1
Much of daily communication takes the form of story narratives. Story comprehension and production depends on a logical sequence of cognitively-based story structures (e.g., story grammar), which guide interpretations, expectations, and inferences about relationships between people and events in a story. Indices of story grammar (e.g., number of total episodes, proportion of T-units within episodes) have been shown to be sensitive to brain injury, but story grammar may not completely characterize a “good” story narrative. It is possible for an individual to have high story grammar scores, reflecting ability to organize semantic content, and still produce an incomplete story. “Good” story narratives likely depend on both organization and content. INTRODUCTION DISCOURSE ANALYSIS PROCEDURE Task: After viewing a picture story with no soundtrack, each participant was instructed to retell the story. Retellings were recorded and transcribed. T-unit segmentation involved identification of each independent clause and its associated subordinate clauses for each transcript. Story grammar analysis (organization) Story grammar was examined via episode structure. An episode consists of (a) an initiating event (IE) that prompts a character to formulate a goal-directed behavior, (b) an action (A), and (c) a direct consequence (DC) marking attainment or non-attainment of the goal. The primary measure of story grammar was the proportion of T- units within episodic structure score (T-units within episodes/total T-units in retelling) combines information regarding both episodes (consisting of an initiating event, an attempt, & a direct consequence) and T-units. Story completeness measure Inventory of key events and characters produced in each transcript of the normative sample was created. Pooled into a matrix across all non-brain-injured controls, inventory items clustered into 7 distinct components. Inclusion criterion for critical component: 80% of controls (range: 83% - 98%) 5 components found to be critical Each narrative reviewed for presence of 5 critical components (1 point for each component). Completeness score: total number of critical components in each story Quantifying Goodness of Story Narratives Karen Lê 1 , Carl A. Coelho 1 , Jennifer Mozeiko 1 and Jordan Grafman 2 1-University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT & 2-National Institute for Neurological Disorders and Stroke, Bethesda, MD The present study investigated an additional measure of story narrative performance—story completeness. The story completeness measure involves a tally of the critical story components mentioned by the storyteller. It was hypothesized that by combining organizational (story grammar) and completeness measures, story goodness could be quantified. Figure 1. RESULTS Figure 3. Figure 5. Table 1: Participants Participa nts (N) Age Range in Years Years of Educati on Armed Forces Qualifica tion Test Bosto n Namin g Test Token Test Control (46) 55-76 12-20 14-95 46-60 94-100 TBI (24) 54-62 10-18 13-97 25-60 91-100 All participants were native English- speaking males, who were Vietnam War veterans. All individuals with TBI survived penetrating head wounds during the Vietnam War. Quadrant 1 Organized Incomplete Normative group: 2% Quadrant 2 Organized Complete Normative group: 83% Quadrant 3 Disorganized Incomplete Normative group: 9% Quadrant 4 Disorganized Complete Normative group: 7% DISCUSSION When measures of story organization and completeness are combined, story goodness can be quantified. The combined measure proved to be sensitive in that it classified the story narratives of control subjects into 4 distinct subgroups of story goodness. There is potential utility for analyzing narratives using the combined measure in clinical populations and lesion analyses. A moderate correlation between the story organization and story completeness measures was noted (.531, p = .01), suggesting the indices are not entirely measuring the same things. Figure 2. Performance across the two measures is plotted in quadrants. Quadrant boundaries were set at 1SD below the mean (dashed lines) for each measure: .49 for story organization (story grammar) and 3.34 for story completeness. Quadrant 1 was defined by scores of > .49 for story organization and ≤ 3.34 for story completeness. Quadrant 2 was defined by scores of > .49 for story organization and > 3.34 for story completeness. Quadrant 3 was defined by scores of ≤ .49 for story organization and ≤ 3.34 for story completeness. Quadrant 4 was defined by scores of ≤ .49 for story organization and > 3.34 for story completeness. PURPOSE PURPOSE METHODS Figure 4. Contact: [email protected] Figure 5.

Much of daily communication takes the form of story narratives. Story comprehension and production depends on a logical sequence of cognitively-based

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Much of daily communication takes the form of story narratives.  Story comprehension and production depends on a logical sequence of cognitively-based

Much of daily communication takes the form of story narratives.

Story comprehension and production depends on a logical sequence of cognitively-based story structures (e.g., story grammar), which guide interpretations, expectations, and inferences about relationships between people and events in a story.

Indices of story grammar (e.g., number of total episodes, proportion of T-units within episodes) have been shown to be sensitive to brain injury, but story grammar may not completely characterize a “good” story narrative.

It is possible for an individual to have high story grammar scores, reflecting ability to organize semantic content, and still produce an incomplete story.

“Good” story narratives likely depend on both organization and content.

INTRODUCTION

DISCOURSE ANALYSIS PROCEDURE

Task: After viewing a picture story with no soundtrack, each participant was instructed to retell the story. Retellings were recorded and transcribed.

T-unit segmentation involved identification of each independent clause and its associated subordinate clauses for each transcript.

Story grammar analysis (organization)

Story grammar was examined via episode structure. An episode consists of (a) an initiating event (IE) that prompts a character to formulate a goal-directed behavior, (b) an action (A), and (c) a direct consequence (DC) marking attainment or non-attainment of the goal.

The primary measure of story grammar was the proportion of T-units within episodic structure score (T-units within episodes/total T-units in retelling) combines information regarding both episodes (consisting of an initiating event, an attempt, & a direct consequence) and T-units.

Story completeness measure

Inventory of key events and characters produced in each transcript of the normative sample was created.

Pooled into a matrix across all non-brain-injured controls, inventory items clustered into 7 distinct components.

Inclusion criterion for critical component:80% of controls (range: 83% - 98%)5 components found to be critical

Each narrative reviewed for presence of 5 critical components (1 point for each component).

Completeness score: total number of critical components in each story

DATA ANALYSIS

Correlation coefficients were calculated for measures of story organization and story completeness.

Quantifying Goodness of Story NarrativesKaren Lê1, Carl A. Coelho1, Jennifer Mozeiko1 and Jordan Grafman2

1-University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT & 2-National Institute for Neurological Disorders and Stroke, Bethesda, MD

The present study investigated an additional measure of story narrative performance—story completeness.

The story completeness measure involves a tally of the critical story components mentioned by the storyteller.

It was hypothesized that by combining organizational (story grammar) and completeness measures, story goodness could be quantified.

Figure 1.

RESULTS

Figure 3.

Figure 5.

Table 1: Participants

Participants (N)

Age Range in Years

Years of Education

Armed Forces Qualification Test

Boston Naming Test

Token Test

Control (46) 55-76 12-20 14-95 46-60 94-100

TBI (24) 54-62 10-18 13-97 25-60 91-100

All participants were native English-speaking males, who were Vietnam War veterans. All individuals with TBI survived penetrating head wounds during the Vietnam War.

Quadrant 1OrganizedIncomplete

Normative group: 2%

Quadrant 2OrganizedComplete

Normative group: 83%

Quadrant 3DisorganizedIncomplete

Normative group: 9%

Quadrant 4DisorganizedComplete

Normative group: 7%

DISCUSSION

When measures of story organization and completeness are combined, story goodness can be quantified.

The combined measure proved to be sensitive in that it classified the story narratives of control subjects into 4 distinct subgroups of story goodness.

There is potential utility for analyzing narratives using the combined measure in clinical populations and lesion analyses.

A moderate correlation between the story organization and story completeness measures was noted (.531, p = .01), suggesting the indices are not entirely measuring the same things.

Figure 2.

Performance across the two measures is plotted in quadrants. Quadrant boundaries were set at 1SD below the mean (dashed lines) for each measure: .49 for story organization (story grammar) and 3.34 for story completeness.

Quadrant 1 was defined by scores of > .49 for story organization and ≤ 3.34 for story completeness.Quadrant 2 was defined by scores of > .49 for story organization and > 3.34 for story completeness.Quadrant 3 was defined by scores of ≤ .49 for story organization and ≤ 3.34 for story completeness.Quadrant 4 was defined by scores of ≤ .49 for story organization and > 3.34 for story completeness.

PURPOSE

PURPOSE

METHODS

Figure 4.

Contact: [email protected]

Figure 5.