49
Self-Regulation and Career Success - 1 - 1. Introduction The job market is undergoing large-scale changes (e.g., globalisation, declining job se- curity, discontinuous employments, life-long learning), and individual careers are changing as well. A number of new constructs have been introduced to account for these changes, such as the concepts of boundary-less career (see Arthur, Khapova, & Wilderom, 2005; Arthur & Rousseau, 1996) or protean career (Hall, 2002). A common core in these different conceptu- alisations of contemporary forms of occupational careers is the assumption that there is a high need for individuals to actively manage their careers by setting themselves goals and trying to attain those goals (Allred, Snow, & Miles, 1996; Murphy & Ensher, 2001). It is there- fore not suprising that interest in conceptualising individual careers from a perspective of self-regulation is growing (e.g., Kanfer, 2005; King, 2004; Vancouver, 2000; Vancouver & Day, 2005; Wood, 2005; Zimmerman, 2000). Despite the current popularity of the self- regulatory perspective respective research on career success is scarce. A recent meta- analysis on determinants of career success (Ng, Eby, Sorensen & Feldman, 2005) listed not a single study on this topic. Self-regulation refers to thoughts, feelings, and actions that are planned and adapted to the attainment of personal goals (Zimmerman, 2000). The idea that people have the power to actively control their careers through such purposeful thought is fascinating (e.g. Bandura, 1997). The present research empirically addresses this idea. We consider two central com- ponents of self-regulatory thoughts, namely beliefs in own capacities and mastery versus prestige related career goals. Self-regulation, however, is not a one-way process from thought to action but rather a cyclical one (Vancouver, 2000; Vancouver & Day, 2005; Zimmerman, 2000). Goals and be- liefs not only influence behaviors and outcomes; behaviors and outcomes affect goals and beliefs as well. Accordingly, the second purpose of the present research is the analysis of a reciprocal influence of self-regulation and career success. What is career success? Is it money and promotion or is it satisfaction and positive evaluation? In order to adequately address the questions we posed above, the complex con-

1. Introduction well. A number of new constructs have been ...Previous research revealed that the determinants of objective vs. subjective success Self-Regulation and Career Success

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 1 -

    1. Introduction

    The job market is undergoing large-scale changes (e.g., globalisation, declining job se-

    curity, discontinuous employments, life-long learning), and individual careers are changing as

    well. A number of new constructs have been introduced to account for these changes, such

    as the concepts of boundary-less career (see Arthur, Khapova, & Wilderom, 2005; Arthur &

    Rousseau, 1996) or protean career (Hall, 2002). A common core in these different conceptu-

    alisations of contemporary forms of occupational careers is the assumption that there is a

    high need for individuals to actively manage their careers by setting themselves goals and

    trying to attain those goals (Allred, Snow, & Miles, 1996; Murphy & Ensher, 2001). It is there-

    fore not suprising that interest in conceptualising individual careers from a perspective of

    self-regulation is growing (e.g., Kanfer, 2005; King, 2004; Vancouver, 2000; Vancouver &

    Day, 2005; Wood, 2005; Zimmerman, 2000). Despite the current popularity of the self-

    regulatory perspective respective research on career success is scarce. A recent meta-

    analysis on determinants of career success (Ng, Eby, Sorensen & Feldman, 2005) listed not

    a single study on this topic.

    Self-regulation refers to thoughts, feelings, and actions that are planned and adapted to

    the attainment of personal goals (Zimmerman, 2000). The idea that people have the power to

    actively control their careers through such purposeful thought is fascinating (e.g. Bandura,

    1997). The present research empirically addresses this idea. We consider two central com-

    ponents of self-regulatory thoughts, namely beliefs in own capacities and mastery versus

    prestige related career goals.

    Self-regulation, however, is not a one-way process from thought to action but rather a

    cyclical one (Vancouver, 2000; Vancouver & Day, 2005; Zimmerman, 2000). Goals and be-

    liefs not only influence behaviors and outcomes; behaviors and outcomes affect goals and

    beliefs as well. Accordingly, the second purpose of the present research is the analysis of a

    reciprocal influence of self-regulation and career success.

    What is career success? Is it money and promotion or is it satisfaction and positive

    evaluation? In order to adequately address the questions we posed above, the complex con-

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 2 -

    struct of career success has to be defined more thoroughly. Career success refers to “the

    real or perceived achievements individuals have accumulated as a result of their work ex-

    periences” (Judge, Higgins, Thorensen, & Barrick, 1999, p. 621). Objective career success

    reflects verifiable attainments like pay, position, and promotions. Subjective career success

    emphasizes the beholder’s subjective evaluation (cf. Dette, Abele & Renner, 2004; Heslin,

    2003, 2005; Judge, Cable, Boudreau & Bretz, 1995; Nicholson & De Waal-Andrews, 2005).

    Do self-regulatory thoughts influence subjective success, objective success, or both compo-

    nents? Do reciprocal influences of career outcomes on self-regulatory thoughts emerge for

    both objective and subjective outcomes or are they limited to one of them?

    In sum, we are interested in the career determinants that are in our heads, and in how

    purposeful thought influences career progression. More specifically, we are interested in the

    influence that our work-related beliefs and goals have on career progression, and in how

    they are affected by objective and subjective career success. Knowing more about such de-

    terminants and consequences of career success is of interest for theorizing on self-

    regulation, it is of interest for theorizing on career development, and it may help to resolve

    questions of career planning, career counseling, and personnel selection. We will test our

    assumptions in a prospective three-year, three-wave longitudinal study with a large sample

    of professionals from different occupational fields.

    1.1 Self-Regulation, Performance, and Career Success

    According to Bandura (1986, 1997, 2001), self-regulation is initiated by personal goal

    setting and comprises self-related processes of observation, evaluation, and reaction (see

    also Carver & Scheier, 1990; Karoly, Boekaerts & Maes, 2005; Vancouver, 2000; Vancouver

    & Day, 2005; Wood, 2005; Zimmerman, 2000). Goals as aims of an action (Locke & Latham,

    2002) or internally represented desired states (Austin & Vancouver, 1996) are hierarchically

    organized and range from biological set points (e.g., body temperature) to complex cognitive

    depictions of desired outcomes (e.g., career success). Goals influence outcomes by directing

    attention, mobilizing effort, affecting persistence, and structuring behavior. Goals predict per-

    formance above and beyond cognitive ability (e.g. Austin & Vancouver, 1996; Locke &

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 3 -

    Latham, 2002; Payne, Youngcourt, and Beaubien, 2007). The present question about the

    impact of goals on career success refers to the content of goals. Despite many differences,

    most conceptualizations of goal content share one core distinction that is of relevance here

    (cf. Austin & Vancouver, 1996; Dweck, 1986; Furnham, Petrides, Tsaousis, Pappas & Gar-

    rod, 2005; Hazer & Alvares, 1981; Locke & Latham, 2002; Nicholls, 1984; Super, 1957; Zy-

    towsky, 1994). It is the distinction between more “intrinsic”, mastery, learning, growth, and

    intellectual stimulation related goals and goal orientations vs. more “extrinsic”, power, per-

    formance prove, materialistic and prestige related goals and goal orientations (Dweck, 1986;

    Furnham et al., 2005; Hazer & Alvares, 1981; Locke & Latham, 2002; Nicholls, 1984; Super,

    1957, 1970; VandeWalle, 1997; VandeWalle, Brown, Cron & Slocum, 1999). We will draw on

    this distinction here.

    Another central component of self-regulation are beliefs in one’s capacity to perform

    some behavior or to meet a standard, e.g., self-efficacy beliefs (Bandura, 1986, 1997). Ac-

    cording to Bandura, individuals with high self-efficacy beliefs set higher goals for themselves,

    put in more effort, and persist longer on a difficult task. Self-efficacy beliefs significantly con-

    tribute to the level of motivation and performance above and beyond cognitive ability (Ban-

    dura & Locke, 2003).

    1.2 Self-efficacy, goal content, performance, and success

    Because there is only very little research on self-efficacy, goal content, and career suc-

    cess, we will briefly consider research on the related issue of performance. Meta-analyses on

    cross-sectional and experimental studies show medium size positive correlations of self-

    efficacy with task performance (Bandura & Locke, 2003; Judge & Bono, 2001; Judge, Jack-

    son, Shaw, Scott & Rich, 2007; Locke & Latham, 1990; Sadri & Robertson, 1993; Stajkovic &

    Luthans, 1998). Cross-sectional field studies revealed that self-efficacy is associated with,

    e.g., sales performance (Barling & Beattie, 1983), research productivity (Kahn & Scott, 1997;

    Vasil, 1992), job performance (Day & Allen, 2004; Lubbers, Loughlin & Zweig, 2005), and

    external performance ratings (Riggs, Warka, Babasa, Betancourt & Hooker, 1994; Judge,

    Thoresen, Pucik & Welbourne, 1999). However, a meta-analysis by Judge et al. (2007) sug-

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 4 -

    gests that the effects of self-efficacy on performance might be much weaker when individual

    difference measures are taken into account. Schwoerer and May (1996) found no effect of

    employees’ self-efficacy on external performance ratings, and recent research by Vancouver

    and Kendall (2006) as well as Yeo and Neal (2006) even found a negative influence of self-

    efficacy on performance if measured on an intra-individual level. Brett and VandeWalle

    (1999) found that both goal orientation and goal content predicted performance in a training

    program. Research by Harackiewicz and colleagues (Harackiewicz, Barron, Carter, Lehto &

    Elliot, 1997; Barron & Harackiewicz, 2001) tested the influence of multiple goals and re-

    vealed that both learning goals and performance goals were important for performance and

    optimal motivation.

    Regarding career success, two cross-sectional studies reported a relationship between

    self-efficacy and indicators of objective career success, e.g., pay and hierarchical position

    (Day & Allen, 2004; Lubbers, Loughlin & Zweig, 2005). Longitudinal research by Saks (1995)

    revealed that task-related self-efficacy had a positive effect on job satisfaction. Frieze, Olson,

    Murrell, and Selvan (2006) showed that both materialistic, prestige-related work values (e.g.,

    wanting to be recognized in one’s field, wanting high pay) and achievement-related work

    values (e.g., wanting to do an excellent job) predicted later salary. Abele and Stief (2004)

    found that people with higher self-efficacy expectation were more successful in their career

    entry than those with lower self-efficacy expectations.

    1.3 Reciprocal influence of performance and success on self-efficacy and goals

    The theoretical notion of a feed-forward mechanism suggests that goals lead to activi-

    ties for attaining them, and that goal-attainment leads to adapted goal-setting and adapted

    self-evaluation which instigate further efforts to attain the adapted goals (Bandura, 1986,

    2001; Bandura & Locke, 2003). Hence, self-efficacy and goals should both be somewhat

    stable, but also malleable by situational influences and long-term experiences. Research

    shows that self-efficacy can be altered directly and without behavioral feedback (cf. Bandura

    & Locke, 2003). However, behavioral feedback has an influence as well. Self-efficacy beliefs

    increase after success and decrease after failure (cf. Cervone et al., 2004; Shim & Ryan,

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 5 -

    2005; Smith, Kass, Rotunda & Schneider, 2006). Stability coefficients of self-efficacy vary

    from .57 to .90 depending on the time interval between measures (Dormann, Fay, Zapf &

    Frese, 2006; Shim & Ryan, 2005). Goals are adjusted to performance feedback (difficulty:

    Ilies & Judge, 2005; goal orientation: Radosevich, Vaidyanathan, Yeo and Radosevich,

    2004). However, we know little about the malleability of goal content (e.g. Hazer & Alvares,

    1981; stability coefficients for goal orientations at time intervals ranging from 1 to 14 weeks

    are about .60, Payne et al., 2007).

    2. Present Research

    The studies summarized so far provide some evidence that self-efficacy has an impact

    on performance, even though the evidence is less conclusive than one might expect given

    the prominence of the construct. There is also research showing that goal content matters. In

    some contexts “intrinsic” goals seem to have more influence on performance, but in many

    other contexts both “intrinsic” and “extrinsic” goals are important. Finally, there is some evi-

    dence for a reciprocal influence of performance on self-efficacy and on goal attributes.

    Evidence regarding the relationship between self-efficacy, goal content, and career suc-

    cess is very limited (Abele & Stief, 2003; Day & Allen, 2004; Frieze et al., 2006; Lubbers et

    al., 2005; Saks, 1995). Almost no research has addressed a longitudinal influence of self-

    efficacy on objective career success. This seems especially warranted since the research

    cited above (Vancouver & Kendall, 2006; Yeo & Neal, 2006) showed that self-efficacy may

    also have a negative influence on performance if it is measured in a within-participants de-

    sign. One study found a longitudinal influence of goal content on career success (Frieze et

    al., 2006). This evidence is not enough to firmly establish whether “intrinsic” and “extrinsic”

    goal content are equally important in predicting career success or whether one is more im-

    portant than the other. Moreover, there are no studies analyzing the influence of self-efficacy

    and goal content on both objective and subjective aspects of career success. However, a

    differentiation between these different facets of success is important for our general question

    whether self-regulatory thoughts mainly have an influence on subjective parameters of suc-

    cess. Previous research revealed that the determinants of objective vs. subjective success

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 6 -

    are somewhat different (Dette et al., 2004; Ng et al., 2005) and that subjective success is

    more strongly tied to individual difference variables than objective success. Finally, data on

    the reciprocal influence of career success on self-efficacy and goal content are lacking. The

    aim of the present study is to fill these research gaps.

    For the present research we conceptualize objective career success by pay and respon-

    sibility level. Subjective career success can be differentiated further (Dette et al., 2004; Hes-

    lin, 2003, 2005). Self-referent subjective success—the most common conceptualization of

    subjective success—refers to an evaluation against own standards, for instance satisfaction

    with one’s career (Greenhaus, Parasuraman, & Wormley, 1990). Other-referent subjective

    success refers to a comparison of one’s career with a comparison standard, for instance the

    career of another person or group (see Dette, et al., 2004; Heslin, 2003; Turban & Dough-

    erty, 1994). This latter approach has only rarely been used despite the fact that people have

    a tendency to compare their actions and outcomes to those of other people, especially in

    areas in which they have to evaluate a “social” reality (Festinger, 1954). The present re-

    search will employ such an other-referent measure of subjective success.

    Since we are interested in outcomes related to occupational career, we will not analyze

    general self-efficacy, but rather occupational self efficacy (Abele, Stief & Andrä, 2000) which

    is on a medium level of specificity. A medium level of specificity is advantageous in predicting

    specific outcomes (Chen, Gully & Eden, 2001; Pajares, 1996).1 Regarding the two classes of

    goal content that we broadly distinguished above, we conceptualize goals in the tradition of

    work values (Super, 1970; Zytowski, 1994). Mastery goals comprise goals related to intellec-

    tual stimulation and growth. Materialistic and prestige-oriented goals comprise goals directed

    at pay, promotions, and upward mobility. Finally, because we are interested in the pure ef-

    fects of self-efficacy beliefs and occupational goals on career success, it is important to con-

    trol for individual differences in performance level. Hence, we will also consider our partici-

    pants’ grade point average (GPA) as a control variable.

    Figure 1 graphically illustrates the hypotheses. We assume that occupational self-

    efficacy has a positive influence on both objective and subjective career success and that

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 7 -

    both classes of goal content have a positive influence on carreer success as well. Further-

    more, we assume that these self-regulatory variables have a direct influence on objective

    career success that is not mediated by the subjective perception of success. We further as-

    sume that occupational self-efficacy and mastery goals are correlated (see Payne, et al.,

    2007; Seijts, Latham, Tasa, & Latham, 2004).

    Hypothesis 1. Occupational self-efficacy influences objective career success.

    Hypothesis 2. Mastery goals influence objective career success.

    Hypothesis 3. Materialistic, prestige-oriented goals influence objective career success.

    Hypothesis 4. Occupational self-efficacy influences subjective career success.

    Hypothesis 5. Mastery goals influence subjective career success.

    Hypothesis 6. Materialistic, prestige-oriented goals influence subjective career success.

    Regarding reciprocal influences one could argue that only subjective success matters,

    because people will only adapt their beliefs and goals, if they feel successful. However, one

    could also argue that objective success matters, because it is the basis for adaptation. We

    will test the hypothesis that both facets are important.

    Hypothesis 7. Objective career success leads to changes in occupational self-efficacy, mas-

    tery goals, and prestige goals.

    Hypothesis 8. Subjective career success leads to changes in occupational self-efficacy,

    mastery goals, and prestige goals.

    2.1 Objective and subjective career success

    Although objective and subjective career-related outcome criteria tend to be positively

    correlated, the size of these correlations is moderate at best. The meta-analysis by Dette et

    al. (2004) revealed an estimated correlation of .28 between objective career success and

    subjective career success. Similarly, the meta-analysis by Ng et al. (2005) found a correla-

    tion of .30 between pay and career satisfaction, and a correlation of .22 between promotions

    and career satisfaction. Our research allows a test of whether the relationship between both

    variables is merely correlational or whether there is a directional influence. If objective career

    success and subjective career success are merely correlated, then the longitudinal influence

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 8 -

    of objective career success on subjective career success should be completely mediated by

    prior subjective career success, and the longitudinal influence of subjective career success

    on objective career success should be completely mediated by prior objective career suc-

    cess. However, if there is a directional influence of objective career success on subjective

    career success or vice versa, then there should be no mediation.

    2.2 Gender

    It is critical for research on career success to consider gender, because women’s and

    men’s career experiences are different; in general women are objectively less successful

    than men (Abele, 2003; Greene & DeBacker, 2004; Kirchmeyer, 1998; Ng et al., 2005). Our

    present aim is not to fully explain why women, on average, are objectively less successful

    than men. We rather strive to analyze possible mediating effects of occupational self-

    efficacy, mastery goals, and prestige goals. In the past, women tended to have lower self-

    efficacy than men (Betz & Fitzgerald, 1987), but this difference has become smaller over

    time (Bandura & Wood, 1989; Greene & DeBacker, 2004; Philips & Imhoff, 1997; Schwoerer

    & May, 1996; Silver, Mitchell & Gist, 1995). Findings on gender differences in work related

    goals are inconclusive (Frieze et al. 2006; Furnham et al., 2005).

    Hypothesis 9. Gender has a direct effect on objective career success: Women are less suc-

    cessful than men.

    Hypothesis 10. If there are gender differences in occupational self-efficacy, mastery goals,

    and/or prestige goals, they should partly mediate women’s lower objective career success.

    We assume smaller or even no gender differences in subjective career success. Accord-

    ing to the shifting standards model (Biernat & Billings, 2001), the use of subjective scales

    leads to reduced differences between groups due to the application of different anchors.

    Women may implicitly use a lower anchor in assessing their success than men. Using a

    lower anchor, however, leads to similar assessments of subjective success despite lower

    objective success.

    3. Method

    3.1 Overview

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 9 -

    We tested our hypotheses with data collected in a prospective longitudinal study with a

    large sample of professionals who had graduated from a German university (see also Abele,

    2003; Abele & Stief, 2003). Two cohorts of graduates completed the first questionnaire some

    weeks after they had passed their final exams. They received the second questionnaire 17

    months later and the third one 3 years after graduation. We conducted the present research

    with a selection of the measures taken in these three questionnaires.

    3.2 Participants and Procedure

    Participants were representative with respect to gender, study major, and GPA for the

    two cohorts we looked at. We did not find any cohort effects or time of measurement effects

    (cf. Palmore, 1978). Therefore, we report results for the combined data from both cohorts.

    Because of data protection directives, the first questionnaire could not be sent directly to

    the graduates. Instead, we had to display the questionnaires in administration offices. All

    graduates had to visit these offices in order to collect some official certificates. Together with

    these certificates they received the first questionnaire. The graduates were also asked to

    provide their address, because the study would be continued some time later. From the

    4,200 questionnaires displayed 1,930 (46%) were sent back to the researchers.

    Time 1. Participants were 825 women and 1,105 men (mean age 27 years). They were

    predominantly German; about 5% came from other European countries. Ninety-four percent

    of the respondents provided their address (N = 1,819). Among other variables, we assessed

    gender, GPA, study major, occupational self-efficacy, mastery goals, and prestige goals at

    time 1. Participants who provided their address did not differ from participants who declined

    to provide their address with regard to these variables.

    Time 2. 102 of the 1,819 participants who had provided their address in the first ques-

    tionnaire had moved to an unknown address at time 2. Of the remaining 1,717 persons,

    1,398 participants (589 women and 809 men; mean age 28.5 years) responded to the sec-

    ond questionnaire (response rate 81.4%). A drop-out analysis revealed that there were no

    differences (gender, age, study major, GPA, time 1 self-regulation variables) between par-

    ticipants who answered the second questionnaire and those who did not. At time 2, we

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 10 -

    measured mastery goals, prestige goals, objective career success, and subjective career

    success among other variables.

    Time 3. Of the 1,663 participants who could be contacted three years after graduation

    (54 individuals had moved to an unknown address), 1,330 (561 women, 769 men; mean age

    30 years) responded to the third questionnaire (response rate 80%). There were again no

    differences (same variables tested as at time 2) between participants who answered the third

    questionnaire and those who did not. Among other variables, we measured occupational

    self-efficacy, objective career success, and subjective career success at time 3.

    As can be seen from this description, we collected data on all self-regulation variables at

    time 1, measured goals again at time 2, and assessed occupational self-efficacy again at

    time 3. This partial asymmetry in measurement is due to the fact that the research which we

    took our data from was not exclusively designed for testing the present hypotheses, and that

    these questionnaires measured a large number of additional variables.

    The following analyses were performed with the 1,219 participants (516 women, 703

    men) who completed all three questionnaires. They had graduated in law (29 women, 37

    men), medicine (88 women, 113 men), arts and humanities (98 women, 37 men), natural

    sciences (49 women, 101 men), economics (80 women, 130 men), engineering (18 women,

    205 men), and teaching (154 women, 80 men).

    3.3 Measures

    3.3.1 Occupational self-efficacy

    The scale we used to measure occupational self-efficacy (Abele, Stief & Andrä, 2000)

    consists of 6 items (sample items “I am confident that I could deal efficiently with the chal-

    lenges of my work if I only want to“; “I doubt that I really have the skills necessary for my

    work”, reversely coded). Participants respond on 5-point scales (1 = not at all to 5 = very

    much). The occupational self-efficacy scale is one-dimensional and shows good internal

    consistency (Abele et al., 2000). For the present sample the internal consistency was Cron-

    bach’s α = .78 (both at time 1 and time 3).

    3.3.2 Goals

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 11 -

    The scales we used to measure mastery goals and materialistic, prestige-related goals

    consist of 4 items each (mastery goals: “I want to work on difficult and challenging tasks”; “I

    want to improve my competencies”; “I want to contribute to innovations”; “I want to continu-

    ously broaden my mind”; materialistic, prestige-related goals: “I want to make a lot of

    money”; “I want to gain high occupational reputation”; “I want to have good career opportuni-

    ties”; “I want to gain high social prestige”). The item formulations were adapted from a Ger-

    man version of the work values inventory (Super, 1970; German version: Seifert & Berg-

    mann, 1983). Participants respond on 5-point scales (1 = not important to 5 = very impor-

    tant). In a pretest with 147 students (90 women, 57 men; mean age 23 years) the two factors

    clearly emerged (factor I: materialistic, prestige oriented goals, 32% explained item variance,

    Cronbach’s α = .77; Factor II: mastery goals, 25% explained item variance, Cronbach’s α =

    .63). In the present sample this two-factorial structure was replicated (factor I, materialistic,

    prestige-oriented goals, 30% explained item variance, Cronbach’s α = .71; Factor II: mastery

    goals, 20% explained item variance, Cronbach’s α = .60). The alpha coefficient for mastery

    goals is rather low. However, the items measuring mastery goals were designed to tap dif-

    ferent facets of the construct. Internal consistency might not be an optimal estimate of reli-

    ability for such heterogeneous subscales (test-retest correlation see Table 1).

    3.3.3 Objective career success

    Objective career success was measured by an index we developed for the current study

    to be applicable to all participants and to every time of measurement. The index was

    weighted to balance pay (weighted by two thirds: maximally 11 points) and responsibility

    status (weighted by one third: maximally 5.5 points). It was composed of the following crite-

    ria: (a) monthly pay before taxes (in thirteen steps from “no income”, coded as 0; “less than

    €500.-”, coded as 0.5 [1 € about 1.30 $]; “less than €1,000”, coded as 1; “less than €2,000.-”,

    coded as 2; and then in equal steps to “less than €10,000”.-, coded as 10; and “more than

    €10,000”; coded as 11) 2, (b) permission to delegate work (0 = no, .92 = yes), (c) project re-

    sponsibility (0 = no, 1.83 = yes), and (d) leadership position (0 = no, 2.75 = yes). Hence, the

    index could vary between 0 (low success) and 16.5 (high success). We assigned zero points

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 12 -

    to participants who had been without employment for at least three months before answering

    the questionnaire.

    We tested the factor structure of our objective career measure by means of confirmatory

    factor analyses for categorical variables using the WLSMV estimator (using Mplus; Muthén &

    Muthén, 2004). The results supported a one-factor structure (t2: χ2 < .01, p = 1.00, WRMR

    =.001, CLI = 1.00, TLI = 1.00; t3: χ2 = 8.00, p < .01, WRMR = .94, CLI = 1.00; TLI = .99).

    3.3.4 Subjective career success

    We operationalized other-referent subjective career success as a comparison with for-

    mer fellow students (“Compared with your former fellow students, how successful do you

    think your career development has been so far?”), because pretests had shown that at our

    participants’ present career stage former fellow students were highly significant comparison

    targets. Participants based their responses on a 5-point rating scale (1 = less successful to 5

    = more successful). Whereas one-item measures are usually suboptimal, in the present case

    the single item captures the essence of other-referent career success that we wanted to as-

    sess (cf., Heslin, 2003).

    Participants who were currently not employed (and had not been for at least 3 months)

    (continuing education, unemployment, parental leave etc.) did not answer this question (time

    2: 69 participants; time 3: 114 participants). The analysis of subjective career success is

    therefore based on 1,060 participants who had been employed both at time 2 and time 3.

    3.3.5 Grade point average

    We standardized individual GPA’s to the respective year’s and respective major’s av-

    erage GPA. A value of “0” means average, a positive value means above, and a negative

    value means below average GPA.

    4. Results

    4.1 Preliminary Analyses

    4.1.1 Grade point average

    Our participants’ mean GPA was close to the overall average, M = .04 (SD = .51). The

    correlations between GPA and the self-regulation variables were low, but due to the large

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 13 -

    sample size, two of them were significant (occupational self-efficacy r = .08, p < .01; mastery

    goals r = .07, p < .05; prestige goals r = .04, ns). Despite these low correlations we tested

    whether the findings we report later regarding influences of self-regulation on career success

    would change if GPA was included. This was not the case. 3

    4.1.2 Study major.

    Our participants had different study majors, and the gender by study major distribution

    was not even. It was therefore necessary to test main effects, and especially interactions of

    study majors with gender and with the self-regulation variables, with respect to the depend-

    ent measures.

    To test for possible interactions of study major with the self-regulation variables in pre-

    dicting career success, and to test for possible interactions of study major with career suc-

    cess in predicting the subsequent self-regulation variables changes we computed regression

    analyses in which we regressed the four success measures (objective career success time 2,

    time 3; subjective career success time 2, time 3) on study major (dummy-coded), the self-

    regulation variables, and the interactions of the self-regulation variables with study major,

    and we computed regression analyses in which we regressed the three self-regulation vari-

    able changes (time 2 goals, time 3 occupational self-efficacy) on study major (dummy

    coded), career success (both objective and subjective) and the interactions of study major

    with career success. Of the resulting 108 interactions 12 were marginally significant and they

    always explained less than 1% of variance. We therefore conclude that these interactions are

    negligible.

    To test for possible interaction effects of study major and gender on career success we

    computed analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with study major and gender as factors and the

    four success measures as dependent variables. Study major had a highly significant influ-

    ence on objective career success at time 2, F (6, 1205) = 81.66, p < .001, η2 = .29, and on

    objective career success at time 3, F (6, 1205) = 41.24, p < .001, η2 = .17, but it did not inter-

    act with gender, both Fs < 1.64, ns. Study major had an influence on time 2 subjective career

    success, F (6, 1046) = 4.17, p < .001, η2 = .023, but not on time 3 subjective career success,

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 14 -

    F < 1, and there were no interactions with gender, both F < 1. Graduates with majors in eco-

    nomics and engineering had higher scores than the other graduates both in time 2 and time

    3 objective career success and in time 2 subjective career success.

    Regarding influences of study major and gender on the self-regulation variables, occu-

    pational self-efficacy slightly differed between study majors, F (6, 1205) = 3.03, p < .01, η2 =

    .015, with lower occupational self-efficacy of teachers (M = 3.60) than engineers and partici-

    pants with majors in arts and humanities (both M = 3.90), with the other subjects falling in-

    between (economics M = 3.77; medicine M = 3.75; law M = 3.69; natural sciences M = 3.66).

    Mastery goals differed between study majors, F (6, 1205) = 8.59, p < .001, η2 = .041, with

    lower values of law graduates (M = 3.47) and teachers (M = 3.56) than graduates from medi-

    cine (M = 3.77), economics (M = 3.78), and arts and humanities (M = 3.80), and with highest

    values of natural sciences graduates (M = 3.89) and engineers (M = 3.90). Prestige goals

    also differed between study majors, F (6, 1205) = 12.69, p < .001, η2 = .059 (graduates from

    law: M = 3.42; economics: M = 3.24; medicine: M = 3.04; engineers: M = 3.03; arts and hu-

    manities: M = 2.91; natural sciences: M = 2.82; teachers: M = 2.76). There were no gender

    by study major interactions (both goals: F < 1; occupational self-efficacy: F(6, 1205) = 1.55,

    ns).

    4.2 Descriptive Findings

    Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations of the measures are presented in Table 1. At

    time 1 there were small, but positive correlations between occupational self-efficacy and

    mastery goals (r = .28), occupational self-efficacy and prestige goals (r = .19), and between

    mastery goals and prestige goals (r = .21). The auto-correlations of occupational self-efficacy

    at time 1 and time 3 (r = .51), mastery goals at time 1 and time 2 (r = .57), and prestige goals

    at time 1 and time 2 (r = .70) were high, but different enough from 1 to indicate some malle-

    ability of the constructs. All time 1 self-regulation variables were significantly related to objec-

    tive career success and subjective career success at time 2. All self-regulation variables at

    time 1, time 2, and time 3 were significantly related to objective career success and subjec-

    tive career success at time 3. The autocorrelation of objective career success at time 2 and

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 15 -

    objective career success at time 3 was high (r = .60). The correlation between time 2 and

    time 3 subjective career success was of medium size (r = .38). The correlations between

    objective career success and subjective career success were significant, but not very high

    (maximum r = .31, minimum r = .13). Participants endorsed mastery goals more (time 1 M =

    3.75; time 2 M = 4.06) than prestige goals (time 1 M = 3.02; time 2 M = 3.12), both t (1218) >

    29.92, p < .001. Except from time 1 prestige goals all correlations of our self-regulation vari-

    ables with gender were significant, but of small size.

    4.3 Hypotheses Testing

    We tested our hypotheses by means of structural equation modeling (SEM) using Mplus

    (Muthén & Muthén, 2004). Structural equation modeling has several advantages. The meas-

    urement model of the predictors can be included; measurement errors can be taken into ac-

    count; the specific postulated paths can be tested; and besides providing the path coeffi-

    cients, a series of overall fit statistics can be reported that show how well the empirical data

    fits the theoretical model (Kline, 2005). We tested all models using Maximum Likelihood es-

    timation with robust standard errors (MLR). All chi square difference tests were adjusted us-

    ing a procedure adequate for the Satorra-Bentler chi square test statistic (Satorra, 2000). We

    first modeled true change scores (TCM; Steyer, Eid & Schwenkmezger, 1997) for the re-

    peated measures of mastery goals, prestige goals, and occupational self-efficacy. Then, we

    tested the models on objective career success and on subjective career success, and we

    also tested the models including gender.

    4.3.1

    Modeling of true change scores. Hypotheses (7) and (8) were concerned with changes

    in the predictors. We decided not to compute simple difference scores because these suffer

    from impoverished reliability. We rather modelled the change scores. Latent growth model-

    ling is one possibility (LGM; Duncan, Duncan & Strycker, 2006; Singer & Willet, 2003). How-

    ever, we decided not to use this approach because our design was comprised of only two

    points of measurement for changes, and LGM is better suited when there are three points of

    measurement (Singer & Willet, 2003). 4 Instead, we applied true change modelling (Steyer et

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 16 -

    al., 1997). In TCM the observed indicators of the latent variable at the second time of meas-

    urement are regressed on the latent variable at the first time of measurement. These re-

    gressed indicators are treated as measures of the latent variable at time 2. The change vari-

    able computed by this procedure is assumed to be free of measurement errors and it is al-

    lowed to correlate and to be regressed like any other latent variable. This change variable

    does not reflect the individual shape of a growth process, but rather reflects true interindi-

    vidual differences in intraindividual change.

    Following recommendations in the structural equation modeling literature (Dwyer, 1983;

    Roberts, 1997) our latent variables were measured by three indicators each (the 6 items

    measuring occupational self-efficacy both at time 1 and time 3 were converted into 3 parcels;

    for the 4-item measures of mastery goals and prestige goals we used two items and one

    parcel computed of the remaining two items). Measurement errors were allowed to correlate

    over time for the same indicators of the same latent variable (Kline, 2005; Steyer et al.,

    1997). 5

    Figure 2 shows the resulting measurement model including the true change variables

    (correlations between the latent constructs are not displayed). It has good fit statistics (χ²/df =

    2.10, CFI = .98, TLI = .97, RMSEA = .030), despite a significant chi-square, χ²(1219) =

    220.35, p < .001, which is due to the large sample size. All factor loadings are highly signifi-

    cant. 6

    4.3.2 Prediction of objective career success

    Hypotheses (1), (2), and (3) had postulated influences of the self-regulation variables on

    objective career success and Hypothesis (7) had assumed a reciprocal influence of objective

    career success on the self-regulation variables. Our empirical data refers to three times of

    measurement with time 1 measuring the self-regulation variables (occupational self-efficacy,

    mastery goals, prestige goals), time 2 measuring objective career success and subjective

    career success as well as again both goals, and time 3 measuring objective career success,

    subjective career success and again occupational self-efficacy. We could test Hypotheses

    (1), (2), and (3) both with respect to time 2 objective career success and time 3 objective

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 17 -

    career success. And we could test Hypothesis (7) with respect to influences of time 2 objec-

    tive career success on both goals and time 3 objective career success influences on occupa-

    tional self-efficacy. 7

    The model we tested was built of 6 latent variables (occupational self-efficacy, mastery

    goals, prestige goals; and their true change scores) and two observed variables (objective

    career success at time 2, objective career success at time 3). We treated objective career

    success as an observed variable because objective career success was built from factual

    information that is more or less free of measurement error (Boudreau, Boswell & Judge,

    2001; Podsakoff & Organ, 1986); and because we had already weighted the components of

    objective career success (see above) and therefore a simple measurement model was not

    appropriate. The model allowed correlations between occupational self-efficacy, mastery

    goals, and prestige goals at time 1, a correlation of the true change scores in mastery goals

    and prestige goals, paths from every predictor at time 1 to time 2 and time 3 objective career

    success, paths from true change scores in both goals at time 2 to objective career success at

    time 3, paths from time 2 objective career success to the goals’ true change scores, and

    paths from time 2 objective career success and time 3 objective career success to the true

    change score of occupational self-efficacy. We also included a path from time 2 objective

    career success to time 3 objective career success, which means that this autoregressive

    model predicts change in time 3 objective career success. We also included a path from oc-

    cupational self-efficacy at time 1 to the true change scores of mastery goals and prestige

    goals, and we tested the paths from the true change scores in mastery goals and prestige

    goals at time 2 to the true change score of occupational self-efficacy at time 3. There were

    21 paths and correlations in this structural model. 14 were significant and a maximum of 28

    paths and correlations could have been postulated. 89 parameters had to be estimated in the

    total model. Due to our large sample size we were well in line with the recommendation of 10

    to 15 persons per estimated parameter (Kline, 2005).

    Figure 3 displays the resulting structural equation model. For the sake of clarity we ex-

    cluded time 1 correlations and we also excluded non-significant paths. The model had good

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 18 -

    fit statistics (χ²/df = 2.51, CFI = .97, TLI = .95, RMSEA = .037), despite a significant chi-

    square, χ²(1219) = 334.28, p < .001. We computed Hoelter’s Critical N (Hoelter, 1983). Given

    that its value is smaller than our sample size, a significant chi square does not matter. In our

    case Hoelter’s Critical N was 300 and, hence, was much smaller than our sample size. The

    R² values show that 7% of variance in objective career success at time 2, and 10% of vari-

    ance in change of objective career success at time 3 was explained by this model.

    Supporting Hypothesis (1) occupational self-efficacy at time 1 influenced objective ca-

    reer success at time 2 (β = .09, p < .05) and objective career success change at time 3 (β =

    .11, p < .01). Supporting Hypothesis (2) mastery goals at time 1 influenced objective career

    success at time 2 (β = .13, p < .01). However, there was no influence of mastery goals at

    time 1 on changes in objective career success at time 3 (β = .00, ns), and also no influence

    of changes in mastery goals at time 2 to changes in objective career success at time 3 (β =

    .10, ns). Supporting Hypothesis (3) prestige goals at time 1 influenced objective career suc-

    cess at time 2 (β = .12, p < .001) and changes in objective career success at time 3 (β = .11,

    p < .001). In accordance with Hypothesis (3) changes in prestige goals at time 2 influenced

    changes in objective career success at time 3 (β = .15, p < .05). Objective career success at

    time 2 influenced objective career success at time 3 (β = .55, p

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 19 -

    are effects built from at least two paths (for instance, prestige goals via time 2 objective ca-

    reer success to the true change score of prestige goals), and they are computed by multiply-

    ing the β-values of the single paths. The coefficient is smaller the more paths are involved.

    Hence, the size of specific indirect paths can only be compared if the number of paths is the

    same. It is possible that certain specific indirect effects are significant whereas the overall

    indirect effect is not.

    Prestige goals had the comparatively strongest total (.18) and direct effect (.11) on time

    3 changes in objective career success. Two specific indirect effects involving prestige goals

    were also significant. One was the path from prestige goals at time 1 via objective career

    success at time 2, the other one was the path from time 1 prestige goals via time 2 objective

    career success and via change in prestige goals. Occupational self-efficacy had a total effect

    of .14 (direct effect .13) with one significant specific indirect effect of occupational self-

    efficacy via time 2 objective career success. Mastery goals had no direct effect on change in

    time 3 objective career success, but one indirect effect via time 2 objective career success.

    4.3.3 Prediction of subjective career success

    Hypotheses (4), (5) and (6) had stated that the self-regulation variables influence sub-

    jective career success, and Hypothesis (8) had assumed that subjective career success has

    a reciprocal influence on these self-regulation variables. We tested these hypotheses with

    the structural model as described above, and replaced time 2 and time 3 objective success

    by time 2 and time 3 subjective career success. Due to the path from time 2 subjective ca-

    reer success to time 3 subjective career success it was again an autoregressive change

    model for time 3 subjective career success.

    The model depicted in Figure 4 had good fit indices (χ²/df = 1.95, CFI = .98, TLI = .97,

    RMSEA = .030) despite a significant chi square, χ²(1060) = 259.60, p < .001. Hoelter’s Criti-

    cal N was 383 in this case, again much lower than our sample size. This means that the

    model can be interpreted despite the significant chi square. The explained variance was 10%

    for subjective career success at time 2 and 11% for the change in subjective career success

    at time 3. In accord with Hypothesis (4) occupational self-efficacy influenced subjective ca-

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 20 -

    reer success at time 2 (β = .23, p < .001) and subjective career success change at time 3 (β

    = .21, p < .001). Supporting Hypothesis (6) the path from prestige goals to subjective career

    success at time 2 was also significant (β = .12, p < .05). In contrast to Hypothesis (5) there

    were no path from mastery goals to time 2 subjective career success (β = .07, ns). And there

    were also no further significant paths from goals to subjective career success (mastery goals

    to time 3 subjective career success change, β = .02, ns; true change score mastery goals to

    time 3 subjective career success change, β = -.05, ns; prestige goals to time 3 subjective

    career success change, β = .04, ns; true change score prestige goals to time 3 subjective

    career success change, β = .09, ns). Finally, subjective career success at time 2 influenced

    subjective career success at time 3 (β = .30, p < .001).

    We again computed total, direct, and indirect effects of our variables on time 3 subjec-

    tive career success change. Table 3 (upper panel) shows that occupational self-efficacy had

    the largest total (.28), direct (.21), and indirect (.07) effect. One specific indirect effect from

    occupational self-efficacy via subjective career success at time 2 was significant. Further-

    more, there was one specific indirect effect of prestige goals via subjective career success at

    time 2.

    4.3.4 Reciprocal influences of objective and subjective career success on the self-regulation

    variables.

    In order to test Hypotheses (7) and (8) on reciprocal influences of objective and sub-

    jective career success on changes in the self-regulation variables we tested a model in which

    both objective career success and subjective career success were included. We tested paths

    from time 2 objective career success and subjective career success on the changes in goals

    and occupational self-efficacy, paths from time 3 changes in objective career success and

    subjective career success on changes in occupational self-efficacy, and paths from changes

    in time 2 goals on changes in time 3 occupational self-efficacy. The analysis revealed that

    changes in mastery goals and prestige goals were influenced by both objective and subjec-

    tive career success (mastery goals: time 2 objective career success β = .18, p < .01, time 2

    subjective career success β = .21, p < .001, R² = .13; prestige goals: time 2 objective career

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 21 -

    success β = .18, p < .001, time 2 subjective career success β = .18, p < .01, R² = .09). The

    change in occupational self-efficacy, however, was only influenced by subjective career suc-

    cess change at time 3 and change in mastery goals (β = .21, β = .22, p < .01, R² = .10). Time

    2 objective career success (β = -.06, ns), time 2 subjective career success (β = .04, ns), and

    time 3 objective career success change (β = .05, ns) had no influence.

    4.4 Model Testing Including Gender

    In order to test Hypotheses (9) and (10) we included gender as an observed variable in

    the above models on objective career success and subjective career success. We added

    paths from gender to time 1 self-regulation variables, to time 2 and time 3 objective career

    success or subjective career success, and to the true change scores.

    4.4.1 Objective career success

    The structural equation model showed a good model fit (χ²/df = 2.83, CFI = .96, TLI =

    .94, RMSEA = .039) despite a significant chi-square, χ²(1219) = 410.90, p < .001. 8 In accord

    with Hypothesis (9) gender influenced time 2 objective career success (β = .20, p < .001) and

    time 3 objective career success change (β = .15, p < .001) with lower success of women than

    of men. Gender explained an additional 4% of variance in time 2 objective career success

    (R² total = .11) and an additional 5% of variance in time 3 objective career success change

    (R² total = .15).

    Two paths from gender to time 1 self-regulation variables were significant, the path to

    occupational self-efficacy (β = .08, p < .05; women had lower scores) and the path to mas-

    tery goals (β = .13, p < .001; women had lower scores). Men showed more changes in pres-

    tige goals (β = .10, p < .05) and in occupational self-efficacy (β = .13, p < .01) than women.

    Gender had a highly significant total effect (.28) on time 3 changes in objective career suc-

    cess (direct effect .15; indirect effect .13, see Table 2, lower panel). The specific indirect ef-

    fects included time 1 occupational self-efficacy, time 2 objective career success, and a path

    from gender via time 2 objective career success and via changes in prestige goals. Support-

    ing Hypothesis (10) these findings suggest that the effects of gender on changes in time 3

    objective career success were both direct and mediated. To further test this mediation we

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 22 -

    compared the complete model (direct and mediated effects) with two models in which we

    dropped one significant path at a time. Dropping the path from gender to occupational self-

    efficacy resulted in a significant chi square difference, ∆ χ²(1) = 6.12, p < .02; and dropping

    the path from changes in prestige goals to time 3 changes in objective career success also

    resulted in a significant chi square difference, ∆ χ²(1) = 9.89, p < .005, i.e. the model includ-

    ing both direct and mediated gender effects was better than the models in which mediated

    effects were not allowed. 9

    4.4.2 Subjective career success

    The model had good fit indices (χ²/df = 2.31, CFI = .97, TLI = .95, RMSEA = .033) de-

    spite a significant chi-square, χ²(1060) = 335.19, p < .001. Supporting a shifting standards

    interpretation gender neither predicted subjective career success at time 2 (β = .05, ns) nor

    subjective career success change at time 3 (β = -.01, ns). There were only two indirect ef-

    fects of gender (see Table 3, lower panel), one involving the effect of gender via time 1 oc-

    cupational self-efficacy, and the second one involving the effect of gender via time 1 occupa-

    tional self-efficacy and via time 2 subjective career success. One additional percent of vari-

    ance in both time 2 subjective career success and time 3 subjective career success change

    was explained by gender, but the paths contributing to this increase were not significant.

    4.5 The Longitudinal Relationship of Objective and Subjective Career Success

    Finally, we analyzed the direction of the relationship between objective and subjective

    career success. If they are merely correlated then the longitudinal influence of objective ca-

    reer success at time 2 on subjective career success at time 3 should be completely mediated

    by subjective career success at time 2 and the longitudinal influence of subjective career

    success at time 2 on objective career success at time 3 should be completely mediated by

    objective career success at time 2. However, if the relationship is directional, then there

    should be no mediation. We conducted analyses of mediation (Baron & Kenny, 1986). The

    prerequisite for such an analysis are significant correlations between all considered variables

    (see Table 1). Then, one tests whether there is a drop in prediction of the criterion by the

    respective predictor, when the possible mediator is controlled for. Mediation is given, if this

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 23 -

    drop is significant. We found that the effect of time 2 objective career success on time 3 sub-

    jective career success was completely mediated by time 2 subjective career success, indi-

    cated by a significant drop in prediction (from β = .13, p < .001; to β = .01, ns), z = 7.99, p <

    .001. The same result emerged for the prediction of time 3 objective career success via time

    2 subjective career success: It was completely mediated by time 2 objective career success

    as indicated by a significant drop in prediction (from β = .20, p < .001; to β = .03, ns), z =

    9.82, p < .001. Conversely, the prediction of time 3 subjective career success via time 2 sub-

    jective career success was not mediated by time 2 objective career success (from β = .38, p

    < .001; to β = .37, p < .001), z < 1, and the prediction of time 3 objective career success by

    time 2 objective career success was also not mediated by time 2 subjective career success

    (from β = .57, p < .001; to β = .56, p < .001), z < 1. These findings suggest that subjective

    career success and objective career success are correlated but—at least at this stage of

    career development—one does not predict the other.

    5. Discussion

    The present research builds on the idea that in time of changing labor markets and of

    changing career patterns there is a strong need for individuals to actively self-regulate their

    careers. We wanted to test whether self-regulation, which largely happens in our heads, has

    an influence on career success, and whether people have the power to actively control their

    careers by purposeful thought. We considered two important components of self-regulatory

    thought, namely self-efficacy beliefs and career-related goals. The latter were divided into

    more “intrinsic” mastery-, and intellectual stimulation related goals versus more “extrinsic”,

    materialistic and prestige related goals. We put forward the general assumption that indi-

    viduals with high occupational self-efficacy beliefs and with both high mastery goals and high

    prestige goals would be more successful than individuals with lower values in these self-

    regulatory variables. We further asked whether such influences are restricted to subjective

    measures of career success, such as satisfaction or comparative evaluation, or whether they

    also have an influence on objective career success, such as pay or responsibility status.

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 24 -

    Considering the cyclical nature of self-regulation we finally asked whether success—

    subjective and/or objective—leads to changes in self-regulation variables.

    5.1 Self-Regulation Influences Career Success

    The data supported our general assumption: Individuals’ self-regulatory thoughts have

    an influence on their career progression in a time span as long as three years. Both career-

    related goals and occupational self-efficacy add to the prediction of subjective success (10%

    explained variance at time 2, 11% explained variance of change in subjective success at

    time 3) and also to the prediction of objective success (7% explained variance at time 2, and

    10% explained variance of change in objective career success at time 3). We controlled for

    our participants’ performance level by considering their GPA. Therefore, the present data

    show that self-regulatory thoughts influence career progression above and beyond cognitive

    ability as assessed by GPA.

    Regarding objective success, materialistic, prestige-related goals had the highest influ-

    ence. Supporting Hypothesis (3), people who just after graduation were oriented at climbing

    up the career ladder, at having high responsibility and recognition, and at earning a lot of

    money did, in fact, have more responsibility and recognition and did earn more money three

    years later than people who pursued this goal to a lower degree. Moreover, we could show

    that not only the initial level of prestige goals influenced objective career success, but that an

    enhancement of prestige goals at time 2 had an influence on subsequent objective career

    success as well. These findings support the theoretical notion of a feed-forward mechanism

    in which goals lead to activities for attaining them and goal-attainment leads to adapted goal-

    setting which instigates further efforts to attain these adapted goals (Bandura, 1986, 2001;

    Bandura & Locke, 2003).

    Supporting Hypothesis (1), occupational self-efficacy also had a significant influence on

    objective career success. People who believed in their skills and motivation to perform the

    occupational tasks they are confronted with were more successful than people with lower

    self-efficacy beliefs. To our knowledge this is the first time that such a long-time influence of

    self-efficacy on objective parameters of success has been demonstrated. The data also vali-

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 25 -

    date cross-sectional findings on the relationship between self-efficacy, pay, and hierarchical

    position (Day & Allen, 2004; Lubbers, et al., 2005). Due to the present design, in which the

    second assessment of occupational self-efficacy took place at time 3, we could not test a

    feed-forward influence of changes in occupational self-efficacy on later career success.

    Therefore, we also could not test whether there is a negative influence of self-efficacy on

    success if measured on an intra-individual level (see Vancouver & Kendall, 2006; Yeo &

    Neal, 2006). 10

    There was also an influence of mastery goals on objective career success. Supporting

    Hypothesis (2), people who wanted to deal with challenging tasks and who wanted to im-

    prove their competencies and skills were objectively more successful at time 2. Mediated via

    time 2 objective career success, time 1 mastery goals also had an influence on time 3 objec-

    tive career success. However, there was no feed-forward mechanism of changes in mastery

    goals on changes in time 3 objective career success.

    The finding that both mastery goals and prestige-related goals are important determi-

    nants of objective career success—at least in the early career phase— fits with the data of

    Frieze et al. (2006) and it fits with meta-analytical results by Payne et al. (2007; see also

    Harachiewicz, et al., 1997; Barron & Harakiewicz, 2001), who reported that job performance

    is positively related to both a learning goal orientation, which is more “intrinsic”, and a prove

    goal orientation, which is more “extrinsic”.

    Regarding subjective success, occupational self-efficacy was most important. People

    with high self-efficacy beliefs felt more successful compared to their former fellow students

    than people with lower occupational self-efficacy, thus supporting Hypothesis (4). This result

    is in accord with meta-analytical findings by Dette et al. (2004) and Ng et al. (2005), who

    showed that individual difference variables are very important for subjective career success.

    It also validates cross-sectional data on self-efficacy and subjective career success (Judge &

    Bono, 2001; see also Saks, 1995). Prestige goals had an influence as well (supporting Hy-

    pothesis 6), but there was no feed-forward mechanism from changes in prestige goals to

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 26 -

    changes in subjective career success. Contrary to Hypothesis (5), mastery goals had no in-

    fluence on subjective career success.

    All in all, the influence of mastery goals on career success was smaller than the influ-

    ence of prestige goals and of occupational self-efficacy. This finding might be astonishing

    given the large body of research showing that “intrinsic” goals are of utmost importance for

    performance and optimal motivation (e.g., Dweck, 1986; Locke & Latham, 2002; Vande-

    Walle, 1997; VandeWalle et al., 1999). However, one way to interpret this stronger influence

    of prestige goals than of mastery goals on objective career success could be the closer con-

    nection between the present operationalizations of prestige goals (among others: “I want to

    make a lot of money”) and objective career success (among others: pay). Furthermore, the

    finding that mastery goals are more important in the early phase of a career (influence on

    time 2 objective career success) than in a later one (no influence on time 3 objective career

    success) makes sense because the respective goals should be most important in times of

    change, i.e., at the beginning of an employment or in case of change. We can only speculate

    why there was no influence of mastery goals on subjective success. One possibility is that

    the high mean and low variance in these goals (see Table 1) obscured a correlation. It is also

    conceivable that people with high mastery goals are more reluctant to evaluate their career

    as comparatively successful because their standards are very high.

    5.2 Career Success Influences Self-Regulation

    We empirically demonstrated the cyclical nature of self-regulation as postulated in self-

    regulation theories (Bandura, 1986, 2001; Vancouver, 2000; Vancouver & Day, 2005; Zim-

    merman, 2000). More successful individuals increased their mastery and prestige-related

    goals more, and became more self-efficient than less successful people. Furthermore, we

    could demonstrate that both objective and subjective success were important for these

    changes. This means that people have to be objectively successful and have to interpret

    their outcomes as success in order to initiate feed-forward processes of changes in goals.

    Supporting Hypotheses (7) and (8) participants who were successful in their careers en-

    hanced both their mastery goals and their prestige goals. Changes in self-efficacy, however,

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 27 -

    were only induced by subjective success. Whereas experimental studies have demonstrated

    the malleability of self-efficacy upon success or failure (Cervone et al., 2004; Gernigon &

    Delloye, 2003; Smith et al., 2006) the present research found no impact of objective career

    success on changes in occupational self-efficacy. Interestingly, changes in occupational self-

    efficacy were also influenced by changes in mastery goals, i.e., when people raised their

    mastery goals they also raised their occupational self-efficacy. Whereas there are already

    findings showing that self-efficacy and mastery goals are correlated (see Payne et al., 2007;

    Utman, 1997; Seijts, et al., 2004; Wood, Mento & Locke, 1987) the present results suggest

    that there is a reciprocal relationship between these constructs. Increases in one of them

    lead to increases in the other one as well.

    5.3 Gender

    In accord with previous research (Abele, 2003; Greene & DeBacker, 2004; Kirchmeyer,

    1998; Ng et al., 2005) and supporting Hypothesis (9), women’s objective success was lower

    than men’s. Including gender into the model of objective career success increased the ex-

    plained variance by 4% in case of time 2 objective career success, and 5% in case of time 3

    changes in objective career success. A detailed analysis of the reasons for this difference is

    beyond the scope of the present article. However, supporting Hypothesis (10), the lower time

    3 objective career success of women was in part mediated by their somewhat lower occupa-

    tional self-efficacy and their smaller increase in prestige-related goals, which, in turn, was

    influenced by women’s lower time 2 objective career success. We also found that women

    showed less increase in occupational self-efficacy than men. As a possible conclusion from

    these findings one can speculate that women’s lower objective career success will evoke

    lower adaptations of career-related goals and perhaps also lower adaptations of occupational

    self-efficacy, which will then lead to lower further career-success, and so on. This means

    that–other things being equal–the gap in objective success between men and women should

    increase over time. Subjective success did not differ between men and women. This finding

    fits with the predictions of the shifting standards model (Biernat & Billings, 2001), according

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 28 -

    to which the use of subjective scales leads to reduced differences between groups due to the

    application of different anchors.

    5.4 Career Success

    Analyzing career success requires a clear conceptual understanding of this complex

    construct. We differentiated between objective success and other-referent subjective suc-

    cess. The moderate correlations between objective career success and subjective career

    success that we found in our research are comparable to meta-analytical findings (Dette et

    al., 2004; Ng et al., 2005). They suggest that both constructs tap into different domains. The

    mediation analysis also showed that subjective career success cannot be influenced by ob-

    jective career success and vice versa. Hence, it is a correlation and not a directed relation-

    ship. Furthermore, the partly different results regarding the determinants of objective versus

    subjective career success clearly indicate that it is important to distinguish the two.

    5.5 Research Implications

    The present results are important extensions of prior work. Regarding self-efficacy they

    show that findings on the positive effect of self-efficacy on task performance and job per-

    formance (Judge & Bono, 2001; Judge, et al., 2007; Judge et al., 1999; Lubbers et al., 2005;

    Riggs et al., 1994; Stajkovic & Luthans, 1998) can be extended to a complex construct like

    career success. Regarding goals, our findings suggest that the content of goals matters and

    that the distinction between more “intrinsic” mastery-related goals and more “extrinsic” pres-

    tige-related goals is promising. It is a core distinction inherent in many conceptualizations of

    goals and motivation (cf. Austin & Vancouver, 1996; Dweck, 1986; Furnham, et al., 2005;

    Hazer & Alvares, 1981; Locke & Latham, 2002; Nicholls, 1984; Super, 1957; VandeWalle,

    1997; VandeWalle, et al., 1999, Zytowsky, 1994). Regarding objective and subjective career

    success in a long-time perspective, our findings clearly suggest that both goals are important

    and that mastery goals alone will not be sufficient (see also Harackiewisz, et al., 1997; Bar-

    ron & Harackiewisz, 2001).

    Further research will have to address how the influence of prestige goals on objective

    career success is mediated. Previous research has shown that the effect of learning goals on

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 29 -

    job performance is mediated via effort and planning (VandeWalle et al., 1999). In a similar

    vein, individuals with high prestige goals possibly spend more time for their work, engage in

    more career-planning, develop more specific competencies, and are more committed to their

    careers. It is also conceivable that people with high prestige goals interpret their job experi-

    ences in a more optimistic way as is suggested by the influence of prestige goals on subjec-

    tive career success. More optimistic interpretations, in turn, could also lead to higher effort

    and a stronger commitment.

    The present research also has limitations which open perspectives for further investiga-

    tion. First, future research should consider more individual difference variables and more

    process variables possibly mediating the influences of self-efficacy and goals. As a recent

    meta-analysis demonstrated self-efficacy effects might decrease if more individual difference

    variables are taken into account (e.g., Judge, et al., 2007). Second, all our participants held a

    university degree. Future research should test whether the present findings can be general-

    ized for people with lower “human capital” (Ng. et al., 2005) in terms of education. Third, the

    present research was only concerned with the first three years of participants’ careers and

    should be expanded to later phases of occupational career development. Fourth, our opera-

    tionalization of subjective career success was an other-referent comparative judgment. This

    is advantageous, since such an operationalization is only rarely applied (Heslin, 2003). How-

    ever, further research should also include self-referent subjective career success, for in-

    stance career satisfaction, in order to capture the full meaning of subjective success (see

    Dette et al., 2004; Heslin, 2003).

    5.6 Applied Implications

    Knowing more about malleable individual differences and of “purposeful thought” (Ban-

    dura, 1997) that influence career success is of utmost importance for career planning and

    career counseling. The present findings show that occupational self-efficacy, mastery goals,

    and prestige goals are good “candidates” for self-regulatory career planning. It is not new

    that self-efficacy beliefs may help in attaining goals (Eden & Aviram, 1993), and it is also not

    new that goal-setting procedures and goal orientations are important for performance (cf.

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 30 -

    Locke & Latham, 1990; Payne et al., 2007; Utman, 1997; Wood et al., 1987). However, it is

    new that occupational self-efficacy, mastery goals, and prestige goals are important with re-

    spect to complex and long-term measures of career success both on an objective and on a

    subjective level. More “intrinsic” mastery goals are of utmost importance in every ambitious

    occupational career. Besides this prerequisite, materialistic, “extrinsic” prestige-related goals

    are even more important in attaining objective and subjective career success. Whereas psy-

    chological theorizing sometimes treats extrinsic motivation and goals as less adaptive than

    intrinsic ones (e.g. Deci & Ryan, 1985) the present findings do not support such a perspec-

    tive.

    Another important applied implication concerns the reciprocal influence of career suc-

    cess on self-regulation. Success seems to initialize a feed-forward process of enhanced self-

    regulation which, in turn, helps to attain more success. However, there may also be the con-

    trary process in which low success does not enhance self-regulation which, in turn, does not

    help to attain more success.

    5.7 Conclusion

    We sought to study the idea that self-regulation, that largely happens in our heads, has

    an influence on factual (pay, position) and evaluative (subjective evaluation) career out-

    comes. In accord with the general assumption that purposeful thought does have an influ-

    ence on complex and long-reaching outcomes, we found that individuals with high occupa-

    tional self-efficacy beliefs and with high both mastery goals and prestige goals were more

    successful in their careers than individuals with lower values in these self-regulatory vari-

    ables. We further found evidence for a reciprocal influence of career success on self-

    regulation. Success led to feed-forward processes of goal adaptation which, in turn, influ-

    enced further success. The findings are interesting with respect to theorizing on self-

    regulation and career development, with respect to the conceptualization of goal content,

    with respect to the complex and opalescent construct of occupational career success, and

    with respect to applied perspectives of training and skills development.

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 31 -

    References

    Abele, A. E. (2003). The dynamics of masculine-agentic and feminine-communal traits:

    Findings from a prospective study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 768-

    776.

    Abele, A. E., & Stief, M. (2004). Die Prognose des Berufserfolgs von Hochschulabsol-

    ventinnen und -absolventen. Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie, 48, 4-16.

    Abele, A., Stief, M., & Andrä, M. (2000). Zur ökonomischen Erfassung beruflicher

    Selbstwirksamkeitserwartungen: Neukonstruktion einer BSW-Skala. Zeitschrift für Arbeits-

    und Organisationspsychologie, 44, 145-151. [On the economic measurement of occupational

    self-efficacy expectations]

    Allred, B. B., Snow, C. C., & Miles, R. E. (1996). Characteristics of managerial careers

    in the 21st century. Academy of Management Executive, 10, 17-27.

    Arthur, M. B., Khapova, S. N., & Wilderom, C. P. M. (2005). Career success in a bound-

    aryless career world. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 177-202.

    Arthur, M. B., & , & Rousseau, D. M. (1996). The boundaryless career: A new employ-

    ment principle for a new organizational era. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Austin, J. T., & Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal constructs in psychology: Structure, proc-

    ess, and content. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 338-375.

    Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory.

    Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.

    Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. Annual Review of

    Psychology, 52, 1-26.

    Bandura, A., & Locke, E. A. (2003). Negative self-efficacy and goal effects revisited.

    Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 87-99.

    Bandura, A., & Wood, R. (1989). Effect of perceived controllability and performance

    standards on self-regulation of complex decision making. Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology, 56, 805-814.

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 32 -

    Barling, J., & Beattie, R. (1983). Self-efficacy beliefs and sales performance. Journal of

    Organizational Behavior Management, 5, 41-51.

    Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in so-

    cial psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of

    Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.

    Barron, K. E., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2001). Achievement goals and optimal motiva-

    tion: Testing multiple goal models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 706-

    722.

    Betz, N. E., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1987). The career psychology of women. San Diego,

    CA: Academic Press.

    Biernat, M., & Billings, L. S. (2001). Standards, expectancies, and social comparison. In

    A. Tesser & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Intraindividual

    processes (pp. 257-283). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Boudreau, J. W., Boswell, W. R., & Judge, T. A. (2001). Effects of personality on execu-

    tive career success in the United States and Europe. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58, 53-

    81.

    Brett, J.F. & VandeWalle, D. (1999). Goal orientation and goal content as perdictors of

    performance in a training program. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 863-873.

    Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Principles of Self-Regulation: Action and Emo-

    tion. In E. T. Higgins, & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (Vol.

    2, pp. 3-52). New York: Guilford.

    Cervone, D., Mor, N., Orom, H., Shadel, W. G., & Scott, W. D. (2004). Self-efficacy be-

    liefs on the architecture of personality: On knowledge, appraisal, and self-regulation. In: R.

    Baumeister, & K. Vohs (Eds.) Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applica-

    tions. (pp. 188-210). New York: Guilford Press.

    Chen, G., Gully, S., & Eden, D. (2001). Validation of a new general self-efficacy scale.

    Organizational Research Methods, 4, 62-83.

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 33 -

    Day, R., & Allen, T. D. (2004). The relationship between career motivation and self-

    efficacy with protegé career success. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 64, 72-91.

    Deci, E. C., & Ryan R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human

    behaviour. New York: Plenum.

    Dette, E. D., Abele, A. E., & Renner, O. (2004). Zur Definition und Messung von Berufs-

    erfolg – Theoretische Überlegungen und metaanalytische Befunde zum Zusammenhang von

    externen und internen Laufbahnerfolgsmaßen. Zeitschrift für Personalpsychologie, 3, 170-

    183. [Defining and measuring occupational success – theoretical considerations and meta-

    analytical findings on the relationship between external and internal measures]

    Dormann, C., Fay, D., Zapf, D., & Frese, M. (2006). A state-trait analysis of job satisfac-

    tion: On the effect of core self-evaluations. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 55,

    27-51.

    Duncan, T. E., Duncan, S. C. & Strycker, L. A. (2006). An introduction to latent growth

    curve modeling – Concepts, issues, and applications. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associ-

    ates.

    Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist,

    41, 1040-1048.

    Dwyer, J. H. (1983). Statistical models for the social and behavioral sciences. New

    York: Oxford University Press.

    Earley, P. C., & Lituchy, T. R. (1991). Delineating goal and efficacy effects: a test of

    three models. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 81-98.

    Eden, D., & Aviram, A. (1993). Self-efficacy training to speed reemployment: Helping

    people to help themselves. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 352-360.

    Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7,

    117-140.

    Frieze, I. H., Olson, J. E., Murrell, A. J., & Selvan, M. S. (2006). Work Values and Their

    Effect on Work Behavior and Work Outcomes in Female and Male Managers. Sex Roles, 54,

    83-93.

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 34 -

    Furnham, A., Petrides, K.V., Tsaousis, I., Pappas, K., & Garrod, D. (2005). A cross-

    cultural investigation into the relationships between personality traits and work values. The

    Journal of Psychology, 139, 5-32.

    Gernigon, C., & Delloye, J.-B. (2003). Self-efficacy, causal attribution, and track athletic

    performance following unexpected success or failure among elite sprinters. Sport Psycholo-

    gist, 17, 55-76.

    Greene, B. A., & DeBacker, T. K. (2004). Gender and Orientations toward the future:

    Links to motivation. Educational Psychology Review, 16, 91-120.

    Greenhaus, J. H., Parasuraman, S., & Wormley, W. M. (1990). Effects of race on organ-

    izational experiences, job performance evaluations, and career outcomes. Academy of Man-

    agement Journal, 33, 64-86.

    Hall, D. T. (2002). Careers in and out of Organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E. Carter, S. M., Lehto, A. T., & Elliot, A. J. (1997).

    Predictors and consequences of achievement goals in the college classroom: Maintaining

    interest and making the grade. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1284-1295.

    Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E. Carter, S. M., Hazer, J., & Alvares, K. (1981). Police

    work values during organizational entry and assimilation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66,

    12-18.

    Heslin, P. A. (2003). Self- and other-referent criteria of career success. Journal of Ca-

    reer Assessment, 11, 262-286.

    Heslin, P. A. (2005). Conceptualizing and evaluating career success. Journal of Organ-

    izational Behavior, 26, 113-136.

    Hoelter, J. W. (1983). The analysis of covariance structures: Goodness-off-fit

    indices. Sociological Methods and Research, 11, 325–344.

    Ilies, R., & Judge, T. A. (2005). Goal regulation across time: The effects of feedback and

    affect. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 453-467.

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 35 -

    Judge, T. A., & Bono, J. E. (2001). Relationship of core self-evaluations traits – self-

    esteem, generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, and emotional stability – with job satisfac-

    tion and job performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 80-92.

    Judge, T. A., Cable, D. M., Boudreau, J. W., & Bretz, R. D. (1995). An empirical investi-

    gation of the predictors of executive career success. Personnel Psychology, 48, 485-519.

    Judge, T. A., Higgins, C. A., Thoresen, C. J., & Barrick, M. R. (1999). The big five per-

    sonality traits, general mental ability, and career success across the life span. Personnel

    Psychology, 52, 621-652.

    Judge, T. A., Jackson, C. L., Shaw, J. C., Scott, B. A., & Rich, B. L. (2007). Self-efficacy

    and work-related performance: The integral role of individual differences. Journal of Applied

    Psychology, 92, 107-127.

    Judge, T. A., Thoresen, C. J., Pucik, V., & Welbourne, T. M. (1999). Managerial coping

    with organizational change: A dispositional perspective. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84,

    107-122.

    Kahn, J. H., & Scott, N. A. (1997). Predictors of research productivity and science-

    related career goals among counseling psychology doctoral students. Counseling Psycholo-

    gist, 25, 38-67.

    Kanfer, R. (2005). Self-Regulation research in work and I/O psychology. Applied Psy-

    chology: An International Review, 54, 186-191.

    Karoly, P., Boekaerts, M., & Maes, S. (2005). Toward consensus in the psychology of

    self-regulation: How far have we come? How far do we have yet to travel? Applied Psychol-

    ogy: An International Review, 54, 300-311.

    King, Z. (2004). Career self-management: Its nature, causes and consequences. Jour-

    nal of Vocational Behavior, 65, 112-133.

    Kirchmeyer, C. (1998). Determinants of managerial career success: Evidence and ex-

    planation of male/female differences. Journal of Management, 24, 673-692.

    Klein, A., & Moosbrugger, H. (2000). Maximum likelihood estimation of latent interaction

    effects with LMS method, Psychometrica, 65, 457-474.

  • Self-Regulation and Career Success - 36 -

    Kline, R.B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York:

    The Guilford Press.

    Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance.

    Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

    Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal setting

    and task motivation: A 35-year odyssey. American Psychologist, 57, 705-717.

    Lubbers, R., Loughlin, C., & Zweig, D. (2005). Young workers