95
VALIDATING THE HIERARCHY OF SOCIAL EMOTIONAL ABILITY DEVELOPMENT By JONATHAN WILLIAM ANDERSON A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 2016

© 2016 Jonathan Anderson

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

VALIDATING THE HIERARCHY OF SOCIAL EMOTIONAL ABILITY DEVELOPMENT

By

JONATHAN WILLIAM ANDERSON

A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL

OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT

OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF

MASTER OF SCIENCE

UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA

2016

© 2016 Jonathan Anderson

There is no other relationship on Earth quite like one that passes between a father and son.

Nor are two father / son relationships alike. I dedicate this thesis to my son, Robert,

from whom I have learned more about love and life than I will ever be able to repay.

5

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

As a non-traditional student, to be able to present this thesis to the Graduate School of the

University of Florida seems nothing less than a miracle. So many have been involved in

providing encouragement, emotional support, love, advice, and direction. I would like to

acknowledge and thank my family and close friends, and especially the people that are part of

the institution that is the University of Florida. In particular, I thank my son Robert for his

encouragement and support. And I thank UF for providing our family with a “reverse tradition.”

My son attended UF twenty-nine years before his Dad began his college career.

The University of Florida is the organizing focus for so many people, and attracts a

profoundly dedicated and caring faculty, staff, and student body. I am especially grateful for the

guidance of my thesis committee. I thank my committee chair, Dr. Victor Harris, without whom

I likely would not have attended graduate school, for his patient direction, guidance, and

encouragement. I thank my internal committee member, Dr. Larry Forthun for the many, many

hours spent helping me improve my critical thinking and writing skills. And I thank my external

committee member, Dr. Taylor Stein for his unwavering positive attitude, encouragement,

feedback, and direction.

I thank Dr. Tracy Johns for all the time she devoted to helping me develop a clearer

understanding of statistics and research; I thank Dr. Marilyn “Mickie” Swisher for her

completely spot-on advice regarding my approach to research. I thank Dr. Martie Gillen for

kindling my interest in research. I thank Dr. Heidi Radunovich for helping me deal with the

stress of my first semester of graduate school. And I thank Dr. Muthusami Kumaran for

encouraging me to attend graduate school in the first place. I thank Dr. Suzanna Smith for her

patient, calm, loving support. I thank Dr. David Diehl and Dr. Kevin Lancer for their help in the

development of the measurement instrument that was so critical to the success of this thesis.

6

And surprisingly, I thank Kate Fletcher for what, at the time, I considered to be her extreme

pickiness when it came to grading my APA citing attempts.

I would be remiss not to acknowledge the wonderful support staff that has helped me at

every turn. Thank You Greg Henderschiedt! Thank You Kathryn Ivey! Thank you to all of the

faculty, staff and students that make the Department of Family, Youth and Community Science

such a special collection of people. Thank you Dr. Tracy Irani for your efforts to demystify the

graduate school process, and for your efforts to reduce the stressors graduate students endure.

And thank you to all of my close friends who have lent so much love and support. It truly does

take a village… and not only for children!

7

TABLE OF CONTENTS

page

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ...............................................................................................................5

LIST OF TABLES .........................................................................................................................10

LIST OF FIGURES .......................................................................................................................12

ABSTRACT ...................................................................................................................................13

CHAPTER

1 INTRODUCTION ..................................................................................................................15

Purpose ...................................................................................................................................15 The Social Emotional Ability Development Model ...............................................................16

Introduction .....................................................................................................................16 Assumptions ....................................................................................................................17 Theoretical Framework ...................................................................................................17

Definitions ..............................................................................................................................17

2 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE ........................................................................................21

Construct Justification ............................................................................................................21 Emotional Clarity ............................................................................................................21

Identifying emotions ................................................................................................21 Understanding emotions ...........................................................................................22

Accepting emotions ..................................................................................................22 Emotional Integration ......................................................................................................24

Interpret emotions ....................................................................................................24

Emotional response ..................................................................................................24 Emotional regulation ................................................................................................25

Social Emotional Integration ...........................................................................................26 Sympathetic response ...............................................................................................26

Empathetic response .................................................................................................27

Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Theory of Development ................................................................28

Assumptions ....................................................................................................................28 Concepts ..........................................................................................................................28

Theoretical Synthesis ..............................................................................................................29 Emotional Clarity ............................................................................................................31 Emotional Integration ......................................................................................................32

Social Emotional Integration ...........................................................................................33 Summary .................................................................................................................................35 Research Questions .................................................................................................................37

8

3 METHODOLOGY .................................................................................................................39

Research Design .....................................................................................................................39 Sample ....................................................................................................................................39 Data Collection .......................................................................................................................40

Population Identification and Selection ...........................................................................40 Instrumentation ................................................................................................................41 Administration of the Instrument ....................................................................................44 Item Analysis ...................................................................................................................44

Data Analysis ..........................................................................................................................46

Reliability Analysis .........................................................................................................47 Cronbach’s alpha ......................................................................................................47 Split-sample reliability analysis ...............................................................................48

Split-half form reliability analysis ............................................................................48 Validity Analysis .............................................................................................................49

Exploratory factor analysis .......................................................................................51

Confirmatory factor analysis ....................................................................................53 Congruent validity analysis ......................................................................................54

Ethical Considerations ............................................................................................................55 Threats to Human Subjects ..............................................................................................55 Confidentiality .................................................................................................................55

Protection of Privacy .......................................................................................................56

4 RESULTS OF DATA ANALYSIS ........................................................................................60

Overview .................................................................................................................................60 Reliability Results ...........................................................................................................60

Cronbach’s alpha ......................................................................................................60 Split sample reliability testing ..................................................................................61

Split form consistency testing ..................................................................................62 Validity Results ...............................................................................................................62

Internal construct validity testing .............................................................................62

External construct validity testing ............................................................................68

5 DISCUSSION .........................................................................................................................81

Overview .................................................................................................................................81 Summary of the Findings........................................................................................................81

Research Question 1 ........................................................................................................81 Research Question 2 ........................................................................................................82

Reliability ........................................................................................................................82 Internal reliability .....................................................................................................82 External reliability ....................................................................................................82

Validity ............................................................................................................................83 Internal validity ........................................................................................................83 External validity .......................................................................................................83

Summary .................................................................................................................................84

9

Important Implications ............................................................................................................85

For Individuals .................................................................................................................85 For Educators ...................................................................................................................86 For Practitioners ..............................................................................................................86

For Researchers ...............................................................................................................86 Cautions and Limitations ........................................................................................................87

APPENDIX: INSTRUMENT ITEMS ...........................................................................................88

REFERENCES ..............................................................................................................................89

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH .........................................................................................................96

10

LIST OF TABLES

Table page

3-1 Descriptive statistics of the sample: Gender ......................................................................57

3-2 Descriptive statistics of the sample: Age ...........................................................................57

3-3 Descriptive statistics of the sample: Race ..........................................................................57

3-4 Descriptive statistics of the sample: Ethnicity ...................................................................57

3-5 Descriptive statistics of the sample: College year .............................................................58

3-6 Descriptive statistics of the sample: Total family income .................................................58

4-1 Cronbach’s alpha reliability statistics: Emotional clarity ..................................................71

4-2 Cronbach’s alpha summary item statistics: Emotional clarity ...........................................71

4-3 Cronbach’s alpha item-total statistics: Emotional clarity ..................................................71

4-4 Cronbach’s alpha reliability statistics: Emotional integration ...........................................71

4-5 Cronbach’s alpha summary item statistics: Emotional integration ....................................71

4-6 Cronbach’s alpha item-total statistics: Emotional integration ...........................................72

4-7 Cronbach’s alpha reliability statistics: Social emotional integration .................................72

4-8 Cronbach’s alpha summary item statistics: Social emotional integration .........................72

4-9 Cronbach’s alpha item-total statistics: Social emotional integration .................................72

4-10 Split sample correlations: Emotional clarity ......................................................................73

4-11 Split sample correlations: Emotional integration ...............................................................73

4-12 Split sample correlations: Social emotional integration ....................................................73

4-13 Split sample correlations: Social emotional ability score ..................................................73

4-14 Split form internal consistency: Social emotional ability inventory ..................................74

4-15 Exploratory factor analysis: KMO and Bartlett's Test .......................................................74

4-16 Exploratory factor analysis: Total variance explained .......................................................74

4-17 Exploratory factor analysis: Component correlations ........................................................74

11

4-18 Exploratory factor analysis: Pattern matrix .......................................................................75

4-19 Confirmatory factor analysis: Total variance explained ....................................................76

4-20 Confirmatory factor analysis: Factor correlations .............................................................76

4-21 Confirmatory factor analysis: Pattern matrix .....................................................................77

4-22 Pearson’s correlations, external construct validity testing: Emotional clarity ...................78

4-23 Pearson’s correlations, external construct validity testing: Emotional regulation .............78

4-24 Pearson’s correlations, external construct validity testing: Social emotional ability

level ....................................................................................................................................78

A-1 Social emotional ability inventory (Harris & Anderson, 2015) .........................................88

A-2 Emotional clarity: Difficulties in emotional regulation scale (Gratz & Roemer, 2004) ....88

A-3 Emotional regulation: Difficulties in emotional regulation scale (Gratz & Roemer,

2004) ..................................................................................................................................88

A-4 Satisfaction with life scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985) ..........................88

12

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure page

1-1 Hierarchy of Social Emotional Ability Development ........................................................20

2-1 Synthesis of the Social Emotional Ability Model and the Sociocultural Theory of

Development ......................................................................................................................38

4-1 Scree Plot of factor analyses component eigenvalues .......................................................79

4-2 Comparison of the factor structure of the SEAD, the principal components extracted

in the EFA, and the factor structure retained by the CFA .................................................80

13

Abstract of Thesis Presented to the

University of Florida Graduate School

in Partial Fulfillment of the

Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science

VALIDATING THE HIERARCHY OF SOCIAL EMOTIONAL ABILITY DEVELOPMENT

By

Jonathan Anderson

December 2016

Chair: Victor Harris

Major: Family, Youth and Community Sciences

This study introduced and validated a new theoretical model that explains processes

inherent to the development of the ability to interact with others, the Hierarchy of Social

Emotional Ability Development (SEAD). This study provided justification from the literature

for the structure and constructs of the SEAD and a synthesis between the dimensions and

constructs of the SEAD and the constructs of Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Theory of Development.

The study also provided justification for the structure and constructs of the SEAD by

quantification of Social Emotional Ability through the Social Emotional Ability Inventory

(SEAI) instrument, which was developed as a part of this study. The SEAI furnished reliable

and valid incremental measurement of social emotional ability for individuals, and differentiated

the theoretical constructs proposed by the SEAD. Following future development and refinement,

this study holds important implications for future development for individuals, practitioners,

educators and researchers. Individuals might use results from the SEAI for improving their

social interactions and life satisfaction. Practitioners might use a more fully developed SEAI

instrument as a diagnostic tool, and might use the SEAD model as a guide for helping clients

improve social interactions and life satisfaction. Educators might use the model to develop

curricula to improve student development, and for parental education so that parents might better

14

prepare their children to become more productive members of society. Researchers might use

the model and the instrument to more completely investigate and explain the relationship

between social interaction and life satisfaction.

Keywords: Emotional ability, life satisfaction, social ability, social ability development, social

emotional ability development, social engagement, social interaction

15

CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION

Social emotional ability is defined as a unique skillset of emotional, cognitive, and

behavioral ability that enables individuals to intentionally and consciously modulate their

emotional experiences and adaptively choose appropriate behaviors for the purpose of

facilitating social engagement (Broderick & Jennings, 2012). Social engagement has been a

subject of interest since 1920, when E. L. Thorndike introduced the concepts of social

intelligence and emotional intelligence (Cherniss, Extein, Goleman, & Weissberg, 2006).

Maslow’s seminal work, Hierarchy of Needs (1954), asserts that social engagement—

connectedness—is a basic human need (Huitt, 2007). Research suggests that social engagement

is an important predictor of well-being, life satisfaction, and happiness (Baumeister, Vohs,

Aaker, & Garbinsky 2013; Cialdini & Patrick, 2009; Lambert et al., 2010). Notably, both social

intelligence and emotional intelligence are defined as abilities that impact the quality of social

engagement. Thorndike (1920) defines social intelligence as the ability to understand others and

adaptively manage social engagement (Kihlstrom & Cantor, 2011). Wang, et al. (2012) defines

emotional intelligence as the ability to use emotions to improve social engagement.

Purpose

Social emotional ability is important because of the impact that quality of social

engagement exerts on individuals as well as people those individuals interact with. While social

and emotional intelligence are well represented in the body of knowledge, there is a paucity of

research concerning individual processes inherent to the development of social emotional ability.

The purpose of this study was to introduce and validate a new theoretical model that addresses

these processes, the Hierarchy of Social Emotional Ability Development (Harris & Anderson,

2015). This study expands the body of knowledge regarding the developmental processes of

16

social emotional ability by validating the constructs of the SEAD through linkages with existing

literature, and by the development of a valid, reliable instrument—the Social Emotional Ability

Inventory (SEAI)—capable of measuring levels of social emotional ability, validating the

constructs of the model, and providing incremental measurements that discriminate the

constructs of the SEAD.

The Social Emotional Ability Development Model

Introduction

The Social Emotional Ability Development (SEAD) model (Harris & Anderson, 2015)

presented a hierarchal progression of eight discrete social-emotional abilities defined within

three theoretical summative constructs (Figure 1-1). The first and foundational summary

construct is emotional clarity, which includes the following social emotional abilities: the ability

to identify; the ability to understand; and the ability to accept emotions. The second and central

summary construct, emotional integration, also includes three social emotional abilities: the

ability to interpret emotional messages; the ability to respond to one’s emotions; and the ability

to regulate emotions. The third and highest ordered summary construct, social emotional

integration, includes two social emotional abilities: the ability to respond sympathetically to

others and the ability to respond empathetically to others. The model articulates that higher

levels of social emotional ability and higher levels of empathetic response in particular, result in

more positive social engagement experiences (Allemand, Steiger, & Fend 2015; Lopes et al.,

2004). This articulation is vital to understanding social emotional ability because social

engagement is a predictor of well-being, life satisfaction, and happiness (Baumeister, Vohs,

Aaker, & Garbinsky 2013; Cialdini & Patrick, 2009; Lambert et al., 2010).

It is important to note that the SEAD is concerned with the development of higher-level

emotion-supported skills, and does not address human development processes. Moreover, the

17

SEAD model presupposes varying levels of emotional development, and does not suggest that

development of social emotional ability coincides with, or is sequentially related to, human

emotional development. For example, trait sympathy begins forming in infancy (Kienbaum,

2014), but higher levels of sympathetic abilities develop much later (Eisenberg, Cumberland,

Guthrie, Murphy, & Shepard, 2005).

Assumptions

Assumptions of the SEAD include the following: a) Development of social emotional

ability occurs in a linear progression facilitated by interactions between cognitive capacity and

environmental circumstances; b) In a manner similar to Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs (Huitt,

2007), components of the SEAD model are hierarchal and independent in that changes in

abilities in any component can occur independently of other components; c) Changes in any

ability may also influence abilities of all other components, which could impact overall levels of

social emotional ability.

Theoretical Framework

Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Theory of Development (SCTD) (1978) informs this study.

The author asserts that the SCTD provides parsimonious, logical, clear constructs that encompass

pertinent aspects of cognitive, emotional, and social development without interjecting

unnecessary complexity.

Definitions

Researchers define important concepts and constructs differently, and the terms found in

the literature are often used to define more than one subject. Therefore, constructs and concepts

in this study are defined as follows:

Accept emotions: The cognitive ability to accept and embrace emotions.

18

Content validity: Assurance that items in a measurement instrument measure what they

are intended to measure.

Discriminatory power: The capacity of an item in an instrument to differentiate among

groups of respondents.

Dimension: Definitions found in the literature for this term lack consistency. For

example, some researchers use the word dimension to define a theoretical construct. For

the purposes of this study, a dimension is a characteristic of a theoretical construct that

lends depth to the theoretical definition and measurement of the construct. A construct

may consist of one or more dimensions, and multiple items from multiple dimensions

may be used within one scale in order to better capture the full range and meaning of the

construct.

Discriminant validity: The strength with which a posited relationship among concepts

actually exists.

Emotional clarity: The latent concept that represents the ability to identify, understand,

and accept one’s emotional experiences.

Emotional integration: The use of cognition and emotion to develop various modes of

motivation, inspiration and creativity as tools in decision-making and behavioral action.

Emotional management: The ability to intentionally and consciously modulate emotional

experiences.

Emotional reactivity: An individual’s in-the-moment reaction to emotional stimuli; a

phenomenon that can be extremely difficult to attenuate in the moment. For example, the

initial reaction to emotional pain is usually reactive in nature.

Emotional response: The ability to cognitively integrate emotional meaning into the

decision-making process to help choose contextually appropriate behaviors.

Empathy: The ability to participate in what others are feeling, including the

comprehension, participation in, and vicarious experience of the emotional states of

others

Empathetic response: The ability to participate in what others are feeling, including the

comprehension, participation in, and vicarious experience of the emotional states of

others, accompanied by the ability to participate empathetically with others in social

interaction.

Identify emotions: The latent concept representing the ability to recognize, name and

label emotions, and differentiate emotional states.

Internalize: The process of incorporating attitudes or behavior into one’s nature through

learning or assimilation.

19

Interpret emotions: The ability to determine the meaning of emotional messages.

Level of current ability: The level of an individual’s cognitive, social, and emotional

ability achieved at a given point in time across the lifespan.

Reliability: The level of consistency and stability demonstrated by a measurement

instrument.

Scaffolding: A progressive process whereby lessons are provided through a sequence of

thoughtful, incremental steps of guidance or instruction from more knowledgeable others.

Scaffolding supports the learning of increasingly more complex tasks.

Social emotional ability: A unique skillset of emotional, cognitive, and behavioral ability

that enables individuals to intentionally and consciously modulate their emotional

experiences and adaptively choose appropriate behaviors for the purpose of facilitating

social engagement.

Social emotional integration: The integration of cognition, emotion, and adaptive social

interaction behaviors with others through sympathetic and empathetic responses.

Sociocultural Theory of Development: A theoretical framework proposed by Vygotsky

(1978) that describes learning as a social process and posits that social and cultural

interaction plays a fundamental role in the development of cognition.

Sympathy: Concern resulting from the emotional distress of others, accompanied by the

desire to alleviate negative feelings caused by other’s distress.

Sympathetic response: The ability to interact with others in response to the emotion,

sympathy.

Understand emotions: The ability to comprehend the meaning of emotions and to know

their nature and intensity.

Variable: For the purposes of this study, a variable is defined as a multiple-item scale

used to measure either a theoretical construct, or dimensions of a theoretical construct.

Zone of proximal development: A collection of tasks dependent upon an individual’s

inability to complete certain tasks, conceptualized as an area. Tasks that are just outside

the grasp of one’s level of current abilities which can be accomplished with the support,

instruction, guidance, and imitative modeling of more knowledgeable others are said to

be in one’s zone of proximal development.

20

Figure 1-1. Hierarchy of Social Emotional Ability Development

21

CHAPTER 2

REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE

Construct Justification

Emotional Clarity

Emotional clarity is the foundational construct of the SEAD. Emotional clarity is defined

as the ability to identify, understand, and accept one’s emotional experiences (Boden, Thompson,

Dizen, Berenbaum, & Baker, 2013; Flynn & Rudolph, 2010). Emotional clarity supports

development of more sophisticated social emotional abilities. According to Nolen-Hoeksema

(2012), people with higher scores on measures of emotional clarity also developed higher levels

of emotional ability to adaptively modulate emotions. Flynn and Rudolph (2010) posited that

emotional clarity supports later development of important emotional skills such as the ability to

understand emotional displays in others and emotional regulation capabilities in oneself. High

levels of emotional clarity are positively related to subjective well-being and adaptive

explanatory styles (Flynn & Rudolph, 2010). Conversely, people with lower levels of the ability

to understand their emotions are believed to expend greater effort managing emotions and have

more difficulty with goal-oriented behaviors (Flynn & Rudolph, 2010). Furthermore, lower

levels of emotional clarity are associated with contextually inappropriate stress responses and

reduced adaptive stress responses (Gohm, Corser, & Dalsky, 2005).

Identifying emotions

The social emotional ability to identify emotions is the first of three dimensions within

the summary construct, emotional clarity. The ability to identify emotions underpins

development of all social emotional ability, and is defined as the ability to recognize, name and

label emotions, and differentiate emotional states (Boden, Thompson, Dizen, Berenbaum, &

Baker, 2013). According to Bar-On and Parker (2000), identifying emotions is a learning

22

process begun in infancy, when perceptions of emotional signals are undifferentiated. The

ability to differentiate emotional signals and identify emotions normatively increases with

developmentally appropriate experiences. However, differentiated emotional expressions can be

quite subtle (Ekman, 2003). In addition, lower ability levels in the area of emotional processing

can have adverse consequences on social decision-making (Bar-On & Parker, 2000).

Understanding emotions

The social emotional ability to understand emotions is the second dimension of the

summary construct, emotional clarity. Understanding emotions is the ability to comprehend the

meaning of emotions and to know their nature and intensity (Helm, 2009). The ability to identify

emotions is integral to the ability to understand emotions. According to Bar-On and Parker

(2000), the ability to understand emotions begins to develop in childhood. Children who are

given clear, non-ambiguous emotional messages generally develop a greater understanding of the

nature of emotional expressions than children who do not. Bar-On and Parker (2000) posited

that infants react to changes in their mothers’ emotional expressions according to its intensity

and their own understanding of the intended emotion. This ability has important implications for

children as they grow and mature into adulthood, as the ability to understand emotions is

essential for emotional health (Bar-On & Parker, 2000). Children who have difficulty

understanding emotions are at risk for developing poor social interactions and impeded

friendship formation (Spackman, Fujiki, & Brinton, 2006). Brackett and Salovey (2006)

proposed that understanding emotions requires cognitive appraisal of emotions—an ability that

supports development of the ability to interpret emotions.

Accepting emotions

The social emotional ability to accept emotions is the third dimension of the summary

construct, emotional clarity. Emotional acceptance is the cognitive ability to accept and embrace

23

emotions, as opposed to the denial or avoidance of emotional experiences (Greenberg, 2004).

Gratz, Bornovalova, Delany-Brumsey, Nick, and Lejuez, (2007) posited that the acceptance or

avoidance of one’s emotions is rooted in experiences encountered in childhood. Greenberg

(2004) suggests that the ability to accept emotions is fundamental to the development of

emotional clarity. Acceptance of all emotions—particularly the acceptance of negative

emotions—is healthy, useful, and adaptive (Shallcross, Troy, Boland, & Mauss, 2010).

Development of the ability to accept emotions is greatly influenced by social experience, and is

dependent upon abilities to identify and understand emotional expressions (Ekman, 2003).

Correspondingly, the ability to accept one’s emotions assumes that emotions are neither good nor

bad. The ability to understand emotions facilitates development of the ability to accept

emotions. Shallcross, Troy, Boland, and Mauss (2010) suggested that some individuals have not

fully developed the ability to accept emotions, which often results in unhealthy emotional

processes. For instance, secondary emotions may be substituted for primary emotions in an

attempt to avoid experiencing emotions perceived as uncomfortable (Ekman, 2003). Both anger

and lust are often termed “secondary emotions” because they are preceded by primary emotions

such as fear, pain, jealousy, frustration, and perceptions of blocked goals (Friel & Friel, 1995).

Anger can quickly appear in some individuals as a substitute for fear, pain, or agony. Similarly,

lust may be substituted for feelings such as tenderness, safety, closeness, and sensuality (Friel &

Friel, 1995). Often, individuals will experience emotional stimuli such as hurt, anger, jealousy,

or desire, and deny these feelings and instead respond reflexively in counterproductive ways

(Ekman, 2003). The ability to recognize the range of perceived “negative or uncomfortable

emotions” as being healthy and socially useful is an important indicator of social emotional

ability development (Harris & Anderson, 2015).

24

Emotional Integration

Emotional integration is the second of three summary constructs of the SEAD.

Emotional integration is defined as the use of cognition and emotion to develop various modes of

motivation, inspiration and creativity as tools in decision-making and behavioral action. The

cognitive processes of emotional clarity support development of emotional integration.

According to Gu, Liu, Van Dam, Hof, and Fan (2013), all complex human behaviors are

determined by the integration of emotional and cognitive processes.

Interpret emotions

The ability to interpret emotions is the first of three dimensions within the summary

construct, emotional integration. The ability to cognitively interpret emotional messages (listen

to emotions) enables individuals to accurately determine what their emotions are communicating

(Matsumoto, 2009). The ability to understand and accept emotions facilitates development of

the ability to interpret the meaning of emotional messages. It is possible to have a basic

understanding of emotions and not to accept certain emotions, thereby limiting development of

the ability to determine emotional meaning (Ekman, 2003).

Emotional response

The social emotional ability to respond to emotions is the second dimension of the

summary construct, emotional integration. Emotional response is defined as the ability to

cognitively integrate emotional meaning into the decision-making process to help choose

contextually appropriate behaviors (Gottman, Katz, & Hooven, 1997). It is important to not

confuse the concept of emotional response with emotional reactivity, which is a phenomenon

that can be extremely difficult to attenuate in the moment (Williams, Bargh, Nocera, & Gray,

2009). Emotional response is concerned with behavioral decisions made over time. Decision-

making is a cognitive process that depends on emotional signals (Matsumoto, 2009), therefore,

25

the ability to accurately interpret emotional meaning supports development of the ability to

appropriately respond to emotions. Individuals with greater levels of social emotional abilities

tend to “listen” to their emotions and are, therefore, more likely to trust what their emotions

indicate. As a result, they are more able to use emotions to facilitate thought and implement

context-driven behavioral responses (Ekman, 2003). Accurate emotional interpretation supports

healthy decision-making and contextually appropriate behavioral response. However, it is

possible to identify emotions but not to accept them, thereby limiting the ability to appropriately

interpret and respond; and, decision-making without an accurate interpretation of what emotions

indicate can lead to profoundly negative social outcomes (Bar-On & Parker, 2000).

Emotional regulation

The social emotional ability to regulate emotions is the third dimension of the summary

construct, emotional integration. Emotional regulation is defined as the ability to intentionally

and consciously modulate emotional experiences (Chapman, Dixon-Gordon, & Walters, 2011).

Emotional regulation begins in childhood (Denham, Zinsser, & Brown, 2012), and the ability to

manage emotions develops across the lifespan. Effective emotional regulation is an important

predictor of successful social interaction (Ivcevic & Brackett, 2014). Development of the ability

to manage emotions is primarily facilitated through the ability to appropriately respond to

emotions. Components of emotional regulation include managing distress, controlling emotional

expression, setting appropriate priorities, and sustaining motivation (Broderick & Jennings,

2012). There is an abundance of evidence in the literature regarding the importance of emotional

regulation (Cohen, 2012; Ivcevic & Brackett, 2014; Lopes, Salovey, Côté, & Beers, 2005; Silk et

al., 2003). According to Silk et al. (2003), emotional regulation can improve mental health,

improve personal relationships, and reduce the risk for psychopathology. Individuals who score

high on emotional regulation skills tend to view themselves as more interpersonally sensitive and

26

pro-social (Lopes, Salovey, Côté, & Beers, 2005). Additionally, Denham, Bassett, and Wyatt

(2010) asserted that understanding emotions is positively associated with the management of

negative emotions. Broderick and Jennings (2012) posited that emotional dysregulation is

responsible for a wide range of social, emotional, and behavioral problems. Emotional

dysregulation has been shown to reduce social interactions and promote aggressive coping styles.

Aggressive coping styles are highly correlated with deficits in emotional regulation and have

been shown to prolong and heighten conflict (Wilton, Craig, & Pepler, 2000).

Social Emotional Integration

Social emotional integration is the SEAD’s third and highest ordered summary construct.

Social emotional integration is defined as the integration of cognition, emotion, and adaptive

social interaction behaviors with others through sympathetic and empathetic responses (Singer &

Lamm, 2009). These interactions facilitate improved social engagement (Domitrovich, Cortes,

& Greenberg, 2007). Improved social engagement is an important predictor of well-being, life

satisfaction, and happiness (Baumeister, Vohs, Aaker, & Garbinsky 2013; Cialdini & Patrick,

2009; Lambert et al., 2010). Development of social emotional integration is supported by

emotional clarity and emotional integration.

Sympathetic response

The social emotional ability to respond to sympathetic emotion is the first of two

dimensions within the summary construct, social emotional integration. Sympathetic response is

the ability to interact with others through thoughtful behavioral reaction to the emotion,

sympathy. Sympathy is defined as a concern brought forth as a result of the comprehension of

the emotional distress of others, accompanied by a desire to alleviate the other’s distress

(Eisenberg, 2000). The definition of sympathy also includes the concern or apprehension that

may be felt by a sympathizer as a consequence of the sympathizer’s boundaries being violated by

27

others who may be emotionally distressed or disturbed (Eisenberg, 2000). Extreme neediness,

lying, manipulation, inappropriate touching and expressions, stalking, etc. may all prompt

apprehension on the part of the sympathizer and the resultant need to set, establish, and maintain

clear and healthy social and emotional boundaries. When experiencing the concern of

apprehensive sympathy, the sympathetic desire to help reduce the other’s distress is still evident,

but may be reduced in accordance with the level of the boundary violation. Both the concerns of

comprehension and apprehension are integral to understanding sympathetic response.

Sympathy begins in early childhood. For example, a mother’s support can serve as a

protective factor in the development of sympathy by buffering children against unsupportive

relationships (Laible & Carlo, 2004). Children normatively begin to develop sympathetic

responses through imitative learning from their primary caretakers’ ability to sympathize.

Research indicates that adolescents who score high in trait sympathy also score high in moral

judgment, which is known to motivate pro-social behavior (Eisenberg, Zhou, & Koller, 2001).

The ability to sympathetically respond to others is fundamental to the development of higher

levels of social emotional ability, as it represents the initial integration of social and emotional

abilities as one responds to sympathetic emotions and strives to help lessen effects of distressing

emotional states in others. The development of sympathetic response ability is supported by the

preceding constructs of the SEAD, with the primary construct emotional regulation being

particularly relevant. Sympathetic response represents the initial social emotional ability that

integrates cognition, emotion, and social behavior.

Empathetic response

The social emotional ability to respond empathetically to others is the second dimension

of the construct, social emotional integration. Empathy is defined as the ability to participate in

what others are feeling and includes the comprehension, participation in, and vicarious

28

experience of the emotional states of others (Eisenberg, 2000; Prinz, 2011). Empathetic response

is defined as the ability to participate empathetically with others in social interaction. Decety

and Lamm (2006) suggested that empathy can be conceptualized as social interaction wherein

one person shares the feelings of an other; empathetic interaction with other individuals plays a

central role in social interaction. Higher levels of empathy are related to less conflict

engagement and more positive problem solving skills, which suggests that people who are more

emotionally responsive to others when faced with conflict may inhibit antisocial responses

(Wied, Branje, & Meeus, 2007). The abilities associated with emotional clarity and emotional

integration, and particularly the social emotional ability to respond sympathetically to others,

support development of the ability to respond to others empathetically. Empathetic response is

the highest order of social emotional ability, and unifies each of the other components of the

SEAD into a holistic and comprehensive ability to negotiate social-emotional states and contexts

effectively, and can be thought of as the capstone of social emotional ability development (Harris

& Anderson, 2015).

Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Theory of Development

Assumptions

The major assumptions of sociocultural theory of development are: a) learning precedes

development and results from interactions between cultural and social environments; b) learning

within these environments is dependent upon the presence of specific cognitive abilities, and is

guided through instruction from a more knowledgeable other; and, c) language constructs and

transforms development through interactive guided participation (Kraker, 2000; Waters, 2013).

Concepts

The major concepts of the sociocultural theory of development are: a) level of current

ability; b) zone of proximal development; and, c) scaffolding (Berger & Thompson, 1991; Fine

29

& Fincham, 2013; Waters, 2013). Level of current ability is defined as the level of an

individual’s cognitive, social, and emotional ability achieved at a given point in time across the

lifespan. This is the dynamic area of knowledge and ability that expands as individuals learn and

grow (Waters, 2013). Zone of proximal development is defined as a collection of tasks

dependent upon an individual’s inability to complete the tasks, conceptualized as an area. Tasks

that are just outside the grasp of one’s level of current abilities which can be accomplished with

the support, instruction, guidance, and imitative modeling of more knowledgeable others are said

to be in one’s Zone of Proximal Development (Waters, 2013). Scaffolding is defined as the

process whereby lessons are provided through a sequence of thoughtful, incremental steps of

guidance, instruction or imitative modeling from more knowledgeable others. Scaffolding

supports the learning of more complex tasks in one’s zone of proximal development (Fine &

Fincham, 2013). Examples of scaffolding include learning to ride a bicycle, and language

acquisition through various interactions such as storytelling. Scaffolding, or laddering, is

important because it explains pathways for the inclusion of complex and abstract abilities into an

individual’s level of current ability (Berger & Thompson, 1991; Pentimonti & Justice, 2010). A

good illustration of the scaffolding process is when an individual learns through instruction the

lower math skill, counting (Waters, 2013). The skill to count is thereby assimilated from one’s

zone of proximal development into one’s level of current abilities. Assimilation of this

knowledge results in the inclusion of slightly more complex lower math skills such as adding and

subtracting into one’s zone of proximal development. The assimilation of ability to add and

subtract—which would not be possible without the ability to count—further expands the

individual’s level of current abilities, and so forth (Waters, 2013).

Theoretical Synthesis

This exploratory study investigates theoretical and developmental processes of

30

emotionally supported abilities critical to effective social interaction, as differentiated by the

Hierarchy of Social Emotional Ability Development model. This new theoretical model presents

a progression of eight discrete social-emotional abilities. These abilities improve as levels of

emotional and cognitive capacity interact with environmental factors (Harris & Anderson, 2015).

As a theory concerned with learning and development across the lifespan, Sociocultural Theory

of Development (SCDT) provides justification for constructs of the SEAD (Figure 2-1).

A review of the literature supports the SCDT as guidance for this study, particularly in

light of constructs of SEAD being dependent upon interactions between higher levels of

emotional and cognitive ability, and the integration of emotional messages, cognition and

behavior. John-Steiner and Mahn (1996) posited that the sociocultural perspective provides

appropriate explanation for processes of learning and development of social emotional skills.

This perspective views learning as a progression from elementary mental functioning to higher,

more complex abstract mental functioning provided through interactions between emotional and

cognitive capacity, and thousands of social experiences (John-Steiner & Mahn, 1996).

Furthermore, Berger and Thompson (1991) posited that the SCTD can be used to explain social,

emotional, and cognitive skills development. As individuals expand their social and emotional

skills beyond existing levels of competence through supportive and imitative instruction, they

complete tasks within their “zone of proximal development,” which is the area of ability just

beyond the reach of one’s existing means and strategies. As tasks are completed, learning occurs

and is internalized, providing cognitive social and emotional skills development. Internalization

is defined as the process of incorporating attitudes or behavior into one's nature through learning

or assimilation. Berger and Thompson (1991) suggested that a series of progressively complex

tasks completed in this manner, referred to as scaffolding, facilitates the ability to learn and

31

internalize progressively more complex cognitive, social, and emotional skills (Pentimonti &

Justice, 2010). French (2007) and John-Steiner and Mahn (1996) suggested that development of

supportive social interaction holds important implications for social and emotional ability

development, as new knowledge is created when individuals internalize learning that is

appropriated through social participation. Social-emotional abilities are rooted in this process of

emotional and social development, and subsequent interactions between cognitive capacity and

environmental factors.

Emotional Clarity

Emotional clarity, the foundational summary construct of the SEAD, can best be

explained through this scaffolding process of Sociocultural Theory of Development. The

concept, emotional clarity, is defined in the literature and represented in the SEAD as the ability

to identify, understand, and accept one’s emotional experiences (Boden et al., 2013; Flynn &

Rudolph, 2010). John-Steiner and Mahn (1996) suggested that building knowledge is a process

that begins with elementary mental functioning, which facilitates increasingly complex mental

functioning through the scaffolding process of SCTD (Pentimonti & Justice, 2010). The

summary construct of the SEAD, emotional clarity, represents the progression from the less

complex social emotional ability to identify, to the more complex social emotional ability to

understand, to the complex social emotional ability to accept one’s emotions. Waters (2013)

suggested that the nature of this progression exactly parallels concepts of the Sociocultural

Theory of Development. This makes sense, as the ability to recognize, name, and label one’s

emotions is a comparatively elementary cognitive function necessary to the ability to understand

one’s emotions. Clearly, understanding one’s emotions is a slightly more complex function than

the ability to identify emotions, and development of the ability to understand emotions without

first having the ability to identify emotions is not possible, much like learning to solve math

32

problems is not possible without first learning to count. Therefore, the level of development of

one’s ability to understand emotions would be partially dependent upon the level of one’s ability

to identify emotions. It also makes sense that the cognitive ability to accept and embrace

emotions is comparatively more abstract and slightly more complex than the abilities to identify

and understand emotions. Thus, this study posits that the summary construct, emotional clarity,

can best be explained by the scaffolding effects asserted by the SCTD and how individual levels

of cognitive social emotional skills inherent in emotional clarity are dependent upon the levels of

development of progressively more complex abilities to identify, understand, and accept one’s

emotions.

Emotional Integration

Emotional integration, the second summary construct of the SEAD, is also best explained

through the scaffolding process of Sociocultural Theory of Development (Pentimonti & Justice,

2010). The concept, emotional integration, is defined in the literature and the SEAD as the

ability to integrate emotion with cognition and behavior through emotional interpretation,

emotional response, and emotional management (Ochsner, Silvers, & Buhle, 2012). This

sequencing of the abilities to cognitively interpret and incorporate emotional meaning into the

decision-making and behavioral selection process represents progressively more complex

scaffolding that integrates emotion in support of thought, and results in development of the

highly complex and abstract social ability to manage emotions through the integration of

emotion, thought and behavioral processes (Pentimonti & Justice, 2010). According to Gu, Liu,

Van Dam, Hof, and Fan (2013), all complex human behaviors are determined by the integration

of emotional and cognitive processes. Ochsner, Silvers, and Buhle (2012) suggested the

integration of emotion, thought and behavior is deployed in explicit strategies to regulate one’s

emotions through cognitive selection from an array of behavioral choices. There is an

33

abundance of evidence in the literature regarding the importance of emotional management and

its impact on social engagement (Cohen, 2012; Ivcevic & Brackett, 2014; Lopes, Salovey, Côté,

& Beers, 2005; Silk et al., 2003).

Ochsner, Silvers, and Buhle (2012) suggests a progressive integration of emotion,

thought and behavior that parallels the development of higher mental processes described by

concepts of the Sociocultural Theory of Development. This is a logical progression. The ability

to cognitively interpret emotional meaning is a comparatively complex high mental function

necessary to development of the ability to integrate emotion with thought in support of the highly

complex and abstract decision-making processes. And it is clear that integrating emotion and

thought into the decision-making process to assist in the choice of contextually appropriate

behaviors is a more complex and abstract function than the ability to integrate emotion with

thought to assist in emotional interpretation. Similarly, the development of the ability to respond

to emotions without first having the ability to interpret emotions also does not make sense.

Therefore, the level of development of one’s ability to respond to emotions would be partially

dependent upon the level of one’s ability to interpret emotions. Logically, the highly complex

ability to respond to and manage emotions is comparatively more complex and abstract than the

abilities to interpret and respond to emotional messages. Thus, this study posited that the

summary construct, emotional integration, can best be explained by the scaffolding effects

asserted by the SCTD, and that individual levels of development of emotional integration

abilities are dependent upon the levels of development of progressively more complex abilities to

interpret, respond to emotions, and regulate emotions.

Social Emotional Integration

Social emotional integration, the highest ordered of the three summary constructs of the

Social Emotional Ability Development model, can also be explained through the scaffolding

34

process of the Sociocultural Theory of Development. The concept, social emotional integration,

is defined in the literature and the SEAD as the integration of cognition, emotion, and adaptive

social interaction behavior with others through sympathetic and empathetic responses (Singer &

Lamm, 2009). The sequencing of the ability to incorporate emotional integration with social

interactions with others through sympathetic response represents progressively more complex

scaffolding that results in development of the very complex and abstract social emotional ability

to interact with others through empathetic responses. According to Decety and Michalska,

(2010), sympathetic and empathetic responses are the basis for most social interaction, and

empathy is among the highest ordered emotions. Decety and Lamm (2006) suggested that

empathy could be conceptualized as social interaction wherein one person shares the feelings of

another person, with empathetic interaction playing a central role in social interaction.

Moreover, levels of empathic response are positively related to purposeful emotional regulation

(Decety & Michalska, 2010). There is an abundance of evidence in the literature regarding the

importance of empathetic ability and its impact on social engagement (Decety and Lamm, 2006;

Singer & Lamm, 2009; Wied, Branje, & Meeus, 2007).

Decety and Lamm (2006) showed a progressive relationship between sympathetic

response and empathetic response that parallels the development of higher mental processes

described by concepts of the Sociocultural Theory of Development. This progression makes

sense, as sympathetic response occurs as a result of experiencing the emotion, sympathy; and the

resulting social interaction is limited to attempts to make the person in distress feel better

(Eisenberg, 2000). Empathetic response, on the other hand, is far more complex, as it involves

complex interactions and behaviors that address many emotions in others (Decety & Lamm,

2006). The ability to respond sympathetically is a complex, highly abstract mental function

35

necessary to development of the ability to respond empathetically. It makes sense that the

logical integration of emotion, thought, and external behavior into the ability to respond

empathetically to the emotions of others is a far more complex and higher abstract function than

the ability to integrate emotion, thought, and external behavior into the ability to respond

sympathetically to the emotions of others. Development of the ability to respond empathetically

to others without first having the ability to respond sympathetically to others would not be

possible. Therefore, the level of development of one’s ability to respond to others empathetically

would be partially dependent upon one’s ability to respond sympathetically. It is apparent that

the highly complex ability to respond empathetically to the emotions of others is far more

complex and abstract than the abilities to respond to others on the basis of one’s own

sympathetic feelings. Thus, this study posited that the summary construct, social emotional

integration, can best be explained by the scaffolding effects asserted by the SCTD, and that

individual levels of social emotional integration abilities are dependent upon the levels of

development of progressively more complex abilities to respond to the emotions of others

sympathetically and the ability to respond to the emotions of others empathetically.

Summary

Even though social interaction is a critical building block of civilization itself, little exists

in the body of knowledge that explains the processes at play regarding how individuals develop

the social emotional ability necessary to interact effectively with others. The purpose of the

present study was to expand the body of knowledge regarding developmental processes of social

emotional ability among individuals by providing justification from the body of knowledge for

constructs of the new theoretical model, the Hierarchy of Social Emotional Ability Development.

This current study was guided by the theoretical framework of Vygotsky’s Sociocultural

Theory of Development, and included a review of the literature that justifies the validity of the

36

SEAD constructs. It also included a synthesis between constructs of the Sociocultural Theory of

Development and constructs of the SEAD theoretical model.

The summary constructs of the SEAD have been shown to be justifiable in both the body

of knowledge and through theoretical linkages; and the hierarchal progression of the SEAD is

logical. The first summary construct, emotional clarity, is comprised of the lower-level cognitive

abilities to identify, understand and accept emotions. It makes sense that these three constructs

would be defined within the summary construct, emotional clarity. It is also logical that these

abilities are progressive in that it would not be possible that the ability to accept emotions could

be fully developed without first having the ability to understand emotions; and it is apparent that

the ability to understand emotions is dependent upon first attaining the ability to identify

emotions.

The next summary construct, emotional integration, is comprised of the more complex

abilities to interpret, respond to, and regulate emotions. These abilities are more complex in that

they progressively integrate cognition, emotion and behavior. It stands to reason that these three

abilities are defined within the summary construct, emotional integration. It also makes sense

that these abilities are progressive in that it is not possible that the ability to regulate emotions

could be fully developed without first having the ability to respond to emotions; and logically,

the ability to respond to emotions is dependent upon the ability to interpret emotional meaning.

The third and highest ordered summary construct, social emotional integration, is

comprised of the progressively more complex and abstract ability to respond sympathetically to

others with the goal of validation and making that person feel better, and the even more complex

and abstract ability to respond empathetically with others with a wide variety of complex and

abstract emotionally based behaviors. Social emotional integration is the highest ordered

37

summary construct because it has been shown that empathy and sympathy are responsible for

much of healthy human social interaction (Singer & Lamm, 2009). This makes sense, as

sympathy and empathy are ways of connecting with others through caring concern. And it

stands to reason that individuals are attracted to those that express genuine concern for them.

Research Questions

This exploratory study was driven by the following research questions: 1). What are the

justifiable constructs of social emotional ability? 2). How can the constructs of the SEAD be

quantified in a valid and reliable survey instrument? To explore potential answers to the first

research question, the researcher justified the eight dimensions of the SEAD by exploring

supportive research from the literature, and provided linkages between existing theoretical

frameworks and the SEAD constructs. To explore potential answers to the second research

questions, the researcher developed the Social Emotional Ability Inventory instrument to provide

reliable and valid incremental measurement of social emotional ability for individuals, and

differentiate the theoretical constructs proposed by the SEAD model.

38

Figure 2-1. Synthesis of the Social Emotional Ability Model and the Sociocultural Theory of

Development

39

CHAPTER 3

METHODOLOGY

Research Design

The present quantitative research was an exploratory, non-random sample study designed

to answer two research questions: 1). What are the justifiable constructs of social emotional

ability? 2). How can the constructs of the SEAD be qualified in a valid and reliable survey

instrument? The study design was cross-sectional, which is appropriate for this study; data were

collected from each participant at one point in time in order to collect quantitative data for two or

more variables that were analyzed to detect patterns of association (Bryman, 2012). In order to

answer the research questions, the body of knowledge was reviewed and examined to provide

construct and structure justification for the theoretical Hierarchy of Social Emotional Ability

Development Model; and the SEAI instrument was constructed to provide reliable and valid

incremental measurement of social emotional ability for individuals, and to differentiate the

theoretical constructs of the SEAD.

Sample

The theoretical population was typical college students. The accessible population was

college students attending the University of Florida. The sampling method was non-random

volunteer. Random sampling was not employed as there were budget and time constraints, and

random sampling is not necessary for exploratory studies where the goal is not generalization

(Baker et el., 2013).

Two hundred thirty-six respondents were recruited from classes at the University of

Florida. Cleaning of the data begun with an inspection for completeness of each case; four

respondents failed to answer multiple items sequentially, and these cases were dropped. The

data were then inspected for missing data and outliers with IBM’s Statistical Package for the

40

Social Sciences (SPSS). Five cases were found that contained between one and four data points

with missing data, and four cases were found that contained univariate outliers. These cases

were deleted listwise. As there were relatively few cases with missing data or outliers, imputing

data replacement techniques were not employed and an inspection for the randomness of missing

data was not necessary (Williams, 2015).

After cleaning, the full sample size was 223 (N = 223). The sample was composed of

191 females and 32 males ranging from 19 to 32 years of age, with a mean age of 21 (SD =

1.23). Sixty-two percent of the sample was White (n = 138); 24% were Black or African

American (n = 54); 6% were Asian (n = 13), American Indian or Alaskan Native (n = 1), or

Pacific Islander (n = 1). Seven percent identified as “Other” (n = 15). One respondent chose to

not identify. Twenty-one percent (n = 46) of the participants described their ethnicity as

Hispanic, Latino, or of Spanish origin. More than half the participants (51%) were college

seniors (n = 114), more than one third (36%) were juniors (n = 81), and 12% were sophomores

(n = 27). One participant was a freshman. More than one-third (35%) reported a total family

income of less than $50,000 per year (n = 78). For a complete description of the sample, see

Tables 3-1 through Table 3-6.

Data Collection

Population Identification and Selection

Participants were students recruited from the University of Florida College of Agriculture

and Life Sciences, Department of Family, Youth and Community Sciences. All participants took

part in this study as volunteers, and were compensated for their time with extra credit classroom

points. Students attending the same classes who did not participate in the study were awarded

extra classroom points as well. Prior to recruiting, approval from UF’s IRB-02 was obtained.

Students who participated were directed to read the “Letter of Information” which identified the

41

nature of he study and the kinds of questions presented. Included in the Letter of Information

was an assessment of possible risks and rewards for taking part in this study. Participants were

advised that their survey responses would be completely anonymous, and no personally

identifiable information was collected or stored.

Instrumentation

Reliability and validity go hand-in-hand; therefore, the methodology employed for the

development of the SEAI instrument was informed by the theoretical definition of each

dimension of constructs of the SEAD, and the framework provided by the theoretical synthesis.

Fifty-six items were proposed as appropriate to the measurement of the constructs of the SEAD.

Items were based on the theoretically defined dimensions of each construct. In order to ensure

reliability and validity, the author recruited a consensus panel to assist with the development of

the SEAI. The panel consisted of six highly qualified experts who provided editorial suggestions

regarding validity of instrument content in relation to the theoretical constructs, the validity of

the items as measurements of the constructs, and the appropriateness of the instrument structure

from the perspective of context of the study population.

Recruitment of members for the consensus panel was based upon their expertise in areas

related to social and emotional developmental processes, such as inter- and intra-familial

relationships; human emotional development; family programming and evaluation; clinical

psychological counseling, and other fields related to topics important to this study. One panel

member had extensive knowledge and experience in the field of familial emotional relationships.

One panel member had extensive knowledge and experience in statistical analysis, and two panel

members had extensive knowledge and experience in the field of survey instrument construction

and data collection. All panel members held Ph.D. degrees. Two prospective panel members

declined to participate; one because she did not feel her expertise fit the subject matter closely

42

enough, and one because she would not have been able to dedicate the time necessary to help

produce a quality instrument. The panel of experts provided face validity and content validity

for the instrument through consensus of expert opinion, and validated both the form and structure

of the instrument.

Construction of individual items focused on methodology providing reliable and valid

results. Items were constructed in such a manner as to measure indicators and contra-indicators

of the same topics. This method of measuring the same topic from different perspectives

provides support for triangulation, which helps ensure reliability and facilitates construct validity

by providing convergent and discriminatory validity (Bryman, 2012; Morse, 2015). To further

support validity, items within each of the variables were based upon dimensions that define each

respective construct. For example, the item “I often cannot tell what emotion I am feeling,” is an

item subsumed within the variable that measures the overall construct of emotional clarity. The

indicator for this item, ability to identify emotions, is one of three dimensions of the construct,

emotional clarity. The other two dimensions, accepting emotions and understanding emotions,

are also indicators of items within the variable that measures the construct, emotional clarity.

Examples of the nature of items in the SEAI are: “Please read each of the following

statements carefully, and estimate the strength with which each statement applies to you; 1)

“Understanding my emotions helps me improve my relationships;” 2) “I frequently use what I

think my emotions mean to help me take better care of myself.” Each item provided seven data

collection points ranging from zero to six points.

Following five iterations of editing and revision by the consensus panel, the SEAI

instrument was reduced from fifty-six items to forty items. The construct, emotional clarity, was

defined by three dimensions and measured by one variable comprised of fifteen items. The

43

construct, emotional integration, was also defined by three dimensions and measured by one

variable comprised of fifteen items. The construct, social-emotional integration, was defined by

two dimensions, and measured by one variable comprised of ten items.

Subsequent to the panel of experts reaching consensus regarding the ability of the

instrument to measure the theoretical constructs and provide reliable, valid incremental

measurement of social emotional ability, cognitive testing of the instrument was conducted

among seven individuals recruited from the accessible population. The purpose of this testing

was to identify covert issues such as appropriateness of item interpretation in the context of the

study population, clarity of instructions, and logical flow of item order. On the basis of this

qualitative analysis, four items were reworded. Data were then collected for a pilot study. The

purpose of the pilot study was to identify overt issues such as non-random missing data,

miscoded items, and issues of a similar nature. Data from the pilot study (N = 27) were

examined, and three miscoding errors were uncovered and corrected.

Following pilot and cognitive testing, items where added for collection of demographic

information such as age, gender, race, ethnicity, education, and total monthly family income.

Items were also added to provide for assessment of external convergent validity. Two sub

indices were adopted from the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) (Gratz &

Roemer, 2004). The DERS is a well-documented, established valid and reliable instrument.

These sub-indices were appropriate for assessing convergent validity because they measured the

same or very similar concepts as those found in the instrument, they were adopted in their

entirety, and employed identical metrics (Agarwal, 2011).

The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) was also adopted and inserted into the

instrument in its entirety (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985). The SWLS was a well-

44

documented, valid and reliable instrument designed to measure life satisfaction. It also

employed identical metrics as those found in the SEAD instrument. The Social Emotional

Ability Development model posits that higher levels of social emotional ability will generally

result in more positive social engagement experiences; and, social engagement is a predictor of

well-being, life satisfaction, and happiness (Harris & Anderson, 2015). This concept is also

reflected in the literature (Baumeister, Vohs, Aaker, & Garbinsky 2013; Cialdini & Patrick,

2009; Lambert et al., 2010). It stands to reason that if the SEAD instrument provided reliable,

valid measurement of social emotional ability, then a positive convergent relationship would

exist between the SEAD Score and the SWLS Score.

Administration of the Instrument

Data were collected through online, anonymous self-assessment techniques using the

Qualtrics assessment tool. Participants were provided access to the survey for one week and

were allowed to navigate forward and backward throughout the survey. Participants were able to

save their progress toward completion of the survey instrument and to complete it at a later time

within the designated time frame, if needed.

Item Analysis

Following data collection, preliminary cognitive and statistical analyses were conducted

to identify items likely to impact negatively on the reliability and validity of the instrument. To

guide cognitive assessment of the suitability of individual items, Cronbach’s alpha testing was

performed to provide alpha correlation coefficients, inter-item correlation summaries, and item

total correlations (Clark & Watson, 1995; UCLA, 2016). Three items were determined to be

unsuitable for their intended measurement and were removed, and two items that returned

negative item-total correlations were removed. Following Cronbach’s alpha testing,

discriminatory power testing was performed using Mann-Whitney U-Testing (Corder &

45

Foreman, 2014). Clark and Watson (1995), suggested that items returning greater than U = .5

for either the upper and lower rankings provide inadequate discriminatory power, and should be

considered for removal. Consequently, two additional items were removed. As a result of the

preliminary item analysis, seven items were removed from the instrument.

To finalize the item analysis process and identify items that might detract from the ability

of the instrument to measure their intended constructs as differentiated by the SEAD, the revised

inventory was subjected to a principal component analysis (PCA). Williams, Brown, and

Onsman (2012) suggested that principal components analysis was useful as a methodology for

reducing multiple items into common factors and identifying items that measured latent

concepts, and those that did not. To be significant, Beavers et al. (2013) asserted that

correlations should be at least .4, and avoid cross loading onto other components by being at

least .2 greater than any other correlation loadings for the same variable. Commensurate with

these guidelines, five items were identified that loaded onto components they had not been

intended to measure, or cross loaded onto components that measured more than one latent

concept as differentiated by the SEAD. Correspondingly, these five items were removed from

the inventory (Table 3-7).

In sum, item analysis identified a total of twelve items that were determined to have a

negative influence on reliability and validity of the instrument, and were consequently removed

from the instrument. This revised instrument that was subjected to data analysis consisted of

three sub scales that measured respective constructs of the SEAD. The first construct, emotional

clarity, was defined by three dimensions and measured by one variable comprised of eleven

items. The second construct, emotional integration, was also defined by three dimensions and

measured by one variable comprised of eleven items. The third construct, social-emotional

46

integration, was defined by two dimensions, and measured by one variable comprised of six

items. According to Hinkin, Tracey, and Enz (1997), there are no standard rules that dictate how

many items are necessary to measure a construct. However, these authors suggest that an

instrument must provide evidence of internal reliability and validity, and all constructs should be

measured by at least five items.

Data Analysis

IBM’s Statistical Package for the Social Sciences Version 23 was utilized to properly

explore the ability of the SEAD instrument to provide reliable, valid, incremental measurement

of social emotional ability. To summarize, twelve items had been removed from the SEAI

during the item analysis process. Accordingly, the dataset was revised to contain only data

collected by the remaining twenty-eight items, plus data collected by three sub scales that had

been inserted into the instrument to provide evidence of external validity, as previously

discussed. In keeping with the study objectives, statistical analysis for this study focused upon

assessing reliability and validity. The sequencing of data analysis was internal reliability tests

that included Cronbach’s alpha testing, split-sample reliability testing, and split form consistency

testing. Data were then analyzed for validity, which consisted of internal validity testing

utilizing factor analyses (including exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis). Results of the

factor analyses were inspected for characteristics of convergent and discriminant validity, and

principal components and principal factors were compared to the expected structure and factors

as differentiated by the SEAD model. Finally, data were analyzed for external construct validity,

which consisted of comparing correlations between scores provided by the instrument and scores

provided by external scales measuring the same or very similar concepts.

47

Reliability Analysis

Cronbach’s alpha

According to Gliem and Gliem (2003), Cronbach’s alpha coefficient, when examined in

conjunction with accompanying correlation summaries of inter-item and item-total correlations,

is one of the most widely used indicators of the internal reliability of quantitative survey

instruments. Cronbach’s Alpha testing was employed in this study to assess internal reliability.

The SEAD instrument satisfied the necessary assumptions for Cronbach’s alpha testing, which

are: The instrument is composed of multiple items that provide a composite score; each item

measures a property that varies quantitatively; there are no “right” or “wrong” responses to the

items; and responses to items provide ratings for each item (Gliem & Gliem, 2003).

Neuendorf (2016) asserted that Cronbach’s alpha can be sensitive to the number of items

measuring a construct and articulated that the mean inter-item correlation is a more consistent

indicator of internal reliability. Neuendorf (2016) suggested that a mean inter-item correlation

between .2 and .4 provides the optimum level of homogeneity. Clark and Watson (1995) posited

that the optimal mean inter-item correlation varies according to the complexity of the construct

being measured and suggested that, when measuring a more complex construct, a range of .15 to

.20 would be more appropriate. Similarly, they posited that when measuring less complex

constructs, a mean inter-item correlation between .40 and .50 would be appropriate.

Furthermore, Clark and Watson (1995), proposed that Cronbach’s alpha for exploratory

social science studies should generally be greater than .6, inter-item total correlation coefficient

means should be greater than .2, item-total coefficients should be greater than .3, and items that

return negative item-total coefficients should be considered for removal. Acceptable thresholds

applied to results for the current study were more conservative. Alpha coefficients greater than

48

.7; inter-item correlation averages generally greater than .2; and item total correlations greater

than .3 were deemed acceptable. Hof (2012) suggested that these thresholds are well within

acceptable ranges for this type of investigation.

Split-sample reliability analysis

According to Swisher (2016), an effective methodology for assessing internal reliability

would be provided by using Cronbach’s alpha testing in combination with split-sample

correlation. In addition to Cronbach’s alpha testing, internal reliability was assessed by

randomly splitting the sample in half and calculating correlations between composite SEAD

score and construct scores for each half. Data met assumptions necessary for calculating

correlations using Pearson’s correlations, as data compared two continuous variables that

represented two paired observations. Examination of scatterplots revealed elliptically shaped

patterns, which indicated that a linear relationship existed between the variables. No outliers

were identified in the data and normality testing ensured normal distribution (Kang, & Harring,

2012). According to Wuensch (2012), a correlation of greater than .2 (p = <.05) is evidence of a

statistically significant relationship, and was therefore used to determine significance of Person’s

correlations.

Split-half form reliability analysis

Consistency is integral to internal reliability. According to Bhattacherjee (2012), split-

half form testing is a measure of consistency between two halves of an instrument. The

instrument was split in half by alternate items (Bhattacherjee, 2012). Split form internal

consistency reliability assessment was provided by calculating Cronbach’s alpha, the Pearson

correlation between the two forms, and the Spearman-Brown split-half reliability coefficient

between scores for each half (Garson, 2009).

49

Validity Analysis

According to Agarwal (2011), providing evidence of construct validity for newly

developed instruments such as the Social Emotional Ability Inventory is critical. Construct

validity is defined as the degree to which inferences can be made from theoretical constructs on

which a study is based (Agarwal, 2011). Clark and Watson (1995) proposed that construct

validity has three components: first, a construct must be framed within a sound theoretical

framework; second, there must be internal statistical evidence of convergent and discriminate

validity; and third, evidence must be provided supporting external convergent and discriminant

validity. Correspondingly, evidence was provided to demonstrate that the constructs of the

SEAD were soundly based in theory; internal construct validity was assessed by providing

statistical evidence of the internal convergent and discriminant validity of the data; and external

construct validity was assessed by providing statistical evidence of external convergent validity

developed through the comparison of statistical relationships between measurements of the

constructs of SEAD and the same or very similar concepts found in the literature. External

discriminant validity testing was not performed.

Internal construct validity: Clark and Watson (1995) proposed that internal construct

validity cannot be tested directly, but evidence of internal construct validity must be verified

through convergent and discriminant characteristics of the items measuring theoretical

constructs. Williams et al. (2012) suggested that factor analysis was an effective methodology

for identifying and confirming latent constructs. Factor analysis is a methodology often

employed when validating a new instrument by providing evidence of internal construct validity

and confirming that an instrument reflects the measurement of intended constructs (Byrne, 2001;

William et al., 2012). Beavers et al. (2013) asserted that principal component analysis and

principal axis factoring (PAF) are the most common methodologies employed in factor analysis,

50

and the results from both methods are quite similar. These authors suggested performing

reliability testing prior to factor analysis to improve accuracy of the results, as this facilitates

interpretation of communality correlation matrices. Correspondingly, reliability testing was

performed prior to factor analysis.

Even though PCA and PAF yield similar results, it was important to understand that there

are substantial differences between the two methodologies. For example, PCA identifies

components of the structure of the instrument caused by item scores, while PAF identifies the

actual factors of an instrument and allows for the identification of the structure of factors thought

to be reflective of the items that measure a construct (Byrne, 2001). In other words, PCA reveals

components caused by resulting scores, and PAF identifies the structure of factors caused by

individual items. There are also important differences in the ways in which the methods measure

variance. Principal component analysis takes into account all variance, including common

variance, specific variance, and measurement error, and therefore produces greater reported

values (Beavers et al., 2013), which results in greater opportunity for cross loading. Principal

axis factoring reports only variance in common, which removes variance error and specific

variance, and provides more accurate but lower correlations (Beavers et al., 2013).

Taking these differences into consideration, this study employed an exploratory factor

analysis using PCA to assess relationships between items and groups of items, and to provide

evidence of internal construct validity. A confirmatory factor analysis was also performed

utilizing PAF as a form of structural equation modeling to define and confirm the underlying

structure of the theoretical constructs of the SEAD as measured by the Social Emotional Ability

Inventory (Pett, Lackey, & Sullivan, 2003; Williams et al., 2012).

51

The factor analyses were appropriate to their intended purposes. According to Pett et al.

(2003), a principal component analysis should be used to establish preliminary solutions when

performing an exploratory factor analysis, and Garson (2007) asserted that the traditional

methodology employed for structural equation modeling was a confirmatory factor analysis

employing principal axis factoring, as researchers could examine factor loadings to determine if

the intended items indeed loaded on latent factors as predicted by the theoretical model.

Exploratory factor analysis

Barry, Chaney, Stellefson, and Chaney (2011) recommended principal component

analysis as the most appropriate methodology for exploratory factor analyses intended for the

assessment of convergent and discriminant validity. Correspondingly, an exploratory factor

analysis was performed to determine if items that converged onto principal components also

failed to load out of construct, as differentiated by the SEAD (Williams et al., 2012). Because

the SEAD instrument was constructed to provide reliable, valid measurement of social emotional

ability as differentiated by constructs of the SEAD model, it was expected that the components

of the instrument would reflect the theoretical constructs of the SEAD model. Consequently, the

primary objective of the exploratory factor analysis was to explore the component structure of

the instrument and also provide evidence of internal construct validity for the instrument, and

evidence of the validity of the SEAD model. Barry, Chaney, Stellefson, and Chaney (2011)

recommended PCA as the most appropriate methodology for an exploratory factor analysis.

Correspondingly, an EFA was conducted employing PCA to assess internal convergent validity,

discriminant validity, and underlying component structure. Prior to the exploratory factor

analysis, data were tested to determine suitability. The value of the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO)

measure of sampling adequacy was inspected to ensure linear relationships greater than .6

(Kaiser, 1970, 1974). Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity was inspected for significance (Bartlett,

52

1954), and the total item correlation matrix was inspected for items having a substantial number

of correlations greater than .3. It should be noted that one of the assumptions of factor analysis

was violated, as data were not collected through random sampling. However, Clark and Watson

(1995) asserted that in real-world analysis, factor analysis assumptions were routinely not met,

and depending on the type of investigation, this rarely affected study outcome. As this was an

exploratory study, it was not expected that non-random sampling significantly affected the study

outcome.

Williams et al. (2012) articulated that factor matrices are complex and often rotated to

facilitate interpretation. Osborne and Costello (2005) suggested that when extracted components

were expected to be related — as was the case with this data — results might best be interpreted

if the communalities correlation matrix was subjected to a Promax rotation. Consequently, this

analysis was conducted using a Promax (k=6) rotation. As suggested by Agarwal (2011), only

principal components that appeared above the “bend” of the scree plot with an eigenvalue greater

than 1 were considered for retention. Williams et al. (2012) asserted that there was a wide

latitude regarding what constituted an acceptable level of accumulated percentage of variance

explained by extracted components, but in the social science studies it was commonly between

50% and 60%, and often as low as 40%. The acceptable level of accumulated percentage of

variance explained for this study was set at 50%.

Beavers et al. (2013) proposed that a “simple factor structure” was the most desirable and

interpretable factor matrix. A simple structure was defined as one composed of at least three

items per component having strong loadings that did not exhibit significant secondary cross

loadings. As is the case with many such thresholds, guidelines for determining specifically what

might constitute a significant secondary or cross loading varied widely in the literature. Beavers

53

et al. (2013) suggested that to be considered significant, correlations should generally be at least

.4, and avoid secondary or cross loading onto other components by being at least .2 greater than

any other correlation loadings for the same variable. Therefore, for this present study, significant

communality correlations were those that generally loaded on components with r values of .4 or

greater, and were at least .2 greater than any other loadings for the same variable.

Correspondingly, items that loaded at less than .4 were suppressed in the communality

correlation matrix output.

Confirmatory factor analysis

Byrne (2001) recommended principal axis factoring as the most appropriate methodology

for a confirmatory factor analyses intended to identify the structure of an instrument and confirm

that the underlying proposed factors were reflective of the items used to measure the theoretical

latent constructs. Therefore, a confirmatory factor analysis was conducted to assess and confirm

the definition and structure of the factors that underlie the latent theoretical constructs of the

SEAD by performing principal axis factoring. Suitability of the data for factor analysis was

previously reported, as was justification for rotation for the communalities correlation matrix.

The communalities correlation matrix was subjected to a Promax (k = 6) rotation, and output for

items that loaded with r values less than .4 were suppressed. Thresholds for significance of

factors loadings were identical to those justified for principal component loadings.

In keeping with best practices for confirmatory factor analysis, factor structure was then

assessed by comparing theoretical intent to inductively identify, interpret, name, and group

identified factors according to construct (Williams et al., 2012). As Williams et al. (2012)

pointed out, the reason for factor analysis in the first place is to identify items with high in-factor

loading matrices that, when taken as a whole, explain the majority of item responses.

54

Congruent validity analysis

The study provided evidence of external construct validity by examining the strength of

correlation between measurements of three specific components of social emotional ability as

differentiated and provided by the SEAI instrument, and measurements provided by three

reliable and valid external scales that measured the same or similar concepts (Barry, Chaney,

Stellefson, & Chaney 2011). Data for correlation testing was collected at the same time, from

the same sample, employing the same metrics; as previously discussed in the methodology

chapter these external scales had been inserted at the end of the SEAI.

The difficulties in emotion regulation scale (DERS) (Gratz & Roemer, 2004) was a

widely used, reliable and valid instrument that contained two sub-scales that employed identical

metrics to the SEAI, and measured the same or very similar latent concepts that are also

measured by the SEAI instrument, emotional clarity and emotional regulation.

The third external scale was the satisfaction with life scale (SWLS) (Diener, et al., 1985).

The SWLS was a widely used, reliable and valid instrument that measured life satisfaction, and

employed metrics identical to those found in the SEAD instrument. As previously discussed in

the methodology chapter, the SWLS was also inserted at the end of the SEAI.

While the SWLS scale was not directly related to social emotional ability, it provided

measurement of life satisfaction (Diener, et al., 1985). The Social Emotional Ability

Development model articulated that higher levels of social emotional ability resulted in more

positive social engagement experiences, and social engagement was a predictor of well-being,

life satisfaction, and happiness (Harris & Anderson, 2015). It would stand to reason that if the

SEAD instrument provided reliable, valid measurement of social emotional ability, then a

positive relationship must have existed between the SEAD Score and the SWLS Score.

55

In order to assess external construct validity, relationships between composite scores for

social emotional ability, emotional clarity, and emotional regulation, and scores for the

respective external scales were examined using Pearson’s Product Moment correlation testing.

Data met assumptions for Pearson’s correlation testing. The procedure compared two

continuous variables that represented two paired observations. Examination of scatterplots

revealed elliptically shaped patterns, which indicated that a linear relationship existed between

the variables. No outliers were identified in the data and normality testing ensured normal

distribution (Kang & Harring, 2012). According to Wuensch (2012), a correlation of greater

than .2 (p = <.05) was evidence of a statistically significant relationship, and this correlation was

therefore used to determine significance of Pearson’s correlations.

Ethical Considerations

Threats to Human Subjects

All participants were provided access to a letter of information providing a brief

explanation of the purpose of the study. There were few, if any, possible perceived threats to the

human subjects involved in this study. Participants were advised that there were few anticipated

risks involved in this study, and researchers intended to keep all names and research locations

confidential. Participants were further informed that it was the intention of the researcher to share

study findings at professional education conferences and in research articles and literature, but

participant responses were not identifiable.

Confidentiality

The researcher was bound by the confidentiality of the University of Florida and all other

local, state and federal laws applicable to this study. No personally subject-identifiable data was

collected.

56

Protection of Privacy

Clearance for this study was obtained from the UF Institutional Review Board 02.

Participants were primarily recruited by oral request in a classroom setting. Some snowballing

and convenience sampling may have occurred.

57

Table 3-1. Descriptive statistics of the sample: Gender

Frequency Percent Valid

Percent

Cumulative

Percent

Valid

Female 191 85.7 85.7 85.7

Male 32 14.3 14.3 100.0

Total 223 100.0 100.0

Table 3-2. Descriptive statistics of the sample: Age

Frequency Percent Valid

Percent

Cumulative

Percent

Valid

19 22 9.9 9.9 9.9

20 52 23.3 23.3 33.2

21 81 36.3 36.3 69.5

22 45 20.2 20.2 89.7

23 11 4.9 4.9 94.6

24 - 32 12 5.4 5.4 100.0

Total 223 100.0 100.0

Table 3-3. Descriptive statistics of the sample: Race

Frequency Percent Valid

Percent

Cumulative

Percent

Valid

White 138 61.9 62.2 62.2

Black or African American 54 24.2 24.3 86.5

Asian 13 5.8 5.9 92.3

American Indian or Alaskan

Native 1 .4 .5 92.8

Pacific Islander 1 .4 .5 93.2

Other: 15 6.7 6.8 100.0

Total 222 99.6 100.0

Declined to answer 1 .4

Total 223 100.0

Table 3-4. Descriptive statistics of the sample: Ethnicity

Would you describe your ethnicity as Hispanic,

Latino, or of Spanish origin? Frequency Percent

Valid

Percent

Cumulative

Percent

Valid

Yes 46 20.6 20.6 20.6

No 177 79.4 79.4 100.0

Total 223 100.0 100.0

58

Table 3-5. Descriptive statistics of the sample: College year

Frequency Percent Valid

Percent

Cumulative

Percent

Valid

Freshman 1 .4 .4 .4

Sophomore 27 12.1 12.1 12.6

Junior 81 36.3 36.3 48.9

Senior 114 51.1 51.1 100.0

Total 223 100.0 100.0

Table 3-6. Descriptive statistics of the sample: Total family income

Frequency Percent Valid

Percent

Cumulative

Percent

Valid

Less than $50,000 78 35.0 35.3 35.3

$50,000 - $69,999 26 11.7 11.8 47.1

$70,000 - $79,999 24 10.8 10.9 57.9

$80,000 - $99.999 23 10.3 10.4 68.3

$100,000 - $149,999 39 17.5 17.6 86.0

$150,000 and above 31 13.9 14.0 100.0

Total 221 99.1 100.0

Missing (Declined to answer) 2 .9

Total 223 100.0

59

Table 3-7. Principal component analysis: Item analysis

Component

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

EM2 .833

EMP3 .806

SYM2 .794

SYM1 .742

SYM3 .638

EMP1 .617

RES1 .919

RES2 .866

INT3 .809

INT2 .664

INT1 .609

INT4 .446

UE1 .863

ID1 .856 -.431

ID3 .786

ID4 .657

UE2 .615

UE3 .502

REG2 .836

REG3 .821

RES4 .713

REG4 .687

RES3 .666

REG1

AE1 .890

AE4 .742

AE2 .683

AE3 .520

ID2 .538 -.753

EMP4 .451 .597

AE_4 .888

SYM5 .960

SYM4 .477

Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis.

Rotation Method: Promax with Kaiser Normalization.

60

CHAPTER 4

RESULTS OF DATA ANALYSIS

Overview

Recall that the objective of data analysis for this study was to provide quantitative results

of data analysis in order to assess the reliability and validity of the Social Emotional Ability

Inventory (Harris & Anderson, 2015). To review, data were collected using the forty items

remaining after expert panel review, cognitive review, and pilot test. One index and two sub

indices were inserted into the instrument to provide the ability to assess external construct

validity and congruency with the existing literature. As the item analysis resulted in the removal

of twelve items from the instrument, data which had been provided by these items were removed

from the data set prior to analysis.

Results were reported in the same sequence in which the data were analyzed. First, the

results of reliability testing were reported, which included Cronbach’s alpha testing, split-sample

reliability testing, and split form consistency testing. Next, the results of internal construct

validity, which included factor analyses, were reported. Finally, the results of external construct

validity testing were reported, which included comparisons of correlations between external

scales and concepts within the SEAD that measured the same of very similar concepts.

Reliability Results

Cronbach’s alpha

In order to assess internal reliability, Cronbach’s Alpha testing was performed

individually on the three constructs of the SEAD: Table 4-1 showed that the variable that

measured the first construct, emotional clarity, contained eleven items and returned a Cronbach’s

Alpha of .80, a mean inter-item correlation of .27 (Table 4-2), and ten of the eleven items had

item-total correlations greater than .3 (Table 4-2); item ID2 had an item-total correlation of .26,

61

but was not removed because it was cognitively determined to have a theoretically sound basis

for inclusion. According to the thresholds referenced above and suggested by Barry, Chaney,

Stellefson, and Chaney (2011), the construct, emotional clarity, displayed the statistical

characteristics of “very good” internal reliability.

Table 4-4 showed that the variable that measured the second construct, emotional

integration, contained eleven items and returned a Cronbach’s Alpha of .78, a mean inter-item

correlation of .25 (Table 4-5), and all eleven items returned item total correlations greater than .3

(Table 4-5). According to the thresholds referenced above and suggested by Barry, Chaney,

Stellefson, and Chaney (2011), the construct, emotional integration, displayed the statistical

characteristics of “respectable” internal reliability.

Table 4-7 showed that the variable that measured the third construct, social emotional

integration, contained six items and returned a Cronbach’s Alpha of .88, a mean inter-item

correlation of .54 (Table 4-8), and all six items had item total correlations greater than .3 (Table

4-9). According to the thresholds referenced above and suggested by Barry, Chaney, Stellefson,

and Chaney (2011), the construct, social emotional integration, displayed the statistical

characteristics of “very good” internal reliability.

Split sample reliability testing

In order to assess internal reliability, the sample was split in half by odd-even case, and

Pearson’s product-moment correlations were run on Social Emotional Ability Scores and also on

composite scores for all three constructs for both halves. Preliminary analyses showed that

necessary assumptions had been met: tested relationships proved linear and normally distributed,

as assessed by Shapiro-Wilk's test (p > .05). Outliers had been previously removed. Table 4-10

showed a statistically significant, very strong positive correlation between the split sample

groups for the Social Emotional Ability Score, r(109) = 1.00, p < .001. Table 4-11 showed a

62

statistically significant, very strong positive correlation between the split sample groups for the

construct, emotional clarity, r(109) = .65, p < .001. Table 4-12 showed a statistically

significant, very strong positive correlation between the split sample groups for the construct,

emotional integration, r(109) = .70, p < .001. Table 4-13 showed a statistically significant,

moderately strong positive correlation between the split sample groups for the construct,

emotional clarity, r(109) = .33, p < .001.

According to the thresholds suggested by Kang and Harring (2012) for Pearson’s

correlations, and guidance provided by Swisher (2016) as referenced above, the Social

Emotional Ability Inventory displayed the statistical characteristics of “very good” internal

reliability.

Split form consistency testing

In order to assess internal consistency reliability, the Social Emotional Ability Inventory

instrument was split along odd-even items, by construct. Table 4-14 showed that Cronbach’s

alpha for the two form halves was .77 and .70, respectively. According to the thresholds

referenced above and suggested by Barry, Chaney, Stellefson, and Chaney (2011), these alphas

indicated good internal consistency reliability. Table 14-4 also showed a very strong positive

Pearson’s correlation between the two form halves, r = .85, and the Spearman-Brown split-half

reliability coefficient was .92. According to Garson (2009), these results indicated very good

internal consistency reliability.

Validity Results

Internal construct validity testing

Internal construct validity was tested using EFA with PCA methodology and CFA with

PAF methodology, which was discussed in detail in the methodology chapter.

63

Exploratory factor analysis: The primary objective of the exploratory factor analysis

was to explore the structure of the instrument in relation to the theoretical model it was

developed to measure, and to provide quantitative evidence of internal construct validity (Barry,

et al., 2011). Under ideal circumstances, the eight dimensions that the instrument was intended

to measure would have loaded discretely on the eight dimensions defined by the SEAD.

However, the EFA was expected to extract less than eight principal components, differentiated

into the three constructs of the SEAD. Past research has shown that differentiating sympathy and

empathy had been very difficult using self-reported instruments (Vossen, Piotrowski, &

Valkenburg, 2015), and there was some concern that the dimension, interpret emotions, and the

dimension, respond to emotions, might present difficulties differentiating as well, because the

concepts are very closely related and not specifically addressed in the literature. However, it was

expected that these items would converge onto the principal components of their intended

constructs, without cross loading onto other constructs.

An exploratory factor analysis for the full sample (N = 223) was performed on the

twenty-eight items of the revised Social Emotional Ability Inventory instrument using principal

components analysis extraction. Prior to analysis, suitability of data for factor analysis was

assessed. Missing values were treated pairwise. Table 4-15 showed that the Kaiser–Meyer–

Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy was .82, which exceeded the minimum

recommended value of .6 (Kaiser, 1970, 1974). A KMO value of .82 is classified as

“meritorious” (Kaiser, 1974). Table 4-15 also showed that Bartlett’s test of Sphericity was

significant at p < .001. These results, in conjunction with a determinant of the matrix not equal

to zero, indicated that data were appropriate for principal components factor analysis (Agarwal,

2011).

64

Because factors underlying the latent constructs of the SEAD were expected to be related,

the communalities correlation matrix was subjected to a Promax (k=6) rotation to improve

interpretability (Osborne & Costello, 2005). The goodness of fit test was significant (p < .001).

Figure 4-1 showed that six components appeared before the “bend” in the scree plot and were

considered for retention. Table 4-16 showed the same six components emerged with eigenvalues

exceeding 1.0. Component number six was considered for removal for a contribution to

accumulated variance of less than 5%, but upon inspection it was determined that this component

had an eigenvalue of 1.03, and converged to measure only one dimension and therefore was

retained. These six components explained between 3.9% and 22.6% of the variance, with an

accumulated variance explained of 60.5%. According to Suhr (2005), components extracted

using PCA are generally expected to be unrelated. Table 4-17 showed that five of the six

components were unrelated.

Table 4-18 revealed a pattern matrix with a simple component structure, and all items

loaded on only one of the six components. There were no items that loaded with values below

.52, and no items cross-loaded onto other components with loadings of .4 or greater by a value of

more than .2. These results indicated that the component matrix was suitable for interpretation

for a degree of adherence to convergent and discriminant validity (Agarwal, 2011). Cognitive

inspection of Table 4-18 revealed that these components were highly interpretable and reflected

the multidimensional definition of social emotional ability development on which the scale was

based. Cognitive inspection of Table 4-18 also revealed that the dimensions, sympathetic

response and empathetic response, loaded onto the same principal component, as did the

dimensions, interpret emotions and respond to emotions.

Correspondingly, six components were extracted. Six items loaded onto the first

65

component, and addressed sympathetic response and empathetic response, which are dimensions

of the construct, social emotional integration, as defined in the theoretically framed model. The

six items that loaded onto the second component addressed emotional regulation, a dimension of

the construct, emotional integration. The five items that loaded onto the third component

addressed the ability to interpret and respond to emotions, which are also dimensions of the

construct, emotional integration. The four items that loaded onto the fourth component

addressed the ability to accept emotions, which is a dimension of emotional clarity. The four

items that loaded onto the fifth component addressed the ability to identify and understand

emotions, which are dimensions of emotional clarity. And the three items that loaded onto the

sixth component addressed the ability to identify emotions, which is a dimension of the

construct, emotional clarity.

In summary, six components were extracted, and discretely represented the three latent

constructs proposed by the SEAD. Six items in component one measured the two dimensions of

the theoretical construct social emotional integration. Eleven items in components two and three

measured the three dimensions of the theoretical construct emotional integration. And the eleven

items in components four, five, and six measured the three dimensions of emotional clarity.

These loadings indicate high levels of in-construct convergent validity, and between-construct

discriminant validity for all items (Barry, Chaney, Stellefson, & Chaney, 2011). See Figure 4-2

for a comparison of the theoretical factor structure proposed by the SEAD and the principal

components that extracted as a result of the EFA.

Confirmatory factor analysis: It was expected that the six components of the SEAI

extracted in the EFA would correspond with the factor structure of the theoretical constructs as

measured by the SEAI, and as proposed by the SEAD model. Because of the inherent

66

differences between principal component analysis and principal axis factoring described by

Beavers et al. (2013) and discussed in detail in the methodology chapter, it was possible that

correlation coefficients between the two methodologies could differ. Furthermore, a PCA is not

intended to identify the factor structure of latent constructs (Beavers, et al., 2013). Accordingly,

a confirmatory factor analysis was deemed advisable to confirm that these differences would not

result in significant discrepancies between the principal components extracted, and the factors

structure identified by the CFA. Correspondingly, the primary objective of the confirmatory

factor analysis was to confirm that the structure of the six underlying components that had been

extracted by the EFA were the same underlying factors of the three latent constructs as

differentiated in the theoretical model, and to identify and confirm the convergent and divergent

characteristics of both the instrument and the theoretical model as presented in the CFA pattern

matrix. A confirmatory factor analysis for the full sample (N = 223) was performed on the

twenty-eight items of the revised Social Emotional Ability Inventory instrument using principal

axis factoring. Data had been previously determined suitable for factor analysis (Table 4-15).

Because it was expected that factors underlying the latent constructs of SEAD would be related,

the communalities correlation matrix was subjected to a Promax (k=6) rotation to improve

interpretability (Osborne & Costello, 2005). Table 4-19 revealed that only five factors were

suitable for retention. The sixth factor identified in the CFA contained only one loading, and

therefore was not retained (Table 4-20). The five factors retained were the same as five of the

six principal components extracted in the EFA, and explained an accumulated 56.6% of the total

variance. Each factor retained contributed between 5.3% and 22.6% of the variance, which is

slightly less robust than the variance reported by the EFA, which was expected (Beavers et al.,

2013). According to Suhr (2005) factors extracted using principal axis factoring should

67

generally be related. Table 4-20 showed that the five components were related.

Table 4-21 revealed a pattern matrix with a relatively simple factor structure, with all

items loading on only one of the five retained factors. There were no items that loaded with

values below .42, and no items cross-loaded onto other factors with loadings of .4 or greater by a

value of less than .2. Cognitive inspection of the pattern matrix revealed that all five factors

were identified as being appropriate to the structural model. These results indicated that the

factor matrix was suitable for assessment for factor structure (Agarwal, 2011).

All five factors were highly interpretable and identified and confirmed both the

theoretically defined Social Emotional Ability Development model (Harris & Anderson, 2015),

the three latent constructs the SEAI intended to measure, and five of the six components

extracted by the EFA. The first factor addressed empathy and sympathy, which are dimensions

of the construct, social emotional integration. The second and third factors addressed emotional

regulation, emotional response, and emotional interpretation, which are dimensions of the SEAD

construct, emotional integration. The fourth and fifth factors addressed emotional acceptance,

emotional understanding, and emotional identification, which are dimensions of the SEAD

construct, emotional clarity.

As expected, the factor loadings identified in the confirmatory factor analysis were

somewhat lower than those reported by PCA for the exploratory factor analysis (Beavers et al.,

2013). Consequently, the pattern matrix in the confirmatory factor analysis differed somewhat

from the pattern matrix in the exploratory factor analysis as far as differentiating the dimensions

and the respective loadings on those dimensions were concerned. However, no items loaded out

of construct, and the confirmatory factor analysis confirmed that the dimensions measuring

constructs represented by items within the principal components analysis and the dimensions

68

measuring the underlying factors of the latent constructs of the SEAD were exactly aligned.

Furthermore, the high in-factor loading matrices provided by these analyses, when taken

as a whole, explained the majority of item responses (Williams et al., 2012). The structure of

principal components extracted by the exploratory factor analysis, and the structure of the factors

identified in the confirmatory factor analyses, even though slightly different, directly reflected

the theoretically defined dimension and construct structure of the Social Emotional Ability

Inventory instrument, as well as the theoretical Social Emotional Ability Development model.

See Figure 4-2 for a comparison of the theoretical factor structure proposed by the SEAD, the

principal components extracted by the the EFA, and the factor structure confirmed by the CFA.

External construct validity testing

Pearson’s product-moment correlation testing was employed to compare relationships

between measurement of dimensions of social emotional ability as differentiated by the SEAD

and external scales inserted at the end of the instrument, for the purpose of providing evidence of

external validity. Correlation effect size were interpreted in the manner suggested in Cohen

(1992), to wit: Results > .10 represent small correlations; results > .30 represent medium

correlations; results > .50 represent large correlations.

Emotional clarity correlation: Emotional clarity was a theoretical construct of the

SEAD, as measured by the Social Emotional Ability Inventory. Emotional clarity was also a

concept that had been measured by other valid and reliable instruments. The Difficulty in

Emotional Regulation Scale (DERS), was a well-known, valid and reliable instrument (Gratz &

Roemer, 2004). The DERS contained a sub-scale that measured emotional clarity, and was

inserted at the end of the SEAI for the purpose of providing evidence of external validity.

Pearson correlations were tested between emotional clarity scores provided by SEAI for

the SEAD construct, and scores provided for emotional clarity from the DERS (Gratz & Roemer,

69

2004). Preliminary analyses showed that necessary assumptions had been met; all the

relationships proved to be linear and normally distributed, as assessed by Shapiro-Wilk's test (p >

.05). Outliers had been previously removed. Table 4-22 revealed that there was a statistically

significant, strong positive correlation (r = .73, p < .001) between the theoretical construct,

emotional clarity, and the external concept, emotional clarity.

Emotional regulation correlation: Emotional regulation was one of the dimensions of

the SEAD measured by the SEAI. Emotional regulation was also a concept that had been

measured by other valid and reliable instruments. The Difficulty in Emotional Regulation Scale

(DERS), was a well-known, valid and reliable instrument (Gratz & Roemer, 2004). The DERS

contained a sub-scale that measured emotional regulation, and was inserted at the end of the

SEAI for the purpose of providing evidence of external validity (Gratz & Roemer, 2004).

Pearson correlations were tested between emotional regulation scores provided by the

SEAI to measure the SEAD dimension, emotional regulation, and scores measuring emotional

regulation from the DERS (Gratz & Roemer, 2004). Preliminary analyses showed that necessary

assumptions had been met; all the relationships proved to be linear and normally distributed, as

assessed by Shapiro-Wilk's test (p > .05). Outliers had been previously removed. Table 4-23

revealed a statistically significant, strong positive correlation (r = .70, p < .001) between the

dimension emotional regulation and the external concept, emotional regulation.

Social emotional ability correlation: Social emotional ability was a latent concept that

represented social emotional ability through an aggregated score of all of the dimensions of the

SEAD model, as measured by the SEAI (Harris & Anderson, 2015). The Social Emotional

Ability Development model articulated that higher levels of social emotional ability resulted in

more positive social engagement experiences, and social engagement, among other things, was a

70

predictor of life satisfaction (Harris & Anderson, 2015). For purposes of providing evidence of

external validity, the Satisfaction With Life Scale, a widely known, valid and reliable instrument

(SWLS) for measuring life satisfaction, was inserted into the SEAD (Diener, et al., 1985).

Pearson correlations were tested between SEAD scores and SWLS scores (Diener, et al.,

1985)). Preliminary analyses showed that necessary assumptions had been met: all the

relationships proved to be linear and normally distributed, as assessed by Shapiro-Wilk's test (p >

.05). Outliers had been previously removed. Table 4-24 showed a statistically significant,

moderate positive correlation (r = .33, p < .001) between Social Emotional Ability Scores and

satisfaction with life scores.

71

Table 4-1. Cronbach’s alpha reliability statistics: Emotional clarity

Cronbach's Alpha Cronbach's Alpha Based

on Standardized Items N of Items

.795 .802 11

Table 4-2. Cronbach’s alpha summary item statistics: Emotional clarity

Mean Minimum Maximum Range Maximum /

Minimum Variance

N of

Items

Item Means 3.836 3.188 4.511 1.323 1.415 .186 11

Inter-Item

Correlations .270 .000 .581 .580 12995.842 .019 11

Table 4-3. Cronbach’s alpha item-total statistics: Emotional clarity

Scale Mean if Item

Deleted

Scale Variance if

Item Deleted

Corrected Item-

Total Correlation

Squared Multiple

Correlation

Cronbach's Alpha if

Item Deleted

ID1 38.07 96.950 .503 .381 .774

ID2 38.29 100.343 .260 .231 .803

ID3 37.68 97.209 .535 .358 .772

ID4 38.13 98.685 .478 .412 .777

UE1 37.95 93.700 .589 .418 .765

UE2 39.00 97.536 .474 .284 .777

UE3 38.67 102.727 .334 .295 .790

AE1 38.83 97.869 .373 .386 .788

AE2 38.50 92.882 .547 .412 .768

AE3 38.81 91.994 .467 .395 .778

AE4 37.99 95.775 .491 .456 .775

Table 4-4. Cronbach’s alpha reliability statistics: Emotional integration

Cronbach's Alpha Cronbach's Alpha Based

on Standardized Items N of Items

.778 .783 11

Table 4-5. Cronbach’s alpha summary item statistics: Emotional integration

Mean Minimum Maximum Range Maximum / Minimum Variance N of Items

Item Means 4.234 3.659 4.924 1.265 1.346 .159 11

Inter-Item Correlations .247 .000 .656 .655 2624.076 .032 11

72

Table 4-6. Cronbach’s alpha item-total statistics: Emotional integration

Scale Mean if Item

Deleted

Scale Variance if

Item Deleted

Corrected Item-

Total Correlation

Squared Multiple

Correlation

Cronbach's Alpha if

Item Deleted

INT1 41.65 85.488 .392 .281 .766

INT2 42.44 84.500 .324 .334 .773

INT3 42.49 82.440 .437 .457 .761

RES1 42.61 81.130 .485 .581 .756

RES2 42.45 83.546 .421 .555 .763

RES3 42.04 77.944 .529 .436 .750

RES4 42.92 76.741 .446 .297 .760

REG1 41.69 82.928 .332 .188 .773

REG2 42.26 77.745 .480 .419 .755

REG3 42.58 75.650 .525 .459 .749

REG4 42.64 79.790 .392 .329 .767

Table 4-7. Cronbach’s alpha reliability statistics: Social emotional integration

Cronbach's Alpha Cronbach's Alpha Based

on Standardized Items N of Items

.875 .876 6

Table 4-8. Cronbach’s alpha summary item statistics: Social emotional integration

Mean Minimum Maximum Range Maximum / Minimum Variance N of Items

Item Means 4.760 4.251 5.184 .933 1.219 .109 6

Inter-Item Correlations .540 .437 .761 .324 1.742 .006 6

Table 4-9. Cronbach’s alpha item-total statistics: Social emotional integration

Scale Mean if Item

Deleted

Scale Variance if

Item Deleted

Corrected Item-

Total Correlation

Squared Multiple

Correlation

Cronbach's Alpha

if Item Deleted

SYM1 23.70 27.119 .629 .432 .861

SYM2 23.38 27.164 .703 .526 .851

SYM3 24.01 24.230 .756 .651 .839

EMP1 24.31 24.431 .707 .608 .849

EM2 23.82 26.427 .684 .497 .852

EMP3 23.58 27.443 .605 .402 .865

73

Table 4-10. Split sample correlations: Emotional clarity

First Half Second Half

First Half Pearson Correlation 1 .648**

Sig. (2-tailed) .000

N 111 111

Second Half Pearson Correlation .648** 1

Sig. (2-tailed) .000

N 111 111 **. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

Table 4-11. Split sample correlations: Emotional integration

First Half Second Half

First Half Pearson Correlation 1 .701**

Sig. (2-tailed) .000

N 111 111

Second Half Pearson Correlation .701** 1

Sig. (2-tailed) .000

N 111 111 **. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

Table 4-12. Split sample correlations: Social emotional integration

First Half Second Half

First Half Pearson Correlation 1 .331**

Sig. (2-tailed) .000

N 111 111

Second Half Pearson Correlation .331** 1

Sig. (2-tailed) .000

N 111 111 **. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

Table 4-13. Split sample correlations: Social emotional ability score

First Half Second Half

First Half Pearson Correlation 1 1.000**

Sig. (2-tailed) .000

N 111 111

Second Half Pearson Correlation 1.000** 1

Sig. (2-tailed) .000

N 111 111 **. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

74

Table 4-14. Split form internal consistency: Social emotional ability inventory

Cronbach's Alpha Part 1 Value .767

N of Items 14a

Part 2 Value .696

N of Items 14b

Total N of Items 28

Correlation Between Forms .848

Spearman-Brown Coefficient Equal Length .918

Unequal Length .918

Guttman Split-Half Coefficient .917

a. The items are: ID1, ID3, UE1, UE3, AE2, AE4, INT2, RES1, RES3, REG1, REG3, SYM1, SYM3, EMP2.

b. The items are: ID2, ID4, UE2, AE1, AE3, INT1, INT3, RES2, RES4, REG2, REG4, SYM2, EMP1, EMP3.

Table 4-15. Exploratory factor analysis: KMO and Bartlett's Test

Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy. .820

Bartlett's Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 2578.155

df 378

Sig. .000

Table 4-16. Exploratory factor analysis: Total variance explained

Component Initial Eigenvalues

Rotation

Sums of Squared Loadingsa

Total % of Variance Cumulative % Total

1 6.319 22.566 22.566 4.619

2 3.484 12.443 35.010 3.588

3 2.574 9.191 44.201 4.184

4 1.978 7.064 51.265 3.390

5 1.493 5.332 56.597 3.411

6 1.078 3.851 60.448 2.436 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis.

a. When components are correlated, sums of squared loadings cannot be added to obtain a total variance.

Table 4-17. Exploratory factor analysis: Component correlations

Component 1 2 3 4 5 6

1 1.000

2 .114 1.000

3 .393 .160 1.000

4 .132 .248 .189 1.000

5 .131 .171 .303 .384 1.000

6 .005 .289 .145 .156 .130 1.000 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis.

Rotation Method: Promax with Kaiser Normalization.

75

Table 4-18. Exploratory factor analysis: Pattern matrix

Component

1 2 3 4 5 6

SYM3 .834

EMP2 .792

EMP1 .769

EMP3 .759

SYM2 .735

SYM1 .709

REG2 .829

REG3 .796

RES3 .648

REG4 .624

RES4 .605

REG1 .580

RES1 .818

RES2 .794

INT3 .737

INT2 .649

INT1 .634

AE1 .910

AE4 .804

AE2 .690

AE3 .610

ID4 .716

UE3 .696

UE1 .594

UE2 .518

ID1 .663

ID3 .629

ID2 .625 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis.

Rotation Method: Promax with Kaiser Normalization. a. Rotation converged in 6 iterations.

76

Table 4-19. Confirmatory factor analysis: Total variance explained

Factor Initial Eigenvalues Rotation Sums of Squared Loadingsa

Total % of Variance Cumulative % Total

1 6.319 22.566 22.566 4.153

2 3.484 12.443 35.010 4.296

3 2.574 9.191 44.201 3.288

4 1.978 7.064 51.265 3.829

5 1.493 5.332 56.597 2.760 Extraction Method: Principal Axis Factoring.

a. When factors are correlated, sums of squared loadings cannot be added to obtain a total variance.

Table 4-20. Confirmatory factor analysis: Factor correlations

Factor 1 2 3 4 5

1 1.000

2 .424 1.000

3 .192 .256 1.000

4 .116 .508 .365 1.000

5 .119 .220 .298 .384 1.000 Extraction Method: Principal Axis Factoring.

Rotation Method: Promax with Kaiser Normalization.

77

Table 4-21. Confirmatory factor analysis: Pattern matrix

Factor

1 2 3 4 5 6 (not retained)

SYM3 .779

EMP2 .750

SYM2 .712

EMP3 .697

EMP1 .691

SYM1 .640

RES1 .878

RES2 .844

INT3 .727

INT2 .596

INT1 .514

REG2 .788

REG3 .771

RES3 .571

RES4 .540

REG4 .511

REG1 .443

UE1 .767

ID1 .760

ID3 .705

ID4 .611

UE3 .506

UE2 .448

AE1 .766

AE4 .752

AE2 .543

AE3 .498

ID2 .447 -.499 Extraction Method: Principal Axis Factoring.

Rotation Method: Promax with Kaiser Normalization.

a. Rotation converged in 6 iterations.

78

Table 4-22. Pearson’s correlations, external construct validity testing: Emotional clarity

Emotional Clarity Clarity Compare

Emotional Clarity Pearson Correlation 1 .732**

Sig. (2-tailed) .000

N 223 223

Clarity Compare Pearson Correlation .732** 1

Sig. (2-tailed) .000

N 223 223 **. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

Table 4-23. Pearson’s correlations, external construct validity testing: Emotional regulation

Emotional Regulation Regulate Compare

Emotional Regulation Pearson Correlation 1 .704**

Sig. (2-tailed) .000

N 223 223

Regulate Compare Pearson Correlation .704** 1

Sig. (2-tailed) .000

N 223 223 **. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

Table 4-24. Pearson’s correlations, external construct validity testing: Social emotional ability level

Social

Emotional Ability Life Satisfaction

SEAD Pearson Correlation 1 .332**

Sig. (2-tailed) .000

N 223 223

Life Satisfaction Pearson Correlation .332** 1

Sig. (2-tailed) .000

N 223 223 **. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

79

Figure 4-1. Scree Plot of factor analyses component eigenvalues

80

Figure 4-2. Comparison of the factor structure of the SEAD, the principal components extracted

in the EFA, and the factor structure retained by the CFA

81

CHAPTER 5

DISCUSSION

Overview

The purpose of this study was to expand the body of knowledge regarding developmental

processes of social emotional ability among individuals by providing justification for constructs

of the new theoretical model, the Hierarchy of Social Emotional Ability Development (Harris &

Anderson, 2015). This study was driven by the following research questions: 1). What are the

justifiable constructs of social emotional ability? 2). How can the constructs of the SEAD be

quantified in a valid and reliable survey instrument?

This current study was guided by the theoretical framework of Vygotsky’s Sociocultural

Theory of Development, and included a review of the literature that justified the validity of the

SEAD constructs, and a synthesis between constructs of the Sociocultural Theory of

Development and constructs of the SEAD theoretical model. This study also guided the

development a multi-item measurement instrument that provided reliable, valid discriminant

quantification of the constructs of the SEAD, and a Social Emotional Ability Score for

individuals.

Summary of the Findings

Research Question 1

The summary constructs of the SEAD were justified from existing research and

theoretical linkages in the body of knowledge, and the hierarchal progression of the SEAD was

shown to be logical. Empirical evidence provided by data collected utilizing the Social

Emotional Ability Inventory instrument also supported this hierarchal progression, and a

confirmatory factor analysis provided empirical evidence of the integrity of the factor structure

in relation to the constructs asserted by the SEAD.

82

Research Question 2

The constructs of the SEAD were shown to be valid, as quantified by the Social

Emotional Ability Inventory. Groundwork was laid for this in Chapter Three by describing

methodologies employed in the construction of the instrument, and the methodologies employed

in the collection and analysis of the data. Chapter Four reported the results of statistical analyses

of the data, which demonstrated the reliability and validity of the instrument, and its ability to

quantify the constructs of Social Emotional Ability as asserted by the SEAD.

Reliability

Internal reliability

Testing showed that the Social Emotional Ability Inventory instrument has strong

internal reliability. Cronbach’s alpha testing provided evidence of strong internal reliability,

with alpha values and mean inter-item correlations that were well above the conservative

acceptable levels set by the researcher for this study. Inter-item correlations also reflected strong

internal reliability and were aligned with the level of complexity of the constructs as suggested

by Clark and Watson (1995). Split-sample correlations also provided evidence of strong internal

reliability. The Social Emotional Ability Score and scores from all three constructs of the SEAD

showed statistically significant, strong positive correlations between the sample halves. Split-

form testing also provided strong evidence of internal consistency, which is fundamental to

internal reliability. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for both halves of the split forms were within

acceptable ranges, and there was also a significant, very strong positive correlation between the

two halves.

External reliability

External reliability was not tested as multiple sampling is required to provide evidence of

external reliability, and this was an initial exploratory cross sectional study.

83

Validity

According to Clark and Watson (1995), construct validity is one of the most important

aspects of research and as such is a critical component of rigor. Construct validity cannot be

measured directly, but is determined by evidence of theoretical validity and the presence of

internal and external convergent and discriminant validity. Evidence supporting theoretical

validity was provided from the literature.

Internal validity

Internal convergent and discriminant testing showed that the Social Emotional Ability

Inventory has strong internal convergent and discriminant validity and, therefore, strong internal

construct validity. Exploratory factor analysis revealed a simple six-component pattern matrix

solution for measuring the three constructs of the SEAD where none of the items cross-loaded

out of factor. This provided evidence of discriminant validity, and all items loaded onto only one

of the six components, which provided evidence of convergent validity. While several

components cross-loaded between dimensions, all did so within construct. The exploratory

factor analysis also supported the progressive, linear nature of the SEAD with each component

showing conformance with the theoretical model. Additionally, the confirmatory factor analysis

validated the theoretical construct structure of the SEAD model.

External validity

External convergent validity testing demonstrated strong external convergent validity

through significant positive strong correlations between concepts of the SEAD and the same or

very similar concepts measured by subscales adopted from the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation

Scale (Gratz & Roemer, 2004). A significant positive moderately strong correlation was also

shown to exist between Social Emotional Ability Scores provided by the SEAI and scores

provided by the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985).

84

External discriminant validity was not tested, as no scales measuring concepts opposite to those

proposed by the SEAD were inserted into the instrument.

Summary

Recall that the purpose of this study was to expand the body of knowledge regarding

developmental processes of social emotional ability by providing justification for the constructs

of the SEAD from the existing literature, and quantifying those constructs through the

development of a valid, reliable instrument with the ability to provide incremental measurements

for constructs of the SEAD model and a composite Social Emotional Ability Score for

individuals. According to the reported findings, these study objectives have been accomplished.

The research questions, which were designed to accomplish these goals, have also been

answered. The present study has shown that the constructs of the SEAD are justified by

providing supportive research from the literature and by developing linkages between existing

theoretical frameworks and the theoretical constructs of the SEAD model. Statistical analysis of

the data provided empirical justification for the quantification of social emotional ability and

constructs of the SEAD through measurement of the eight dimensions, as specified by the

theoretical model. Results demonstrated that the instrument has strong internal reliability and

construct validity, and measures the intended latent constructs. Confirmatory factor analysis

demonstrated that the factor structure exactly mirrors the theoretical construct structure of the

SEAD model. External convergent and congruent validity were demonstrated by significant

strong positive correlations with existing instruments. And the Social Emotional Ability Score

generated by the instrument showed a significant positive correlation with the Satisfaction with

Life Score, as predicted by the SEAD model.

85

Important Implications

Maslow’s seminal work, Hierarchy of Needs (1954) proposes that social engagement is a

basic human need (Huitt, 2007), and social engagement is an important predictor of well-being,

life satisfaction, and happiness (Baumeister, Vohs, Aaker, & Garbinsky 2013; Cialdini &

Patrick, 2009; Lambert et al., 2010).

This study holds important and far-reaching implications for individuals, educators,

practitioners, and researchers because it addresses the processes at play in development of an

individual’s ability to participate in social engagement. It is important to keep in mind that the

SEAD model provides a developmental roadmap that addresses eight social emotional abilities,

which are teachable within the domains of individuals, couple relationships, parenting,

education, and professional remediation, among others. Also, the Social Emotional Ability

Inventory provides scores for each of the abilities identified in the SEAD model; as a result, the

SEAI might therefore be used as a diagnostic instrument to provide guidance within these

domains.

Additionally, results of the Flesch Kincaid Grade Index (Wilson, Rosenberg, & Hyatt,

1997) indicate that comprehension of the Social Emotional Ability Inventory requires an 8th

grade reading level, which suggests that subject to further development, the SEAI might be

deployed among a wide range of populations.

For Individuals

Individuals might use results from the Social Emotional Ability Inventory to guide efforts

to improve their social emotional ability, and thereby improve their social engagement

experiences and level of life satisfaction. Parents might use guidance provided by the Social

Emotional Ability Development model to help provide direction for childrearing, to teach

emotional regulation, sympathy, and empathy skills, and to improve inter-familial interaction.

86

For Educators

Educators might use guidance provided by the Social Emotional Ability Development

model for developing or expanding curricula important to student development, personal and

professional development, and to address the improvement of all manner of relationships,

including curricula directed toward family relationships, couple relationships, and interactions in

the workplace.

For Practitioners

For practitioners, the eight dimensions of social emotional ability development could be

used as a diagnostic tool to increase awareness and understanding of their influences on contexts,

such as family backgrounds and childhood experiences, adaptive or maladaptive individual traits,

and learned interactional processes that promote or inhibit healthy social interaction. Each

dimension could also be incorporated into therapeutic practices such as cognitive-behavioral and

emotion-focused therapy to assist clients with better processing of their emotions and increased

empowerment toward healthy decision-making.

For Researchers

Researchers might use the model and the instrument to more completely investigate and

explain the relationship between social interaction and life satisfaction, and to develop

programming that may mediate specific impacts of low socialization. Further research is

necessary to confirm and expand the findings of this study, and to provide evidence of external

reliability and external discriminant validity. Further studies are also necessary to refine the

within-construct discriminant capability of the instrument in order to improve accuracy and to

improve its utility as a diagnostic tool. This would greatly simplify use of the instrument by

laypersons, educators, and researchers if all eight dimensions of the SEAD were discriminately

measured by the same number of items with the potential to yield comparable scores without

87

employing complex weighting formulae. And, following refinement of the instrument, it would

provide further support for the validity of the constructs of the SEAD and evidence of

scaffolding as proposed by the theoretical framework of the model to examine relationships

between Emotional Clarity mean scores and Social Emotional Integration mean scores for the

bottom quartiles of the sample to confirm that higher levels of fundamental emotional abilities

are related to higher levels of social emotional integration.

Cautions and Limitations

It should be kept in mind that this was an initial exploratory study, and results were not

intended to be and are not generalizable. Sampling was not random, and the sample was biased

in ways likely to affect the study outcome; the sample was composed of a disproportionate ratio

of females (85.6%) to males (14.4%); Research suggests that females are generally more

emotionally complex than males, and also have a greater ability to recognize and interpret

emotions (Wied, Branje, & Meeus, 2007). The sample was also composed disproportionately of

young people. The mean age was 21, and the age range was 19 - 32, which would preclude male

participants with college aged children. Research suggests that fathers with college aged

students report significantly lower emotional abilities than their respective aged college students

regardless of gender, and the mother of those children (Guastello & Guastello, 2003).

These sample biases likely had a lowering effect on variance, which would likely result

in lower correlations—making results of this study somewhat lower than those likely to be

obtained from a more representative sample. Also, there is an absence of external discriminant

testing, as no measurements of concepts opposite to those proposed in the SEAD model were

taken. As this was an initial exploratory study, there is no evidence of external reliability, which

is normatively provided by replication.

88

APPENDIX

INSTRUMENT ITEMS

Table A-1. Social emotional ability inventory (Harris & Anderson, 2015)

Inventory removed by author.

Items are available upon request.

Contact:

Victor Harris

[email protected]

Table A-2. Emotional clarity: Difficulties in emotional regulation scale (Gratz & Roemer, 2004)

Concept Item Identifier Item

Emotional Clarity C-EC1 I have difficulty making sense out of my feelings.

C-EC2 I have no idea how I am feeling.

C-EC3 I am confused about how I feel.

C-EC4 I know exactly how I am feeling.

C-EC5 I am clear about my feelings.

Table A-3. Emotional regulation: Difficulties in emotional regulation scale (Gratz & Roemer, 2004)

Concept Item Identifier Item

Emotional

Regulation C-ER1 When I’m upset, I believe that I’ll end up feeling very depressed.

C-ER2 When I’m upset, I believe that I will remain that way for a long time.

C-ER3 When I’m upset, I believe that wallowing in it is all I can do.

C-ER4 When I’m upset, it takes me a long time to feel better.

C-ER5 When I’m upset, I believe that there is nothing I can do to make myself

feel better.

C-ER6 When I’m upset, I know that I can find a way to eventually feel better.

C-ER7 When I’m upset, my emotions feel overwhelming.

C-ER8 When I’m upset, I start to feel very bad about myself.

Table A-4. Satisfaction with life scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985)

Concept Item Identifier Item

Life Satisfaction C-LS1 In most ways my life is close to my ideal.

C-LS2 The conditions of my life are excellent.

C-LS3 I am satisfied with my life.

C-LS4 So far I have gotten the important things I want in life.

C-LS5 If I could live my life over, I would change almost nothing.

89

REFERENCES

Allemand, M., Steiger, A. E., & Fend, H. A. (2015; 2014). Empathy development in adolescence

predicts social competencies in adulthood Journal of Personality, 83(2), 229.

doi:10.1111/jopy.12098

Agarwal, N. K. (2011). Verifying survey items for construct validity: A two‐stage sorting

procedure for questionnaire design in information behavior research. Proceedings of the

American Society for Information Science and Technology, 48(1), 1-8.

Baker, R., Brick, J. M., Bates, N. A., Battaglia, M., Couper, M. P., Dever, J. A., ... &

Tourangeau, R. (2013). Summary report of the AAPOR task force on non-probability

sampling. Journal of Survey Statistics and Methodology, 1(2), 90-143.

Bar-On, R., & Parker, J. D. (2000). The handbook of emotional intelligence: Theory,

development, assessment, and application at home, school, and in the workplace. San

Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Barry, A. E., Chaney, E. H., Stellefson, M. L., & Chaney, J. D. (2011). So you want to develop a

survey: Practical recommendations for scale development. American Journal of Health

Studies, 26(2).

Bartlett, M. S. (1954). A note on the multiplying factors for various χ2 approximations. Journal

of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 16(2), 296-298.

Baumeister, R., Vohs, K., Aaker, J., & Garbinsky, E. (2013). Some key differences between a

happy life and a meaningful life. Journal of Positive Psychology, 8(6), 505-516.

doi:10.1080/17439760.2013.830764

Beavers, A. S., Lounsbury, J. W., Richards, J. K., Huck, S. W., Skolits, G. J., & Esquivel, S. L.

(2013). Practical considerations for using exploratory factor analysis in educational

research. Practical assessment, research & evaluation, 18(6), 1-13.

Berger, K. S., & Thompson, R. A. (1991). The developing person through childhood and

adolescence (3rd ed.). New York, N.Y: Worth Publishers.

Bhattacherjee, A. (2012). Social science research: principles, methods, and practices. Retrieved

from http://scholarcommons.usf.edu/oa_textbooks/3/

Boden, M., Thompson, R., Dizen, M., Berenbaum, H., & Baker, J. (2013). Are emotional clarity

and emotion differentiation related? Cognition & Emotion, 27(6), 961-978.

doi:10.1080/02699931.2012.751899

Brackett, M. A., & Salovey, P. (2006). Measuring emotional intelligence with the Mayer-

Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT). Psicothema, 18, 34-41.

90

Bryman, A. (2012). Social research methods. Oxford University Press. Oxford, UK.

Broderick, P. C., & Jennings, P. A. (2012). Mindfulness for adolescents: A promising approach

to supporting emotion regulation and preventing risky behavior. New Directions for

Youth Development, 2012(136), 111-126. doi:10.1002/yd.20042

Chapman, A. L., Dixon-Gordon, K. L., & Walters, K. N. (2011). Experiential avoidance and

emotion regulation in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Rational- Emotive &

Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 29(1), 35-52.

Cherniss, C., Extein, M., Goleman, D., & Weissberg, R. P. (2006). Emotional intelligence: What

does the research really indicate? Educational Psychologist, 41(4), 239-245.

doi:10.1207/s15326985ep4104_4

Cialdini, J. T., & Patrick, W. (2008). Loneliness. New York: Norton.

Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: Basic issues in objective scale

development. Psychological assessment, 7(3), 309.

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological bulletin, 112(1), 155.

Cohen, M. T. (2012). The importance of self-regulation for college student learning. College

Student Journal, 46(4), 892. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

Corder, G. W., & Foreman, D. I. (2014). Nonparametric statistics: a step-by-step approach. John

Wiley & Sons.

Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2006). Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience. The

Scientific World Journal, 6, 1146-1163.

Decety, J., & Michalska, K. J. (2010). Neurodevelopmental changes in the circuits underlying

empathy and sympathy from childhood to adulthood. Developmental science, 13(6), 886-

899.

Denham, S., Bassett, H., & Wyatt, T. (2010). Gender differences in the socialization of

preschoolers' emotional competence. (pp. 29-49). San Francisco: Wiley Periodicals.

doi:10.1002/cd.267

Denham, S. A., Zinsser, K. M., & Brown, C. A. (2012). The emotional basis of learning and

development in early childhood education. In B. Spodek & O. Saracho (Eds.), Handbook

of research on the education of young children (3nd Ed., pp. 67–88). New York, NY:

Lawrence Erlbaum.

Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life

scale. Journal of personality assessment, 49(1), 71-75.

91

Domitrovich, C. E., Cortes, R. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (2007). Improving young children’s

social and emotional competence: A randomized trial of the preschool “PATHS”

curriculum. The Journal of Primary Prevention, 28(2), 67-91.

Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annual Review of

Psychology, 51, 665-697.

Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., & Koller, S. (2001). Brazilian adolescents’ prosocial moral judgment

and behavior: Relations to sympathy, perspective taking, gender-role orientation, and

demographic characteristics. Child Development, 72(2), 518-534. doi: 10.1111/1467-

8624.00294

Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., & Shepard, S. A. (2005). Age

changes in prosocial responding and moral reasoning in adolescence and early adulthood.

Journal of research on adolescence, 15(3), 235-260.

Ekman, P. (2003). Emotions revealed: Recognizing faces and feelings to improve communication

and emotional life. New York, NY: Times Books.

Fine, M. A., & Fincham, F. D. (2013). Handbook of family theories: A content-based approach.

GB: Routledge Ltd. doi:10.4324/9780203075180

Flynn, M., & Rudolph, K. D. (2010). The contribution of deficits in emotional clarity to stress

responses and depression. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31(4), 291-

297. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2010.04.004

French, G., (2007). Children’s early learning and development. National council for curriculum

and assessment. Retrieved from http://www.ncca.ie

Friel, J., & Friel, L. (1998). Adult children: The secrets of dysfunctional families. Deerfield

Beach, Florida: Health Communications, Inc., 107-109, 117.

Garson, G. D. (2009). Reliability analysis: Statnotes. North Carolina State University [E-book

chapter]. Retrieved from http://faculty.chass.ncsu.edu/garson/PA765/statnote.htm

Gliem, R. R., & Gliem, J. A. (2003). Calculating, interpreting, and reporting Cronbach’s alpha

reliability coefficient for Likert-type scales. Midwest Research-to-Practice Conference in

Adult, Continuing, and Community Education.

Gohm, C. L., Corser, G. C., & Dalsky, D. J. (2005). Emotional intelligence under stress: Useful,

unnecessary, or irrelevant? Personality and Individual Differences, 39(1), 1017-1028.

Goodwin, L. D., & Leech, N. L. (2006). Understanding correlation: Factors that affect the size of

r. The Journal of Experimental Education, 74(3), 249-266.

92

Gratz, K. L., Bornovalova, M. A., Delany-Brumsey, A., Nick, B., & Lejuez, C. W. (2007). A

laboratory-based study of the relationship between childhood abuse and experiential

avoidance among inner-city substance users: The role of emotional nonacceptance

Behavior Therapy, 38(3), 256 - 268. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2006.08.006

Gratz, K. L. & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and

dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the Difficulties in

Emotion Regulation Scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26,

41-54.

Greenberg, L. S. (2002). Emotion-focused therapy: Coaching clients to work through their

feelings. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1997). Meta-emotion: How families communicate

emotionally. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Gu, X., Liu, X., Van Dam, N., Hof, P., & Fan, J. (2013). Cognition-emotion integration in the

anterior insular cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 23(1), 20-27. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr367

Guastello, D. D., & Guastello, S. J. (2003). Androgyny, gender role behavior, and emotional

intelligence among college students and their parents. Sex roles, 49(11-12), 663-673.

Helm, B. W. (2009). Emotions as evaluative feelings. Emotion Review, 1(3), 248-255.

doi:10.1177/1754073909103593

Hinkin, T. R., Tracey, J. B., & Enz, C. A. (1997). Scale construction: Developing reliable and

valid measurement instruments. Journal of Hospitality & Tourism Research, 21(1), 100-

120.

Harris, V. W. & Anderson, J. W. (2015). Validating constructs of social emotional ability

development. [Unpublished manuscript] University of Florida

Hof, M. (2012). Questionnaire evaluation with factor analysis and Cronbach’s Alpha: An

example. [Course material]. Retrieved fromhttp://www.rug.nl/?lang=en

Huitt, W. (2007). Maslow's hierarchy of needs. Educational Psychology Interactive. Valdosta,

GA: Valdosta State University. Retrieved from

http://www.edpsycinteractive.org/topics/regsys/maslow.html

Ivcevic, Z., & Brackett, M. (2014). Predicting school success: Comparing conscientiousness,

grit, and emotion regulation ability. Journal of Research in Personality, 52, 29-36.

doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2014.06.005

John-Steiner, V., & Mahn, H. (1996). Sociocultural approaches to learning and development: A

Vygotskian framework. Educational Psychologist, 31(3-4), 191-206.

doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3103&4_4

93

Kaiser, H. F. (1960). The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educational and

Psychology Measurement, 20, 141-151.

Kaiser, H. (1970). A second generation little jiffy. Psychometrika, 35(4), 401-415.

doi:10.1007/BF02291817

Kaiser, H. (1974). An index of factorial simplicity. Psychometrika, 39(1), 31-36.

doi:10.1007/BF02291575Kienbaum, J. (2014). The development of sympathy from 5 to 7

years: Increase, decline, or stability? A longitudinal study. Frontiers in Psychology, 5,

468. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00468

Kang, Y., & Harring, J. R. (2012). Investigating the impact of non-normality, effect size, and

sample size on two-group comparison procedures: An empirical study. Presented at the

annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association (AERA), SIG:

Educational Statisticians, Vancouver, BC.

Kihlstrom, J.F. & Cantor, N. (2011). Social intelligence. Retrieved from

http://socrates.berkeley.edu/~kihlstrm/social_intelligence.htm

Kraker, M. J. (2000). Classroom discourse: Teaching, learning, and learning disabilities.

Teaching and Teacher Education, 16(3), 295-313. doi:10.1016/S0742-051X(99)00063-3

Laible, D. J., & Carlo, G. (2004). The differential relations of maternal and paternal support and

control to adolescent social competence, self-worth, and sympathy. Journal of Adolescent

Research, 19(6), 759-782. doi:10.1177/0743558403260094

Lambert, N.M., Stillman, T.F., Baumeister, R.F., Fincham, F.R., Hicks, J.A., & Graham, S.M.

(2010). Family as a salient source of meaning in young adulthood. Journal of

PositivePsychology, 5, 367-376.

Lopes, P. N., Salovey, P., Côté, S., & Beers, M. (2005). Emotion regulation abilities and the

quality of social interaction (R. E. Petty, Ed.). Emotion, 5(1), 113-118.

doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.113

Lopes, P. N., Brackett, M. A., Nezlek, J. B., Schütz, A., Sellin, I., & Salovey, P. (2004).

Emotional intelligence and social interaction. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin,

30(8), 1018-1034. doi:10.1177/0146167204264762

Matsumoto, D. (2009). The origin of universal human emotions. San Francisco: San Francisco

State University.

Morse, J. M. (2015). Critical analysis of strategies for determining rigor in qualitative inquiry.

Qualitative health research, 25(9), 1212-1222.Maslow, A. (1954). Motivation and

personality. New York: Harper.

94

Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2012). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: The role of gender.

Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8(1), 161-187. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-

032511-143109

Neuendorf, K. A. (2016). Internal consistency reliability: Can Cronbach’s alpha be too high?

[Course material]. Retrieved from

http://academic.csuohio.edu/kneuendorf/c63111/hand5a.pdf

Ochsner, K. N., Silvers, J. A., & Buhle, J. T. (2012). Functional imaging studies of emotion

regulation: A synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion.

Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1251(1).

Osborne, J. W., & Costello, A. B. (2005). Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: Four

recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Practical assessment, research

& evaluation, 10(7), 1-9.

Pentimonti, J. M., & Justice, L. M. (2010). Teachers’ use of scaffolding strategies during read

alouds in the preschool classroom. Early Childhood Education Journal, 37(4), 241-248.

doi:10.1007/s10643-009-0348-6

Pett, M. A., Lackey, N. R., & Sullivan, J. J. (2003). Making sense of factor analysis: The use of

factor analysis for instrument development in health care research. Sage.

Prinz, J. (2011). Against empathy. The Southern Journal of Philosophy, 49, 214-233.

doi:10.1111/j.2041-6962.2011.00069.x

Shallcross, A. J., Troy, A. S., Boland, M., & Mauss, I. B. (2010). Let it be: Accepting negative

emotional experiences predicts decreased negative affect and depressive symptoms.

Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(9), 921-929. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.05.025

Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2003). Adolescents’ emotion regulation in daily life:

Links to depressive symptoms and problem behavior. Child Development, 74(6), 1869-

1880.

Singer, T., & Lamm, C. (2009). The social neuroscience of empathy. Annals of the New York

Academy of Sciences, 1156(1), 81-96.

Spackman, M. P., Fujiki, M., & Brinton, B. (2006). Understanding emotions in context: The

effects of language impairment on children's ability to infer emotional reactions.

International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41(2), 173-188.

doi:10.1080/13682820500224091

Suhr, D. D. (2005). Principal component analysis vs. exploratory factor analysis. SUGI 30

proceedings, 203, 230.

95

Swisher, M. E. (2016). Some useful techniques for operationalization. [Swisher Cheat Sheet].

Retrieved from http://fycs.ifas.ufl.edu/swisher/6802_16/Operationalization.pdf

UCLA (2016). What does Cronbach's alpha mean? Retrieved from

http://www.ats.ucla.edu/stat/spss/faq/alpha.html

Vossen, H. G., Piotrowski, J. T., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2015). Development of the adolescent

measure of empathy and sympathy (AMES). Personality and Individual Differences, 74,

66-71.

Wang, N., Wilhite, S. C., Wyatt, J., Young, T., Bloemker, G., & Wilhite, E. (2012). Impact of a

college freshman social and emotional learning curriculum on student learning outcomes:

An exploratory study. Journal of University Teaching and Learning Practice, 9(2)

Retrieved fromhttp://ro.uow.edu.au/jutlp/

Waters, H.S., (2013). Sociocultural theories of development. [Class lecture PowerPoint]. Stony

Brook University, N.Y. Retrieved from http://www.stonybrook.edu

Wied, M. D., Branje, S. J., & Meeus, W. H. (2007). Empathy and conflict resolution in

friendship relations among adolescents. Aggressive Behavior, 33(1), 48-55. doi:

10.1002/ab.20166

Williams, L. E., Bargh, J. A., Nocera, C. C., & Gray, J. R. (2009). The unconscious regulation of

emotion: nonconscious reappraisal goals modulate emotional reactivity. Emotion, 9(6),

847.

Williams, B., Brown, T., & Onsman, A. (2012). Exploratory factor analysis: A five-step guide

for novices. Australasian Journal of Paramedicine, 8(3), 1.

Williams, R. (2014). Missing data part 1: Overview, traditional methods. Retrieved from

http://www3.nd.edu/~rwilliam/

Wilson, W. M., Rosenberg, L. H., & Hyatt, L. E. (1997, May). Automated analysis of

requirement specifications. In Proceedings of the 19th international conference on

Software engineering (pp. 161-171). ACM.

Wilton, M. M., Craig, W. M., & Pepler, D. J. (2000). Emotional regulation and display in

classroom victims of bullying: Characteristic expressions of affect, coping styles and

relevant contextual factors. Social Development, 9(2), 227-245. doi: 10.1111/1467-

9507.00121

Wuensch, K.L. (2012). Karl Wuensch's statistics lessons. East Carolina University Department

of Psychology. Retrieved from http://core.ecu.edu

96

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH

Jonathan William Anderson attended public school in Dade County, Florida, and entered

the workforce in 1963 immediately following high school. Jonathan was drafted into the

military in 1966 and served with the U.S. Army in the Vietnam War during 1968. Following his

military service, Jonathan owned and operated several successful businesses.

After 45 years as a businessman, Jonathan decided to attend college, and began classes at

SantaFe College in Alachua County, Florida in the summer of 2010. After earning his Associate

of Arts degree in the spring of 2012, Jonathan began attending classes at the University of

Florida’s (UF) College of Agriculture and Life Science, Department of Family, Youth and

Community Science (FYCS). He earned a Bachelor of Science degree with a major in FYCS

and a minor in international studies. Jonathan graduated cum laude and was selected as one of

two outstanding scholars for the summer 2014 commencement.

As an undergraduate, Jonathan became keenly interested in the processes whereby

individuals develop social emotional abilities, and when he began attending graduate school in

the fall of 2014 he chose this as the subject area for his master’s thesis. In December of 2016,

Jon earned a Master of Science from UF in family, youth and community science. Throughout

his time in college, Jon maintained a 4.0 grade point average.