the collective of individuals (i.e., the organization as a whole) may not be fully applicable to the individual learning considered in this research. However, in as much as they are found to model individual learning, these theories are useful because they are congruent with our understanding of the maintenance and building of mental models. Of the two approaches described, mental-model maintenance and mental-model building, the one more likely to happen is mental-model maintenance (e.g., Quinn, 1980; Kiesler & Sproull, 1982; Grønhaug & Falkenberg, 1989). Acceptance of mental-model maintenance as a form of learning is high because it can be routinely confirmed through a person’s past experience. Vandenbosch and Higgins (1994) write how, in contrast, mental-model building implies that decision makers face uncertainty. The theory of mental-model building has been tested through empirical evidence. People tend to make decisions or take in part or all action based on their mental models. The mental-model-building form of learning requires stimulus such as feedback from actions and information that contrasts with current thinking. DSS could be designed to induce mental-model building by injecting a particular type of information. Therefore, we attempt to identify the presence of mental-model building by introducing the stimulus of

Page 4: Document11

114

computer-mediated counter-arguments (CMCA). A counter-argument is information contrasting with what the decision maker previously hypothesized, and computer-mediated counter-argument is defined as opposite information provided by a DSS during the decision-making process. In addition, in our study, we also attempt to determine whether individuals using a system with counter-argument report a higher level of satisfaction. The research model (Figure 1) shows the relationship between these factors.

Figure 1. Research model

From a cognitive perspective, existing mental models determine the ideal construction and concept framing process. Due to cognitive-based biases, people tend to read information that fits into their current mental model. The information-selection process allows them to gather evidence that can support their existing mindset. Although the received information either strengthens or weakens the current mental model (Vandenbosch & Higgins, 1996), theory indicates that once one’s belief is formed, it is not easy to change if there is no additional stimulus (Hoch & Deighton, 1989). Since subjects in the group without computer-mediated counter-argument tend to maintain their current mental model rather than building a new mental model, therefore, we hypothesize the following:

H1a: The score of mental-model maintenance for subjects who use DSS without computer-mediated counter-argument is higher than for those who use DSS with computer-mediated counter-argument.

On the other hand, people question their existing mindset when they receive contradictory evidence (Vandenbosch & Higgins, 1996). Counter-arguments provided by the system give contradictory evidence that challenges decision makers’ perspectives and provides a chance for them to reevaluate their current mental model. Under this setting, people are more likely to abandon their current mental model or take appropriate steps to improve their mental model. Therefore, subjects in the group with computer-mediated counter-argument tend to reform their mental model, rather than maintain their mental model. We hypothesize the following:

H1b: The score of mental-model building for subjects who use DSS with computer-mediated counter-argument is higher than for those who use DSS without computer-mediated counter-argument.

Decision satisfaction can be separated into process satisfaction and outcome satisfaction (Green & Taber, 1980). Process satisfaction refers to users’ perceived efficiency and fluency of using a decision support system to facilitate decision making. On the other hand, outcome satisfaction refers to users’ perceived effectiveness and expected performance toward the decision that has been made (Sanders & Courtney, 1985). For subjects in the group with CMCA, the existence of counter-argument challenges their prior assumptions, and this process may lead to negative emotional outcomes. Decision makers have to spend more time on making a decision and the fluency of the decision-making process is low. Negative moods and increased fatigue are caused by interruptions (Schonpflug & Battman, 1988; Zohar, 1999). Therefore, decision makers tend to rate satisfaction with the decision-making outcome as low, even if the interruption is helpful (Speier, Vessey, & Valacich, 2003). However, different results are expected toward

Computer-mediated counter-argument

Learning mental-model building mental-model maintenance

Satisfaction toward process toward outcome

H1

H2

Page 5: Document11

115

the outcome satisfaction. Although the counter-arguments interrupt the decision-making process by allowing decision makers to consider both positive and negative opinions suggested by the system, the decision maker is predicted to report satisfaction toward their decision. Therefore, we hypothesize the following:

H2a: Subjects who use DSS with computer-mediated counter-argument will feel less satisfied about the decision-making process.

H2b: Subjects who use DSS with computer-mediated counter-argument will feel more satisfied about the decision-making outcome.

Research methods An experiment was conducted to test the listed hypotheses. Participants were assigned randomly into two groups, with/without computer-mediated counter-argument. They were asked to accomplish a stock-investment task using the DSS provided by the authors. A stock-investment task was selected because DSS are intensively adopted to support stock investment decision making to cope with high uncertainty and process data in large quantities. In general stock-investment decision-making tasks, subjects are allowed to invest certain amounts of money in one or more selected investing targets. Real decision making is more complex, and the final investment portfolio may contain several combinations of investments. Since this study focuses on understanding learning effects only, a simplified investment environment was constructed. That is, people were requested to invest their money in two companies belonging to different industries instead of among all available companies in the open market. The stock-investment task used in this paper was modified from Melone, McGuire, Hinson, and Yee (1993). Subjects were requested to put the given amount of money into two different stocks. To maximize their return, subjects allocated that money into two different options (a company in the financial holding industry or high-tech industry) based on whatever subset they deemed appropriate. DSS with “what if” analysis was provided as a decision aid. The system provided basic market information and current market status (with news format). Basic market information included the name of company, capital, business scope, expected ROI in the next five years, and stock price for the past five years. Current market status included positive and negative information about these two companies and the industries in which they belong. Information was summarized from articles published in investing-related journals and magazines. Although the investment information presented in this experiment was collected from real financial news, events, and reports, the content was reviewed and modified by two business-school PhD candidates. We removed the names and brand names that appeared in the original news report to avoid any inappropriate connection between the experiment and real-world events. Further, we refined the wording of news titles and news content. The length of the presented news headlines and story bodies were condensed to 10–20 words and 150–200 words, respectively. The content validity was then assured by eight financial professionals, including professors, experienced investors, senior managers in the high-tech industry, and accountants. News with ambiguous titles or content was dropped or modified. The direction (positive or negative) and importance of each piece of news and the accompanying headline was determined based on opinions provided by the above eight financial professionals. Measurement User satisfaction includes two major dimensions: satisfaction toward the system and toward the decision making (Sanders and Courtney, 1985). Since this study focused on the new function, namely counter-argument provided by the DSS, several questions were used to understand users’ attitude toward the DSS. A total of three questions adopted from Sanders and Courtney (1985) were used to measure subjects’ satisfaction with the DSS. On the other hand, three questions adopted from Green and Taber (1980) were used to measure satisfaction toward the decision-making process.

Page 6: Document11

116

Learning in this study refers to, with regard to DSS use, participants’ using the information to challenge their assumptions or using the information to maintain their initial beliefs. A total of six questions obtained from (Vandenbosch & Higgins, 1996) were used to measure learning effects: three for mental-model maintenance and three for mental-model building. The measurement uses a seven-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strong disagree) to 7 (strong agree). Manipulation check Since we wanted to manipulate the effect of with and without counter-argument, we asked subjects whether or not they were aware of the pop-up window showing counter-argument information during the experiment. We found that 90 subjects in the group with CMCA were aware and only two subjects were not; and that 86 subjects in the group without CMCA were not aware (or could not recall) and only nine subjects were. The result of subjects’ awareness is shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Results of subjects’ awareness

Awareness

Total number Yes No Could not recall

Computer-mediated Counter-argument

w/o 9 71 15 95 w 90 1 1 92

Total number 99 72 16 187 According to a reading-behavior study by using eye-tracking machines, in a two-column condition, people tend to focus on content that appears on the left-hand side first. Since the information for these two companies appeared in a two-column format, we randomized the location of the messages to avoid the possible confounding effect. As shown in Table 2, the location of the news and whether or not there was CMCA are independent.

Table 2. Results of subjects in different information displayed location

Location of displayed information Computer-mediated counter-argument

Total number of subjects w/o w

IT-related news on the left-hand side 51 45 96 IT-related news on the right-hand side 44 47 91

Total number of subjects 95 92 187

Table 3. DSS functions (Hung et al., 2007) Menu Function

Market summary Detailed information on the stock market, including stock quotes, change, day’s range, volume, etc.

Industry quotes Detailed information on the specific industries. Company information Provided information on six companies used for trading in the experiment, including basic

information, historical prices, news, and streaming charts. Technical analysis Analyzed technical aspects, such as the moving average, relative strength index, and

stochastic line indicators. Recent news Provided three categories of news: political, international, and financial. Calculator Calculated investment returns and more. System A GUI-based DSS, with 4GL programming language and a relational database management system, was developed for the experiment. The system was able to support Simon’s decision processes, which allow people to perform intelligence, design, and choice activities. Actual design activities followed Hung, Ku, Liang, and Lee’s recommendations (2007), and the detail system tools are shown in Table 3. Furthermore, the purpose of this study was to understand how users’ information-reading behaviors affected their final decision. To support this, a total of 5 to 15 positive and 5 to 15 negative message titles about each company were displayed on the main page for the non-information-loading group. After users clicked on the message title, the system popped up an “always on top” screen,

Page 7: Document11

117

which contained detailed message content. To record the exact reading time for each message, this pop-up “always on top” window could only be closed by clicking the “close” function. Finally, for experiment group, after they clicked the “close” button, another window containing a reversed direction message popped up.

Basic information Technical analysis

Information listing Detailed information

Computer-mediated counter-argument Decision making Figure 2. Screenshots of the experimental system

Procedure Before the experiment started, each participant read and signed a consent form describing the purpose of the research. Subjects were told that any decision they made was independent from their academic performance and that

Page 8: Document11

118

the experiment process was anonymous. All experiments were held by the same facilitator to reduce possibilities of bias. In the first stage, the experimenter gave a five-minute introduction of two investment options (a financial holding company and a high-tech company). After the brief introduction, subjects were asked to make a quick decision first. We consider the decision at this stage as prior belief. Next, what-if analysis tools and the current status of each market were shown on the screen. Positive and negative information was represented by a brief title only. Participants had to click on the title to obtain detailed information. They could use what-if analysis tools to calculate possible returns according to their understanding of the market and the provided information. In this stage, the subjects’ information acquisition behaviors, including reading sequence and time, were recorded by the system automatically. When participants felt comfortable making their final decision, they pressed a button on the screen, and the system led them to the final decision screen. After participants made their final decision, they were asked to provide their level of confidence toward their final decision and answer predefined questions. Finally, they were thanked for their participation. Pilot tests The experiment was pilot-tested twice with master and senior students from the MIS department. After the first pilot test, with eight volunteer students, we refined the process flow and divided the company information page into two pages: one for company information and the other for the preference test. In addition, we invited three students studying human-computer interfaces to review our experiment system and provide us with some functional and interface-design comments. Based on these comments, we improved our system and made its operation smoother and more user-friendly. The second pilot test was conducted with fifteen students. We found no revisions were needed after the second pilot test, so this version was considered final. Demographics A total of 187 subjects were recruited from part-time MBA programs of seven different universities and one academic institution. Since our predefined task was stock investment, we focused on subjects with stock or other investing experience. Five of the six subjects had experience with stock investment, while one of the six had no experience with stock investment but had other investment experience. The average age was 33.1 years old with a standard deviation of 5.6. The eldest subject was 52 years old, and the youngest was 22 years old. For those subjects in the with-CMCA group, one out of five had never invested in the stock market, and the average age was 33.19 years old with a standard deviation of 4.843. The eldest subject was 48 years old, and the youngest was 22 years old. For those subjects in the without-CMCA group, one out of five had never invested in stock, and the average age was 33.01 years old with a standard deviation of 6.325. The eldest subject was 52 years old, and the youngest was 22 years old.

Table 4. Validity and reliability

Variables Items Factor loading

Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor4

Mental-model maintenance (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.74)

Support my viewpoint 0.86 Support action taking 0.77 Reinforce belief 0.70

Mental-model building (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.70)

Challenge my viewpoint 0.82 Criticize my cognition 0.81 Reinvestigate my assumption 0.73

Satisfaction process (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.87)

Process efficiency 0.79 Process satisfaction 0.69 Process clarity 0.68

Satisfaction decision (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.95)

Happy with the decision 0.83 Pleased with the decision 0.82 Satisfied with the decision 0.82

Page 9: Document11

119

Data analysis Item reliability, convergent validity, and discriminant validity tests are often used to test the robustness of measurement. Individual item reliability can be examined by observing the factor loading of each item. Convergent validity should be assured when multiple indicators are used to measure one construct. Convergent validity can be examined by bivariate correlation analyses and reliability of questions. Discriminant validity focuses on testing whether the measures of constructs are different from one another. It can be assessed by testing whether the correlation between pairs of construct are below the threshold value of 0.90. As shown in Table 4, given that all criteria were met, validity and reliability of our measurement are assured in our study. Hypotheses testing There are four hypotheses in this category, based on different decision outcomes: mental-model maintenance, mental-model building, decision-process satisfaction, and outcome satisfaction. In this study, MANOVA was selected because the correlation matrix showed a moderate- to high-level correlation among variables and to control an experiment-wide error rate resulting from conducting multiple ANOVA (Hair, Black, Babin, Anderson & Tatham, 2006). In Table 5, descriptive analysis and the correlation among variables were also provided for the validation of assumption. “Mean” represents the averaged response toward each construct. “Skewness” and “kurtosis” represent the extent to which our sample fit into the normality assumption. The MANOVA test result in Table 6 shows that, except for process satisfaction, CMCA has effects on each dependent variable when confirmation bias is controlled.

Table 5. Descriptive analysis and correlation matrix

Descriptive analysis Correlation matrix

Mean Std dev. Skewness Kurtosis MMM MMB SATP SATD Mental-model maintenance 4.87 0.78 −0.03 1.00 1.00

Mental-model building 5.16 0.87 −0.09 −0.16 −0.09 1.00 Satisfaction toward process 4.97 0.93 −0.71 1.51 0.31 0.45 1.00 Satisfaction toward outcome 5.02 0.97 −0.33 0.51 0.32 0.46 0.78 1.00

Table 6. Effect of CMCA on learning and satisfaction Source Dependent variable F Sig.

Intercept

MM building 6109.007 .000 MM maintenance 7370.924 .000 Process satisfaction 5246.856 .000 Outcome satisfaction 4992.449 .000

CMCA

MM building 4.711 .031 MM maintenance 7.912 .005 Process satisfaction 2.388 .124 Outcome satisfaction 5.868 .016

The hypothesis that the score of mental-model maintenance for subjects who use DSS without CMCA will be higher than for those who use DSS with CMCA is supported. As Table 7 indicates, for subjects in the group without CMCA, the average-rated mental-model maintenance was 5.03 (out of 7). For subjects in the group with CMCA, the average-rated mental-model maintenance score was 4.71 (out of 7). The significant result indicates that CMCA can effectively reduce mental-model maintenance. The hypothesis that the score of mental-model building for subjects who use DSS with CMCA will be higher than for those who use DSS without CMCA is supported. In addition, as Table 7 shows, for subjects in the group without CMCA, the average-rated mental-model building was 4.96 (out of 7); for subjects in the group with CMCA, the average-rated mental-model building was 5.36 (out of 7). The MONOVA result shows that counter-argument has an effect on outcome satisfaction but not on process satisfaction. The hypothesis that subjects who use DSS with CMCA will feel more satisfied toward the system is not

Page 10: Document11

120

supported. However, significant difference can be found between these two groups and H2, and thus, the satisfaction toward decision-making outcome is supported. The average scores and standard deviation for process and outcome satisfaction for each group are provided in Table 7.

Table 7. Analytical results of mental-model maintenance

Computer-mediated counter-argument w/o w

Mental-model maintenance 5.03 (0.88) 4.71 (0.64) Mental-model building 4.96 (0.81) 5.36 (0.87) Process satisfaction 4.88 (0.90) 5.07 (0.96) Outcome satisfaction 4.86 (0.89) 5.19 (1.02) Discussion One of our hypotheses (H2) was not supported. Contrary to our expectation that subjects provided with CMCA would be interrupted by the popping-up of counter-argument during the decision-making process and would tend to feel less satisfied, subjects in the group with counter-argument felt even more satisfied than the group without counter-argument. A couple of reasons may have caused this to happen. First, decision-process satisfaction is measured by a subject’s perceived efficiency, satisfaction, and clearness toward the decision-making process. In general, decision makers do not read all the information provided by the system. Accompanied by the experimenter’s introduction, the decision-making process is very easy to understand and follow. The measurement may not exactly reflect the actual problem with counter-argument. Therefore, there is no significant difference between these two groups. There is a need to develop another instrument to understand subject’s satisfaction toward the decision-making process. Second, we measured the subjects’ perceived process satisfaction and perceived outcome satisfaction together, after the decision had been made. Although the questionnaire focused on the decision-making process, the decision makers’ perceived satisfaction toward the process may have been affected by their perceived satisfaction toward the decision outcome. Therefore, future research should reconsider the most appropriate time for collecting process satisfaction. Conclusion Because human beings have bounded rationality, our decision-making process is largely affected by cognitive biases (Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). This study focused on the introduction of a de-bias function, which may reduce the negative consequences of confirmation bias in the computer-supported decision-making context. The relationships between counter-argument and decision outcomes were hypothesized. After collecting data from 187 experienced financial investors, three out of four proposed hypotheses were supported (the relationship between the providing of CMCA and process satisfaction was not supported). We successfully showed how the de-bias function can trigger individual learning by leading decision makers to challenge their current mental models. This result contributes to academics and practitioners in several ways. Implications for research First, our results indicate that decision support systems with specific functions can stimulate learning by reforming decision makers’ mental model. How users can learn through the use of information systems has long been ignored by information system research (Alavi & Leidner, 2001). In this study, we focused on two different approaches to learning: mental-model maintenance and mental-model building. However, biases emerged while decision makers attempt to maintain current mental models. This research shows that with pre-design functions, users may challenge their initial beliefs and reform their mental models. Second, there was no difference with regard to process satisfaction with or without counter-argument. This indicates that although a new function may be useful to minimize overconfidence and to guide the decision maker to balance

Page 11: Document11

121

their decision, people may be against the de-bias function in the decision-making process. The existence of counter-argument may interrupt decision makers’ information reading and downgrade their processing fluency. This implies that, although counter-argument allows decision makers to challenge their assumptions, an unwanted emotional side effect may also appear. Future research may explore other emotional consequences of using de-biasing functions embedded in computer-supported decision-making tools. Implications for practice Learning takes place when learners enhance their current mental models or build new mental models. It is vital to build an environment to help individuals to avoid cognitive bias as well as to support the building and reforming of mental models. Computer-supported learning is popular and broadly adopted by the contemporary education system. In addition to providing various types of knowledge in a more efficient or effective manner through the support of information technology, approaches such as counter thinking, which provide stimulus for learners to challenge their existing mental model, should be adopted by educators or embedded into the learning system. For a decision-support-systems designer, de-bias functions should be included in the system design. Our study shows that systems should provide certain functions to prevent bias or support learning. However, designers should also note that some unwanted effects may be caused by those de-bias functions. For example, in our study, we provided one piece of counter-argument when subjects read a piece of news. The pop-up counter-argument may lead to negative emotions, for example, annoyance or confusion. Too many counter-arguments or too much information may increase the effect of information overload. Therefore, the frequency and timing used to provide counter-argument is important. System designers should also consider the type of task and other factors. Limitations and future research This study is not without limitations. First, a between-group comparison was conducted and subjective evaluation of perceived mental-model building was used to evaluate the learning effect. Some may argue that although this approach may capture part of the learning effect, a pre- and post-experiment comparison should be made in order to truly reflect how counter-argument changes the mental model. Therefore, further research should extend this study by employing within-individual experiment and examining the change of one’s mental model objectively. Second, this was a cross-sectional study and the existence of counter-argument was unexpected by the subjects. People may get used to the appearance of counter-argument and ignore it automatically (e.g., Handzic & Tolhurst, 2002). Future research may extend this study by conducting a longitudinal study to examine the above assumption. Third, only a financial decision-making task was used. Since confirmation bias can be applied to various decision-making tasks and counter-argument may serve as a useful tool for eliminating it, future research should extend the proposed concept to other decision-making areas to test the effect of counter-argument on decision-making. Fourth, only one de-biasing function was included in this study. One bias may be eliminated through different approaches under different settings. Future research should include other de-biasing functions and compare the effect of different de-biasing functions under different settings. Lastly, although we have illustrated the effect of confirmation bias and methods to prevent it, we did so with a simple experiment design. Future studies may examine this issue in more complicated circumstances, such as a group decision-making or social decision-making process. References Alavi, M., & Leidner, D. E. (2001). Research commentary: Technology-mediated learning: A call for greater depth and breadth of research. Information System Research, 12(1), 1–10.

Argyris, C., & Schön, D. (1978). Organizational Learning: A Theory of Action Approach. Boston, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Arnott, D. (2006). Cognitive biases and decision support systems development: A design science approach. Information Systems Journal, 16(1), 55–78.

Barber, B. M., & Odean, T. (2002). Online investors: Do the slow die first? The Review of Financial Studies, 15(S2), 455–487.

Bhandari, G., Deaves, R., & Hassanein, K. (2008) Debiasing investors with decision support systems: An experimental investigation. Decision Support Systems, 46(1), 399–410.

Page 12: Document11

122

Cannon-Bowers, J. A., Salas, E., & Converse, S. (1993). Shared mental models in expert team decision making. In J. N. J. Castellan (ed.), Individual and group decision making. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 221–246.

Craik, K. J. W. (1943). The Nature of Explanation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Davis, F. D., & Kottemann, J. E. (1994). User perceptions of decision support effectiveness: Two production planning experiments. Decision Sciences, 25(1), 57–76.

Festinger, L. A. (1957). Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Fischhoff, B. (1982) Debiasing. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, 422–444.

Gagne, R. M. 1977. The condition of learning, New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

Green, S. G., & Taber, T. D. (1980). The effects of three social decision schemes on decision group process. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 25(1), 97–106.

Grønhaug, K., & Falkenberg, J. S. (1989). Exploring strategy perceptions in changing environments. Journal of Management Studies, 26(4), 349–359.

Hair, J. F., Black, B., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (2006). Multivariate data analysis, 6th ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Handzic, M., & Tolhurst, D. (2002). Evaluating an interactive learning environment in management education. Educational Technology & Society, 5(3), 113–122.

Heath, C. (1996). Do People Prefer to Pass Along Good or Bad News? Valence and Relevance of News as Predictors of Transmission Propensity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 68(2), 79–94.

Hoch, S. J., & Deighton, J. (1989). Managing What Consumers Learn from Experience. Journal of Marketing, 53(2), 1–20.

Holyoak, K. J. (1984). Analogical thinking and human intelligence. In Advances in the psychology of human intelligence, R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 199–223.

Hung, S. Y., Ku, Y. C., Liang, T. P., & Lee, C. J. (2007). Regret avoidance as a measure of DSS success: An exploratory study. Decision Support Systems, 42(4), 2093–2106.

Kiesler, S. & Sproull, L. (1982). Managerial response to changing environments: Perspectives on problem sensing from social cognition. Administrative Science Quarterly, 27(4), 548–570.

March, P. (1991). How to people with a mild/moderate mental handicap conceptualise physical illness and its cause. British Journal of Mental Subnormality, 37(73), 80–91.

Melone, N. P., McGuire, T. W., Hinson, G. B., & Yee, K. Y. (1993). The effect of decision support systems on managerial performance and decision confidence. Proceedings of the 26th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences, 482–489.

Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2(2), 175–220.

Norman, D. A. (1982). Learning and memory. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman and Company.

Patel, A., Kinshuk, & Russell, D. (2000). Intelligent tutoring tools for cognitive skill acquisition in life long learning. Educational Technology & Society, 3(1), 32–40.

Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child, New York: Basic Books.

Quinn, J. B. (1980). Strategies for change: Logical incrementalism, Homewood, IL: Richard D Irwin.

Rouse, W. & Morris, N. (1986). On looking into the black box: Prospects and limits in the search for mental models. Psychological bulletin, 100(3), 349–363.

Russo, J. E., Medvec, V. H., & Meloy, M. G. (1996). The distortion of information during decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 66(1), 102–110.

Sanders, G. L., & Courtney, J. F. (1985). A field study of organizational factors influencing DSS success. MIS Quarterly, 9(1), 77–93.

Santana, M. (1995). Managerial learning: a neglected dimension in decision support systems. Proceedings of the 28th Annual Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences, 82–91.

Schonpflug, W., & Battmann, W. (1988). The costs and benefits of coping. In S. Fisher & J. Reason (Eds.), Handbook of life stress, cognition, and health, New York: Wiley, 699–713.

Page 13: Document11

123

Sinitsa, K. (2000). Learning individually: A life-long perspective. Educational Technology & Society, 3(1), 17–23.

Speier, C., Vessey, I., & Valacich, J. S. (2003). The effects of interruptions, task complexity and information presentation on computer-supported decision-making performance. Decision Sciences, 34(4), 771–797.

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124–1131.

Vandenbosch, B., & Higgins, C. (1994). The measurement of learning from executive information systems. Proceedings of the Twenty-Seventh Hawaii International Conference on System Science, Hawaii, 572–585.

Wason, P. C. (1960). On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12(3), 129–140.

Zohar, D. (1999). When things go wrong: The effect of daily work hassles on effort, exertion, and negative mood. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 72(3), 265–283.