10 Jaak Panksepp’s Response: Commentary by Clifford Yorke

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/25/2019 10 Jaak Panksepps Response: Commentary by Clifford Yorke

    1/5

    This article was downloaded by: [Gazi University]On: 18 August 2014, At: 06:35Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: MortimerHouse, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

    Neuropsychoanalysis: An Interdisciplinary Journal

    for Psychoanalysis and the NeurosciencesPublication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rnpa20

    Jaak Panksepps Response: Commentary by Clifford

    YorkeClifford Yorke

    a

    aFieldings, South Moreton, Nr. Didcot, Oxon, 0X11 9AH, United Kingdom, e-mail:

    Published online: 09 Jan 2014.

    To cite this article:Clifford Yorke (1999) Jaak Panksepps Response: Commentary by Clifford Yorke,Neuropsychoanalysis: An Interdisciplinary Journal for Psychoanalysis and the Neurosciences, 1:2, 251-254, DOI:

    10.1080/15294145.1999.10773266

    To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15294145.1999.10773266

    PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

    Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the Content) containedin the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make norepresentations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose ofthe Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be reliedupon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shallnot be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and otherliabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to orarising out of the use of the Content.

    This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematicreproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in anyform to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15294145.1999.10773266http://www.tandfonline.com/action/showCitFormats?doi=10.1080/15294145.1999.10773266http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditionshttp://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditionshttp://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15294145.1999.10773266http://www.tandfonline.com/action/showCitFormats?doi=10.1080/15294145.1999.10773266http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rnpa20
  • 7/25/2019 10 Jaak Panksepps Response: Commentary by Clifford Yorke

    2/5

    Ongoing Discussion

    Jaak Panksepp s

    Response: Commentary

    by

    Clifford Yorke

    251

    I appreciate the courteous and considered reply by

    Jaak Panksepp to the points I made in discussing his

    response to the paper by Solms and Nersessian. Be

    fore addressing them, I should like to say, that, to my

    mind, the entire exchange shows every sign that the

    positions taken by the various participants will lead

    to the clarification vital for constructive debate.

    In questioning the feasibility of the experimental

    use of pharmaceutical agents

    in the course

    clinic l

    psycho n lysis

    I was in no way supporting the fenc

    ing-in

    of

    the discipline and arbitrarily precluding

    contacts with other forms of knowledge. If that were

    so, I would not be taking part in this discussion. I am

    entirely in agreement with Panksepp that we

    do

    need

    to probe more deeply into the problem of how the

    widely used psychotherapeutic drugs modify

    the

    emo

    tional dynamics of human personality and other di

    mensions

    of

    the human mind

    p

    178). I firmly

    believe that, if psychoanalysis can help

    to

    craft the

    needed tools (p. 178), it has a compelling duty to

    do so.

    I have looked once more at the relevant part

    of

    my commentary, to try to see whether it invited misun

    derstanding, but concluded that the difficulty was per

    haps

    of

    another kind. One can be familiar, even very

    familiar, with the main tenets of psychoanalysis but,

    without personal experience of the treatment process

    itself, it may be hard to appreciate the nature of the

    clinical setting in which psychoanalysts are obliged to

    operate. It may be helpful for those participants who

    lack that experience if I try to say something about

    both the setting and its rationale. It may help to reduce

    misunderstanding. If what follows seems a digression,

    I believe it to be a necessary one.

    Normally, the patient lies

    supine

    on a couch. This

    may afford a helpful degree of

    physic l

    relaxation, but

    it has other, far-reaching, effects. It fosters a measure

    of helplessness in the face of professional authority,

    however willingly the patient accepts the conditions.

    The analyst is out of sight, sitting

    behind

    the patient,

    who has nothing more stimulating to look at than the

    ceiling (usually blank). Complete privacy means that

    the telephone is inoperative and no interruptions al-

    Clifford Yorke, F.R.C. Psych., D.P.M., is a Training and Supervising

    Analyst, British Psychoanalytic Society; Honorary Consultant Psychiatrist,

    Anna Freud Centre, London.

    lowed. External stimuli are further reduced by the lack

    of

    social interchange: the analyst is, for the most part,

    listening: his occasional comments are designed only

    to foster the patient s self-understanding or to further

    the analytic process. The basic rule requires the

    patient

    to try

    to say everything that comes to mind,

    regardless of whether or not it seems appropriate, in

    context, or personally or socially acceptable. This

    free association, in the context of the analytic set

    ting, largely restricts the patient s attention to his own

    mental processes, so that the capacity to test reality

    is reduced or partially suspended. The restriction of

    motility imposed by the patient s physical position

    means that mental excitation cannot normally be dis

    charged (or gratified) in action, but mainly through

    recollection, fantasy, and affect. All these circum

    stances facilitate regression-a partial return to earlier

    stages of psychological development, a pull toward

    childhood-and

    give the primitive thought processes

    characteristic of the unconscious a greater influence

    on current experience. It is that influence that usually

    gives every analytic hour an underlying theme.

    It may be worth adding that a few patients are

    too frightened to lie down or,

    if

    they do, find the degree

    of

    regression so deep that it is hard for them to get up

    at the end

    of

    the session and at once resume normal

    adult functioning. The technical methods

    of

    dealing

    with these difficulties need not concern us here; I sim

    ply want to emphasize the potential power

    of

    the re

    gressive pull. It also has a bearing on some further

    points to be made after a brief consideration of the

    analyst s role.

    For the analyst, too, is restricted in what he can

    say or do. He must say nothing that can interfere with

    the patient s focus on his own mental processes. For

    that reason, he cannot talk about himself or his real

    life; to do so would inhibit or interfere with the pa

    tient s fantasies about him. Questions are distracting,

    and are rarely asked (analytic patience will generally

    supply the answer in due course). Neither (within the

    limits

    of

    common sense) will the patient s questions

    be answered: the reason

    why

    the question is asked is

    by no means always conscious, and is generally more

    important for the analysis than any formal answer.

    The analyst makes only interventions that further the

    analytic work-in particular, comments that tend to

    bring unconscious material into consciousness. As far

  • 7/25/2019 10 Jaak Panksepps Response: Commentary by Clifford Yorke

    3/5

    s

    possible, he tries to avoid suggestion, above all,

    listening in a very special way. The analyst listens

    withfree floating attention

    trying not to attach greater

    importance to

    anyone

    of the patient s utterances than

    he does to another, and makes no conscious effort to

    remember.

    This method of listening means that, time

    and time again, relevant information from earlier ses

    sions sometimes weeks or months before--comes to

    mind unbidden, and acts as a pointer to the less con

    scious implications of what the patient is saying. The

    analyst is, of course, listening with an analyzed mind

    that facilitates an understanding

    of

    the underlying

    meaning of what the patient has said.

    It should not be thought that free association

    (which is not in

    fact

    free but motivated) will itself

    convey, directly, unconscious material. The patient is

    nothing

    if

    not

    conflicted.

    Mental content that the pa

    tient was unable to tolerate in everyday life will face

    opposition from psychological defenses, and normally

    these defenses are themselves unconscious. For that

    reason, a defense will generally need to be addressed

    before the drive representative behind it can be

    brought into awareness. As for affects, these can only

    be conscious. They do not, however, always present

    themselves when they might be expected to do so To

    take a rather vivid example from an actual analysis: a

    patient told his analyst that he would like to break

    every bone in his (the analyst s) body, but spoke with

    no evidence of anger, let alone rage. The analyst said

    that the patient would perhaps feel very frightened

    if

    he were able to feel that he really wanted to do him

    so much damage. He would not have told the patient

    that he did feel something when the patient knew very

    well that he didn t. Dealing with the inhibition of the

    processes by which affects reach awareness often calls

    for slow and patient work. There are other defenses

    against affective awareness: these are different in kind

    from defenses against the mental content of drive rep

    resentatives.

    Mental content unaccompanied by affective ex

    perience has its counterpart in affect seemingly di

    vorced from mental content. One can feel furious, for

    example, without recognizing the object

    of

    that fury.

    In that case, the technique

    of

    interpretation is aimed

    at the uncovering of the unconscious mental represen

    tations.

    The pull back to childhood plays a major part in

    the development

    of

    that vital agent in analytic work,

    tr nsference the reexperience, in the present, of feel

    ings, thoughts, and attitudes toward the analyst more

    appropriate to parental and other important figures

    from the past. It may be useful to say a few words

    Clifford Yorke

    about this remarkable phenomenon. Perhaps the first

    thing to say is that spontaneous transference occurs in

    everyday life. Some people approach any figure of

    authority with attitudes that may, however remotely,

    derive from a childhood attitude to a father. A bank

    manager may be approached

    s

    a benign paternal fig-

    ure who can be relied upon to look after his client s

    best interests, or

    s

    a somewhat daunting character

    only too likely to turn down a request for a loan. Or

    someone may, almost instantly, dislike a woman who

    is too reminiscent of unacceptable maternal traits or,

    conversely, instantly like her for opposite reasons. The

    person concerned may not know why certain people

    bring out strong likes or dislikes even when there has

    been

    no

    time

    to

    get to know them. But spontaneous

    transference can be

    of

    any degree

    of

    intensity.

    In analysis, spontaneous transference is there

    from the beginning. It is affected by the patient s

    hopes, expectations, and conscious and unconscious

    fantasies about the analyst to whom he is entrusting so

    much. The illness and the wish to get better contribute

    strongly

    to

    its nature and quality, but the affective

    component is usually substantial. Furthermore,

    if

    a

    patient s need for love is unmet in reality or not fully

    satisfied, the analyst or, for that matter, any new per

    son in the patient s life, will be met by what Freud

    (1912) called libidinal anticipatory ideas. There is

    a readiness for transference. But, as the analysis gets

    under way, the transference becomes more complex

    and increasingly centered around the person

    of

    the

    analyst. A full explanation of this phenomenon is be

    yond brief summary: but the two most important fac

    tors are regression (discussed above) and the

    repetition compulsion (Freud, 1914, 1920, 1925). The

    compulsion

    to

    repeat earlier modes of attachment and

    behavior reflects a conservative trend in the organism:

    older methods of adaptation are preferred at the ex

    pense of the new. It is most readily observed in chil

    dren s play and in their insistence that the same story,

    when repeated, must be told in the same way. A dis

    tressing form of the repetition compulsion is seen in

    traumatic neurosis. The traumatizing incident is lived

    (or dreamed) over and over again: each time, the over

    whelming excitation aroused by that event is fraction

    ally discharged. It is usually a minute step toward

    recovery. But, in general, the conservative trend in the

    repetition compulsion is rarely absolute:

    if

    it were

    there would be no such thing

    s

    progressive develop

    ment and certainly no capacity for adventure. There

    are people who are terrified of anything new, but they

    may

    find

    psychoanalysis rather too novel for their

    tastes. But the repetition compulsion is real enough: I

  • 7/25/2019 10 Jaak Panksepps Response: Commentary by Clifford Yorke

    4/5

    Ongoing Discussion

    was particularly impressed when I met a patient who

    had twice married tuberculous husbands and nursed

    each through to his death.

    The level

    of

    regression varies in different disor

    ders. But, generally, drive organization (the mental

    organization structured around the drive representa

    tives and derivatives at any given phase of develop

    ment) reverts to a point at which, from the standpoint

    of personal history, there was a particularly strong

    drive attachment to an infantile object. That has im

    portant consequences for the nature

    of

    what can prop

    erly be called the transference neurosis. Similarly, the

    nature and quality of ego regression and superego re

    gression varies, too, even at different phases of the

    same analysis. The analyst may, for example, be seen

    s an external source of proscription (or support), like

    the parental figure of a 3-year-old-well before the

    superego has been fully internalized. All this will be

    variously reflected in the transference.

    I have not discussed here what has been called

    countertr nsference Classically this referred to the

    analyst s unconscious responses to the patient stem

    ming from his own past, to be dealt with by self-scru

    tiny and, when called for, some self-analysis. Today,

    its meaning is more controversial, and I omit it from

    this brief account.

    Some years ago, I tried to examine these pro

    cesses from the standpoint

    of

    metapsychology (Yorke,

    1965): that attempt needs updating, and I hope to re

    turn to the subject elsewhere. It is a crucial matter for

    psychoanalysts, since there can be

    no

    sound theory of

    technique without a clarification

    of

    these important

    issues. We need to know, for example, why and how

    interpretation works, or fails to work, and what adap

    tations of technique are called for in widely differing

    types of disorder. (The remarks made above

    o

    not,

    for example, apply to children and

    to

    those on the

    border

    of

    psychosis.) On this occasion I have tried to

    do no more than give a bare outline of how an analysis

    is conducted, in the hope that it will further discus

    sions between our disciplines. It has, I believe, a legiti

    mate claim to mutual interest. The metapsychology

    of

    the treatment process, of the method by which, after

    all, psychoanalysis has made most of its discoveries,

    is something we all need to understand better from our

    two perspectives.

    I return to Jaak Panksepp s comments. Pharma

    ceutical agents cannot be administered in the course

    of psychoanalytic treatment without interfering with

    the necessary conditions without which the treatment

    cannot operate. In my original commentary I said that

    if drugs were used which divorced thought from af-

    5

    fect, associations would lose their power to convey

    information without which the analyst would not be

    able to operate. I believe that statement to be gener

    ally true, but it is not meant to set aside considerations

    of

    common sense. Patients do, occasionally, turn up

    under the influence (say)

    of

    alcohol, for reasons that

    have nothing to do with experiment, and we can ob

    serve the effects. The analysis will effectively be sabo

    taged, and the reasons for the behavior (e.g., a negative

    transference reaction and/or self-destructiveness) can

    not be analyzed either. I once tried to treat a heroin

    addict who attempted a old turkey recovery, but

    s soon as she ran into painful experiences she re

    turned to the needle and analysis was impossible. On

    the other hand, a patient referred by a psychiatrist who

    was seriously anergic and unable to talk until treated

    by antidepressants would,

    if

    she wanted self-under

    standing, become treatable if her background mood

    had returned to something like its norm.

    These examples are meant to do no more than

    point to the fact that drugs can make treatment possi

    ble or quite impossible. The experimental use of phar

    maceutical agents is a very different matter. It is a

    safe general rule that the analyst should never himself

    prescribe. To do so would require him to step outside

    his analytical role, with unpredictable consequences.

    The transference relationship would be seriously inter

    fered with; the analyst, however well intentioned, is,

    one way or another, directly acting on the patient s

    body. That may be fine if

    the patient is consulting a

    doctor for a physical disability, but that is not the view

    he will take of

    his analyst s responsibilities. The pa

    tient may resent that interference, at least uncon

    sciously, even if he has given formal consent. The

    unconscious being what it is, he may see the drug s

    an elixir of life, as a poison, or indeed anything else.

    His emotional response will vary accordingly. What

    ever else, he will feel, correctly, that the analyst has

    stepped out

    of

    his proper role. Analysis, at best, is a

    delicate undertaking and, as I hope my brief resume

    of the method has suggested, calls for the reduction

    of

    external stimuli and interference to the lowest possible

    level. I will not repeat here the objections I voiced in

    previously published material.

    This difference of opinion with Panksepp only

    applies, however, to the use of drugs during formal

    psychoanalysis itself, and it is likely that psychoana

    lytically informed methods

    of

    investigation could be

    devised. Solms and Nersessian, in their concluding

    remarks (p. 224), make suggestions which I would

    endorse. If, as I believe, Panksepp is in accord with

  • 7/25/2019 10 Jaak Panksepps Response: Commentary by Clifford Yorke

    5/5

    5

    these suggestions, there is no good reason why there

    should be any significant disagreement on this score.

    On the question

    o

    drives, and their link with

    affects, I find little to add to what has already been

    said. The differences between Panksepp and myself

    have been diminished, I believe, but there is room for

    further discussion. Although Freud included hunger

    and thirst among the self-preservative instincts in

    his earlier formulations, appetitive strivings ought not

    to be

    equated

    with drives in the psychoanalytic sense.

    The somatic oral erotogenic zone is not in itself the

    source

    o a biological need. There is much to debate

    in matters o this kind. In respect o the drives, I find

    the comments o Solms and Nersessian very much to

    the point. On this and the question o affects there are

    a number

    o

    psychoanalytic tools that can be used to

    examine the effects

    o

    drugs prescribed for validmedi

    cal reasons. The concept o developmental lines (A.

    Freud, 1963) is one o them. But it is worth remember

    ing that we can reach a very useful provisional psy

    choanalytic diagnostic assessment o patients with all

    kinds o disabilities, including serious physical defects

    that affect psychological functioning, by the use o

    Anna Freud's profile schema A Freud, 1962). It can

    be applied to children, adults, psychotic patients (Free

    man, 1973), and to drug-induced conditions. A pilot

    study

    o

    ten severe heroin addicts was made many

    years ago (Wiseberg, Yorke, and Radford, 1973). The

    status

    o

    the profile in relation to the concept

    o

    devel

    opmental lines has been discussed elsewhere (Yorke,

    1980).

    Studies

    o this kind still fall within the ideo

    graphic rather than the nomothetic approaches to sci

    ence. I was interested in Panksepp's responses to my

    comments on this distinction, and from the nomothetic

    standpoint neuropsychology can be expected to offer

    us valuable information. The clinico-anatomical

    method has, however, already given us invaluable

    work without recourse to mensuration. As for aca-

    Clifford Yorke

    demic psychology, I am less enthusiastic than Pank

    sepp about the value o rating scales. In all this I

    would, however, echo the reminder, by our editors,

    that a psychological model only becomes accessible

    to

    physical investigation once the neural correlates of

    the components o the model have been identified

    p 213). Success may only be partial in the foresee

    able future, but I cannot think we are aiming at impos

    sible goals.

    References

    Freeman, T. (1973), The metapsychological profile schema.

    In: The Psychoanalytic Study

    of

    the Psychoses. New

    York: International Universities Press.

    Freud,

    A

    (1962), Assessment

    o

    childhood disturbances.

    The Psychoanalytic Study

    of

    the Child 17:149-158. New

    York: International Universities Press.

    (1963), The concept o developmental lines. The

    Psychoanalytic Study

    of

    the Child 18:245-265. New

    York: International Universities Press.

    Freud, S (1914), Remembering, repeating and working

    through (Further recommendations on the technique o

    psycho-analysis, II). Standard Edition

    12:

    145-156.

    London: Hogarth Press, 1958.

    (1920), Beyond the Pleasure Principle. Standard

    Edition 18:1-64. London: Hogarth Press, 1955.

    (1925), An Autobiographical Study. Standard Edi-

    tion 20:1-74. London: Hogarth Press, 1959.

    Wiseberg, S., Yorke, C., Radford, P (1973), Aspects o

    self-cathexis in mainline heroin addiction. In: Studies

    in

    Child Psychoanalysis Pure and Applied

    Mongr. 5.

    New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Yorke,

    C

    (1965), Some metapsychological aspects

    o

    inter

    pretation. Brit.

    Med. Psycho

    ,

    38:127-142.

    Clifford Yorke

    Fieldings

    South Moreton

    r Didcot Oxon

    OX]]

    H

    United Kingdom

    e mail: [email protected]